Hostname: page-component-5c6d5d7d68-7tdvq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-08-18T11:02:35.902Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Impact of the pandemic and its containment measures in Europe upon aspects of affective impairments: a Google Trends informetrics study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 June 2023

Istvan-Szilard Szilagyi
Affiliation:
Department of Anesthesiology and Intensive Care Medicine, Medical University of Graz, 8036 Graz, Austria Department of Medical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Medical University of Graz, 8036 Graz, Austria
Eva Eggeling
Affiliation:
Fraunhofer Austria Research GmbH, 8010 Graz, Austria
Helmar Bornemann-Cimenti
Affiliation:
Department of Anesthesiology and Intensive Care Medicine, Medical University of Graz, 8036 Graz, Austria
Torsten Ullrich*
Affiliation:
Fraunhofer Austria Research GmbH, 8010 Graz, Austria Institute of Computer Graphics and Knowledge Visualization, Graz University of Technology, 8010 Graz, Austria
*
Corresponding author: Torsten Ullrich; Email: torsten.ullrich@fraunhofer.at
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

In late 2019, a new virus began spreading in Wuhan, China. By the end of 2021, more than 260 million people worldwide had been infected and 5.2 million people had died because of the severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 (SARS-CoV-2). Various countermeasures have been implemented to contain the infections, depending on the country, infection prevalence, and political and infrastructural resources. The pandemic and the containment measures have induced diverse psychological burdens. Using internet queries as a proxy, this study examines the psychological consequences on a European level of SARS-CoV-2 containment measures.

Methods

Using informetric analyses, this study reviews within 32 European countries a total of 28 search parameters derived from the International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems (ICD-10) as aspects of affective disorder.

Results

Our results show that there are several psychological aspects which are significantly emphasized during the pandemic and its containment measures: ‘anxiety’, ‘dejection’, ‘weariness’, ‘listlessness’, ‘loss of appetite’, ‘loss of libido’, ‘panic attack’, and ‘worthlessness’. These terms are significantly more frequently part of a search query during the pandemic than before the outbreak. Furthermore, our results revealed that search parameters such as ‘psychologist’, ‘psychotherapist’, ‘psychotherapy’ have increased highly significantly (p < 0.01) since the pandemic.

Conclusions

The psychological distress caused by the pandemic correlates significantly with the frequency of people searching for psychological and psychotherapeutic support on the Internet.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

In late 2019, a new infectious disease began to spread in China in the city of Wuhan, resulting in fever, severe respiratory symptoms, fatigue, gastrointestinal and neurological problems, and other serious conditions, often with a lethal outcome (Fernández-de-las-Peñas et al., Reference Fernández-de-las-Peñas, Palacios-Ceña, Gómez-Mayordomo, Florencio, Cuadradob, Plaza-Manzano and Navarro-Santana2021). This single-stranded RNA virus was named ‘corona virus’ or SARS-CoV-2 (COVID-19) by the World Health Organization (WHO) because of its spiky form. As of March 2020, the WHO declared the outbreak of SARS-CoV-2 to be a pandemic (WHO, 2021b). The virus has an insidious characteristic because it spreads very rapidly, undergoes rapid mutations, is transmissible easily from person to person through droplet infection and physical contact, and can affect all people, regardless of age. By March 2020, the number of infections outside China was already increasing exponentially. By the end of November 2021, more than 260 million people worldwide had already been infected and 5.2 million people had died due to consequences associated with SARS-CoV-2 (Baloch, Baloch, Zheng, & Pei, Reference Baloch, Baloch, Zheng and Pei2020; WHO, 2021a; Worldometers.info, 2021). Higher risk for major complications, and even ultimately for death, due to SARS-CoV-2 infection is seen in elderly patients, in patients with comorbid diseases, and among patients who have been impaired by immunodeficiency.

Various countermeasures have been introduced to contain COVID-19 infections, depending on country, infection prevalence, and political and infrastructural resources. The set of measures includes home-based work, home schooling, closure of stores and public places, travel restrictions, and even a complete lockdown of social life. These interventions resulted in a reduction of infection rates in countries where measures were taken compared to those where not. The effectiveness of the lockdown measures, in particular, continue to show a beneficial trend toward decreasing infection rates at least 20 days after the end of the measurement (Alfano & Ercolano, Reference Alfano and Ercolano2020). Although these measures were successful, their consequences in economic, environmental, and psychological terms quickly became apparent. In particular, the psychological consequences proved to be profound. Thus, the pandemic as well as the applied measures had unfavorable psychological consequences, especially in the psycho-affective sphere. Increases in generalized anxiety, isolated phobias, depression, stress, anger, sleep disturbances, and harmful, addictive, and destructive behaviors emerged (Ahmed et al., Reference Ahmed, Ahmed, Aibao, Hanbin, Siyu and Ahmad2020; Atalan, Reference Atalan2020; Bäuerle et al., Reference Bäuerle, Teufel, Musche, Weismüller, Kohler, Hetkamp and Skoda2020; Dozois, Reference Dozois2020). Several additional consequences were also observed following the lockdown interventions, such as fear of contagion, experiences of frustration, social disconnection as well as stigmatization (Brooks et al., Reference Brooks, Webster, Smith, Woodland, Wessly and Greenberg2020; Planchuelo-Gómez, Odriozola-González, Irurtia, & de Luis-García, Reference Planchuelo-Gómez, Odriozola-González, Irurtia and de Luis-García2020).

Notably, the psychological impact appears to be very pervasive and severe. There is a clinically prominent pattern of depression and anxiety in SARS-CoV-2 hospitalized patients, which is related to the severity of the consequences of the infection as well as the length of the hospitalization (Alshareef, Al Zahrani, Alzhrani, Suwaidi, & Alamry, Reference Alshareef, Al Zahrani, Alzhrani, Suwaidi and Alamry2020; Cao et al., Reference Cao, Fang, Hou, Han, Xu, Dong and Zheng2020). Apart from infected individuals, general population may also experience several negative psychological consequences associated with the pandemic and its countermeasures, such as depression, anxiety, panic attacks, reduced sleep quality, increased stress response, psychomotor agitation, psychosis, and suicidal tendencies, as well as an increase in adverse coping responses to stressful situations, such as alcohol and drug use. These symptoms occur more frequently in regions where SARS-CoV-2 infections had a higher incidence and were described overall with a worsening of psychological well-being (Ahmed et al., Reference Ahmed, Ahmed, Aibao, Hanbin, Siyu and Ahmad2020; Bäuerle et al., Reference Bäuerle, Teufel, Musche, Weismüller, Kohler, Hetkamp and Skoda2020; Choi, Hui, & Wan, Reference Choi, Hui and Wan2020).

Using internet search terms as a proxy, this study examines the psychological consequences on a European level of SARS-CoV-2 containment measures. The study is based on the assumption that individuals are intrinsically motivated, both physiologically and psychologically, to take individual and social actions to restore their health and/or quality of life. Furthermore, we assume that, when experiencing discomfort of a physiological or psychological nature, they utilize symptomatic search parameters in commonly used Internet search engines to get support. As previous studies, including the work by Ginsberg et al. (Reference Ginsberg, Mohebbi, Patel, Brammer, Smolinski and Brilliant2009) have shown, this assumption is neither unreasonable nor unrealistic.

The following study is based on scientometric research. The goal of scientometric research is to evaluate scientific questions by analyzing and presenting the structure and regularities of quantitative changes in information elements using scientific methodology (Qiu, Zhao, Yang, & Dong, Reference Qiu, Zhao, Yang and Dong2017). Therefore, our study leans on the analysis of dynamics of Internet search parameters of the most common Internet search engine of possible affective problems linked to the different containment measures (Deci & Ryan, Reference Deci and Ryan1985; Tang & Ng, Reference Tang and Ng2006). Consequently, we explore which search parameters associated with affective impairment are most salient across different containment measures.

Methods

We designed and performed this infodemiological study according to the STROBE criteria (STrengthening the Reporting of OBservational studies in Epidemiology; Elm et al., Reference Elm, Altman, Egger, Pocock, Gøtzsche and Vandenbroucke2007), at the Medical University of Graz in close collaboration with Fraunhofer Austria Research GmbH and the Graz University of Technology. Following the International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems, search parameters belonging to affective disorders were explored. These search parameters have been translated into the official languages of the examined countries.

In the period from January 2017 to June 2021, the frequencies of the translated search parameters keywords were acquired using Google Trends analysis and have been have cleansed as described in detail below. These data were temporally and geographically related to the number of cases and countermeasures taken as reported by each country to the European Centre for Disease Prevention and Control (ECDC, 2021). Figure 1 presents the workflow of the data acquisition and analysis of this study. Based on this data and their correlations two main issues of interest are the focus of this study:

  • The first research question is to determine which psychological search parameters associated with affective disorders according to the ICD-10 classification are significantly more frequent among the SARS-CoV-2 specific containment measures.

  • In the second research question we explore what psychological search parameters concerning psychological support providers occur significantly more often during SARS-Covid2 containment measures.

Figure 1. Data flow overview of the process of data acquisition, preparation, and consolidation.

Data acquisition and processing

The data set is composed of data provided by the European Centre for Disease Prevention and Control and by Alphabet Inc. (Mountain View, California). The European Centre for Disease Prevention and Control provides the daily number and the 14-day notification rate of new reported COVID-19 cases and deaths by EU/EEA countries. These data provide the basis for describing the course of the pandemic. Furthermore, the European Centre for Disease Prevention and Control provides data on non-pharmaceutical country response measures to SARS-CoV-2 (ECDC, European Centre for Disease Prevention & Control, 2021). The second data source is provided by the holding company Alphabet Inc. offering the Web service Google Trends that analyses the popularity of top search queries in Google Search across various regions and languages via https://trends.google.com/ (Yossi, Reference Yossi2012).

The data set of this study comprehends 235 weekly records from the period January 2017 to June 2021 of 32 European countries with 19 search term categories. The weekly records have been queried covering the following periods:

  • January 2017 to June 2021, as well as the nine-months-periods

  • January 2017 to September 2017,

  • October 2017 to June 2018,

  • July 2018 to March 2019,

  • April 2019 to December 2019,

  • January 2020 to September 2020,

  • October 2020 to June 2021.

The goal of splitting the overall period of January 2017 to June 2021 into short intervals of nine months each is to achieve a higher accuracy by avoiding unadjusted normalization and discretization in periods with only few Google queries. Furthermore, to reduce sampling artifacts caused by the daily sampling of the data set of all Google queries, the Google Trends analyses were repeated multiple times on different days (as of December 2021, nine repetitions starting in July 2021).

Each Google Trends analysis has been conducted in all the following 32 countries in all official languages of the corresponding country:

Austria, Belgium, Bulgaria, Croatia, Cyprus, Czechia, Denmark, Estonia, Finland, France, Germany, Greece, Hungary, Iceland, Ireland, Italy, Latvia, Lichtenstein, Lithuania, Luxembourg, Malta, Netherlands, Norway, Poland, Portugal, Romania, Slovakia, Slovenia, Spain, Sweden, Switzerland, United Kingdom

i.e. in total, the analyses were performed in 42 individual country-language combinations. Each analysis had a specific, psychological search term in focus, which was manually translated into the respective language. In cases where the translation was ambiguous due to synonyms, declension, etc. all translation variants were used.

The search terms derived from ICD-10 categorizing affective disorders, include:

anxious, anxiety, dejection, depressed, depression, exhausted, exhaustion, guilt, insomnia, listlessness, loss of appetite, loss of libido, depressed/sad/ bad mood, panic attack, sad, sadness, sleepless, sleep disorder/problem, tired, weariness, worthless, worthlessness, as well as psychiatry, psychology, psychologist, psychotherapist, psychotherapy, psychological treatment.

In total, depending on the number of synonyms and declensions, up to 38 different search queries are evaluated for each of the 42 individual country-language combinations. Online Supplementary Appendix B comprehends all query terms; both the English ones and their translations. According to Alphabet Inc., Google Trends provides access to queries. This database has been accessed automatically via the official Web-based interface using the Python library ‘pytrends’. The complete source code of the Python scripts to access Google Trends data is listed in online Supplementary Appendix C. The returned search statistics are normalized to the time and location of a query scaled on a range of 0 to 100 based on a topic's proportion to all searches on all topics and rounded to an integer. Additionally, to the introduced rounding error, the dataset returned by Google Trends has a sampling error (Steegmans, Reference Steegmans2021). The sampling error occurs because Google uses only a sample of all searches and information on the sample size is not disclosed; and Google Trends uses a different sample on every single day. In particular, the sample size is not adjusted to the distribution of search queries, i.e. the number of samples is not extended for infrequent search queries. In order to overcome these sampling artifacts and the discretization during the internal normalization by Google Trends the downloaded data has to be consolidated to a consistent data set. This process is performed independently for each country and for each search term group.

The first data processing step merges the time series covering the complete analysis period (January 2017 – June 2021) for a specific country and a specific search term group. According to Alphabet Inc. each time series is based on a sampling of all search queries, where the sampling size as well as the size of the underlying total data set is not mentioned, filtered by country and time frame. Then for each week within the time frame the relative proportion of analyzed search terms in the filtered sample is determined, normalized/scaled to 100 and rounded to integer values. Since the sampling of the underlying total data set is done on a daily basis, Google Trends queries will produce different results on different days. The first step merges these different results and determines a weekly average. Afterwards the averaged time series is scaled to 100.0 (but not rounded). In addition to the time series covering the complete analysis period (January 2017 – June 2021) supplementary time series, each covering nine months, increase the overall accuracy. For this purpose, all-time series of a 9-month interval are combined to a short-term, mean time series. A scaling factor is then determined for each 9-month interval so that the maximum of the averaged 9-month interval is mapped to the maximum of the global, averaged time series from the corresponding time interval. In the next processing step the scaled, short-term analysis data is merged. Two reasons for merging the data are distinguished: multilingualism and linguistic variety. Data from countries with more than one official language are queried in all official languages and are merged with weighing factors representing the proportion of corresponding native speakers in the population of the country. This weighted summation is performed in the following countries:

Belgium, Cyprus, Finland, Ireland, Luxembourg, Malta, and Switzerland.

Belgium has three official languages: Dutch, French and German. As no census exists, there are no official statistical data regarding the distribution or usage of Belgium's three official languages; the estimated distributions of primary languages in Belgium (and the used weights) are 59% Dutch, 40% French, and 1% German. Cyprus has two official languages, Greek and Turkish. Based on the 2011 census the distribution (Government of Cyprus, Statistical Service Cyprus, 2022) of native speakers is 81% (Greek) to 1% (Turkish); the completing percentages do not speak any official language natively and the resulting weights are 0.988 and 0.012. The two main official languages of Finland are Finnish and Swedish. According to official statistics Finnish is spoken by 87% (weighting factor 0.946) of the population, and Swedish is spoken by 5% (weighting factor 0.054) of the population (Statistics Finland, 2021). In the Republic of Ireland, the languages Irish and English have official status; according to the latest Europe-wide survey of languages in Europe by the European Commission 93% of the population speak English and 3% of the population speak Irish as mother language (Van Parys, Reference Van Parys2012); the corresponding weights are 0.969 and 0.031. The three official languages of Luxembourg are French, German and Luxembourgish. In the private sector the main language is French (56%; 0.683), followed by Luxembourgish (20%; 0.244), and German (6%; 0.073) (Pigeron-Piroth & Fehlen, Reference Pigeron-Piroth and Fehlen2015). Malta has two official languages: Maltese and English. A study collecting public opinion on what language was ‘preferred’ discovered that 86% (weighting factor 0.878) of the population express a preference for Maltese and 12% (weighting factor 0.122) for English (Capdevila, Reference Capdevila2004). Switzerland has four national languages: mainly German (spoken by 62.8% of the population in 2016), French (22.9%), Italian (8.2%), and Romansh (0.5%) (Confederatione Svizerra, Federal Statistical Office, 2021). Linguistic varieties due to synonyms within one language are averaged without weighting. Last but not least, a comprehensive dataset was created combining all countries weighted by population size as currently listed by the ECDC (as of July 2021).

The results of the data acquisition are a time series for each pair of country and query term category. Figure 2 shows as examples the time series for search queries for the term ‘anxiety/anxious’ from the countries Germany (top) and Portugal (bottom). These diagrams illustrate two patterns that can be observed in several data sets: (1) the time series plot of the term ‘anxiety/anxious’ in Germany shows a peak in temporal proximity to a pandemic wave; (2) the graph of the Portuguese data set shows a gradually increasing trend with the beginning of the pandemic. This trend is particularly well visible in the two-month moving average window.

Figure 2. The data acquisition provides a time series for each country and each search term. The upper visualization shows the data on “anxiety/anxious” searches in Germany; the lower one accordingly for Portugal. Each graph shows in red the relative number of searches scaled to 100%, with the uncertainty due to sampling and rounding plotted as the estimated standard deviation for each time point as an additional thin red line; the black line shows the moving average of a two-month time window. The temporal relation to the pandemic is based on the number of nationally reported infection cases, which is also scaled and shown as a dark gray plot. The counter measures in force at the national level at that time are plotted below the time series.

Data analysis

The main analysis step is based on Mann–Whitney-U-tests carried out using IBM-SPSS 22 (Armonk, New York). With this test, we compare for each country the frequency of a query parameter with respect to the time in which a countermeasure was taken and implemented. For each triplet of country, term, and countermeasure, the test either returns invalid (because the measure was not implemented in the corresponding country, or Google Trends returns an empty time series), or a p-value. If the test returns p-values less than or equal to 0.05, the result is considered significant, and a value less than 0.01 is considered very significant, i.e. in these cases the distributions in the two groups – before and during the countermeasure – differed (very) significantly.

All results for all triplets of country, term, and countermeasure are listed in online Supplementary Appendix A. Based on the identification of the significant changes, the dependency on the individual countries is eliminated in the second step: This is done by determining the percentage of countries in which a countermeasure led to a significant or a very significant change. This second step considers all countries equally – regardless of their population size. As the European countries differ in population size significantly (see Table 1), it is sensible to take this aspect into account. To give more weight to population size, the second step was repeated in a third step with a weighting by population size, i.e. for a countermeasure and for a search term, the weighted percentage is determined rather than the simple percentage of affected countries with (very) significant changes. The used weights correspond to the population size as currently listed by the ECDC (see Table 1).

Table 1. Each European country implemented countermeasures independently (Yan et al., Reference Yan, Zhu, Gu, Huang, Sun, Zhang and Chen2021)

This table lists the population size for each country, and the accumulated product of each countermeasure with the period of its duration (measured in days). All data are provided by the European Centre for Disease Prevention and Control (ECDC, 2021)

Finally, in a last step the dependency on the search term is eliminated by determining the percentage of countries in which a countermeasure led to a significant or a very significant change in any search term. Analog to the second step, this step is performed a second time using the population weight.

Results

Analyzed by country, the following significant differences of psychological aspects related to the containment measures were found: the countermeasure ‘teleworking recommendations’ is associated with significant or very significant changes for the search term ‘anxiety’ in 17 countries including Austria, Belgium, Croatia, France, Germany, Greece, Ireland, Italy, Luxembourg, Malta, Norway, Poland, Portugal, Romania, Slovenia, Spain, and Switzerland. No significant changes have been found in 10 countries: Bulgaria, Czechia, Denmark, Finland, Latvia, Lithuania, Netherlands, Slovakia, Sweden, and United Kingdom. For the pair ‘teleworking recommendations’ and ‘anxiety’ has a significant change in 17 of 27, or 63% of the countries. Weighted by population size, this value even rises to 73.9%. If this calculation is performed with ‘teleworking recommendations’ not only for all countries, but also for all search terms examined, significant changes are identified in 37.5% of all countries and all search terms. Table 2 lists all results of these calculations and explicitly names the first and second dominating effect.

Table 2. Each countermeasure is tested statistically on its influence on Internet search terms of psychological disorders and symptoms

The table is ordered by the percentage of countries with a (highly) significant correlation. The search term with the highest correlation is listed as dominating effect.

To give more weight to population size, this calculation has been repeated with a weighting by population size. Weighted by population, among examined countries leads to different results due to the large variation in European population sizes as listed in Table 1. In the weighted calculation, the following significant differences of psychological aspects related to the containment measures have been identified: the ‘closure of workplace’ has a significant / very significant effect on 57% of the population, if in place. The measure is correlated with ‘listlessness’ and ‘panic attacks’; the ‘closure of entertainment venues’ is a countermeasure which is correlated with ‘worthlessness’ and ‘anxiety’ for 50.4% of the population of all examined countries. A similar influence can be measured for the ‘closure of gyms sports centres’. In times, in which this measure is in place, 50% of the population shows a significant change in the frequencies of the search terms ‘anxiety’ and ‘panic attacks’. The complete list of measures, the proportion of the population showing significant changes in the frequencies of the analyzed query terms, and the terms with the highest change rate are listed in Table 3.

Table 3. Analogous to Table 2, each countermeasure is tested statistically on its influence on Internet search terms of psychological disorders and symptoms

In this table the results are weighted with the population size of each correlated country.

Finally, all data sets have been combined into two European datasets: a dataset that considers all countries equally, regardless of population size, and a weighted dataset by population size. Both data sets reveal significant increases in the frequencies of most search terms related to psychological aspects in the time ranging three years before and since the official pandemic declaration by the WHO. In detail, the Internet search terms ‘anxiety’, ‘dejection’, ‘weariness’, ‘listlessness’, ‘loss of appetite’, ‘loss of libido’, ‘panic attack’, and ‘worthlessness’ have been entered in search queries more often in Europe during the pandemic than before. Furthermore, the non-weighted dataset shows an increase in ‘insomnia’ and the population-weighted dataset shows an increase in ‘exhaustion’ and in ‘guilt’; however, these effects only show up in one data set – not in both.

In contrast, a significant decrease in the search frequency could be found for the search term ‘depression’ in the non-weighted dataset; weighted by population there is no significant increase or decrease for the search term ‘depression’ (see Table 4).

Table 4. The overall impact of the pandemic on individual search parameters before and during the pandemic in Europe

a Changes of the psychological-related search parameter increased significantly after the first confirmed COVID-19 case was reported.

b Changes of the psychological-related search parameter decreased significantly after the first confirmed COVID-19 case was reported.

c No significant changes.

Considering the frequency of search criteria related to treatment or treatment provider, both datasets show that the search frequency has increased significantly since the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic for the following search terms: ‘psychologist’, ‘psychotherapist’, ‘psychotherapy’. A significant decrease occurred only for the search term ‘psychology’. As with the search query on ‘depression’, a significant increase for ‘psychology’ is only found in the unweighted dataset; the weighted dataset shows no significant increase or decrease for ‘depression’ and ‘psychology’. All results of the complete European data set are listed in Table 4 and plotted in Fig. 3.

Figure 3. The overall distribution of frequencies in the population-weighted data set of all countries (in a range between 0 and 100) of most search terms related to psychological aspects in the time ranging three years before and since the official pandemic declaration by the WHO.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first study that informetrically examines psychological consequences of the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic of respective containment measures within 32 countries.

The curated dataset used has several characteristic differences compared to other existing datasets: The dataset is focused on psychological aspects and does not address physical symptoms in comparison to Szilagyi et al. (Reference Szilagyi, Ullrich, Lang-Illievich, Klivinyi, Schittek, Simonis and Bornemann-Cimenti2021). Moreover, the curated dataset has been designed to be very comprehensive in time, i.e. with a pre-pandemic period of almost three years, it is possible to extract a pre-pandemic model based on data and compare it with the pandemic data. In this way, temporal differentials can be identified (compared to ‘Google COVID-19 Search Trends symptoms dataset’, Google LLC, 2020). Furthermore, the dataset includes a regionally differentiated picture that is not otherwise available for Europe in this precision (data.world, 2022).

The primary goal this study is to examine the psychological impact of each containment measure, through analysis of psychological Internet query terms temporarily related to the corresponding countermeasures. As a second objective, this study determines the trends in search parameters related to psychological and psychiatric treatment and treatment providers.

Our results show that the search criteria for all psychological aspects that constitute different symptoms of affective-psychological disorders according to ICD-10 have changed considerably since the pandemic; see Table 4 and Fig. 3 for details. Exploring the data by population, the 32 countries examined reveal that with the introduction of containment measures, in particular the following psychology related search terms have changed significantly:

‘anxiety’, ‘dejection’, ‘listlessness’, ‘loss of appetite’, ‘loss of libido’, ‘panic attack’, ‘weariness’, and ‘worthlessness’

Anxiety

The very prominent psychological aspect ‘anxiety’ involves a feeling that emerges in response to an unspecified threat. It manifests itself not only in a sensation of threat of some kind, but it is also commonly accompanied by a large cascade of physiological reactions, such as changes in muscle tension, heart rate variability, or blood pressure, among others. It induces behavior of avoidance and reinforces unfavorable coping patterns. Experienced frequently, anxiety can adversely affect quality of life and, in further consequence, cause psychological as well as physiological illnesses. According to our results, the feeling of anxiety, is highly prominent in the pandemic period and ranks highest in most of the containment measures, such as: ‘closure of educational institutions: primary schools’, ‘closure of entertainment venues’, ‘closure of gyms sports centres’, ‘interventions are in place to limit all indoor-outdoor mass public gatherings’, ‘stay at home orders for the general population (lock-down)’, ‘stay at home recommendation’ and ‘teleworking recommendations’ (see Tables 2 and 3).

Three main mechanisms can be attributed for the high level of anxiety associated with the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic outbreak and its containment measures: the evaluation of the current situation and resulting responses, the concern over infection and its health consequences, and the impact of the stream of concerning information provided by the media.

Two basic models, cognitive emotion theory and evaluation theory, originate from emotion psychology can be referred to in the explanation of the affective consequences of the pandemic and the containment measures. Both theories assume that an event is evaluated according to its actuality, value, as well as its threatening nature. As a result, such an evaluation triggers emotions, impulses to take certain actions, and physiological reactions (Arnold, Reference Arnold and Reymert1950; Lazarus, Reference Lazarus1991). Transferred to the pandemic situation, this model would result in greater anxiety, since the virus infection and its health consequences might be evaluated by individuals as very threatening. The containment measures, such as the closure of schools, various workplaces, or areas of social activities, leads to a suppression of impulses for actions, such as visiting loved ones, pursuing social activities, working, or going to school, all of which can be perceived as a threat and an interference. Consequently, higher anxiety occurs at the level of perception. Anxiety during the pandemic can be attributed also to specific needs in interpersonal relationships and the evaluation and impact of specific situations. In addition, the social psychological model Construal Level Theory (CL theory) offers another possible explanation for the interpersonal experiences related to the pandemic and lockdown events. The CL theory provides an explanation of how we mentally evaluate individuals, objects, events, and ideas that we encounter, a process that is directly dependent by our psychological distance from the respective entities. However, psychological distance varies from person to person and is determined by geographic circumstances, time-related determinants, social acquaintances, and the probability of events occurring (Bowen, Reference Bowen2021; Stillman, Fujita, Sheldon, & Trope, Reference Stillman, Fujita, Sheldon and Trope2018; Zheng et al., Reference Zheng, Miao, Lim, Li, Nie and Zhang2020). In accordance with CL theory, a prominent influence in the pandemic is shown by geographical distances, among others to loved ones, friends to beloved activities, such as sports or other types of hobbies, but also to regular structured activities, such as work or school visits. The temporal aspect of assessing the pandemic situation is, due to the high number of infections, recurrent containment measures, and constantly new virus mutations, a great unknown that presumably may trigger insecurity. The strong restriction of social interactions as well as the estimation of a possible high occurrence of infection as well as associated consequences cover all levels of a psychological distance, whose evaluation and individual processing might cause affective reactions such as anxiety (Bowen, Reference Bowen2021; Zheng et al., Reference Zheng, Miao, Lim, Li, Nie and Zhang2020).

Another recent fear construct linked to the pandemic is coronaphobia, which can cause both functional and psychological problems. It can be defined by the worry of infecting oneself or family members with the virus SARS-CoV-2 (Vizard, Davis, White, & Beynon, Reference Vizard, Davis, White and Beynon2020). However, it cannot be ruled out that coronaphobia may also cause health anxiety due to a high degree of misinterpretation of bodily sensations, so that harmless physiological sensations or changes were interpreted as signs of infection. In addition, severe health anxiety ensured that many people practiced extensive safety behaviors in the aftermath of the pandemic, such as excessive hand hygiene, frequently laundering, and over-disinfection of hands or clothing. The misinterpretation of physical sensations during the pandemic may have led to an increased anxiety regarding the possibility of suffering from a covid infection (Asmundson & Taylor, Reference Asmundson and Taylor2020).

Lastly, the dissemination of information may have contributed to the increase or development of anxiety about the pandemic and its containment efforts. The rapid availability of information, the constantly increasing number of infections, the details about the virus’ fast spread, the many fatalities, the initially unclear treatment methods, and the widely strict – and also diverse and common – containment measures resulted in a large amount of predominantly negative reports about the virus from almost all media sources, which could ultimately have an unfavorable impact on health anxiety (King et al., Reference King, McQuaid, Leeson, Samuel, Grant, Azeem and Crawford2022; Landi, Pakenham, Boccolini, Grandi, & Tossani, Reference Landi, Pakenham, Boccolini, Grandi and Tossani2020) and furthermore on the frequency of Google queries for parameters such as ‘anxiety’.

Panic attack

Based on the data acquired in our study, panic attacks are also among the psychological aspects that are listed particularly frequently and are associated with containment measures. They are characterized among others with symptoms such as: chest pain, shortness of breath, generalized anxiety, coronaphobia, tremor, dizziness, nausea, vomiting (Bhatia, Goyal, Singh, & Daral, Reference Bhatia, Goyal, Singh and Daral2020; Javelot & Weiner, Reference Javelot and Weiner2020). In the following containment measures, the search parameter ‘panic attack’ features prominently: ‘closure of non-essential stores’, ‘interventions are in place to limit all indoor-outdoor mass public gatherings’, ‘closure of gyms sports centres’ and ‘closure of workplaces’. Various explanatory models for panic disorder can be derived: among others, panic attacks can also be explained by fear of fear, as an interoceptive conditioning, whereby a constant attentiveness to bodily functions is derived.

In the pandemic situation, it can also be assumed that the evaluation of actual situation and the associated containment measures may play a substantial part in the development of panic reactions. Assessing the current situation as extremely threatening and abstract due to the mobility restrictions, the obligatory stay at home and the impulses of not being able to satisfy the own needs can subsequently lead to panic states. It has been shown that mental health status or mental coping during the pandemic depends significantly not only on numerous factors such as sociodemographic characteristics like social class, ethnicity, and gender, but also on mental coping measures. People who are unable to adequately manage psychological stress are not only more vulnerable to affective disorders such as anxiety or panic attacks but are also at higher risk of developing physically linked diseases as well. Therefore, containment measures such as closing stores, gyms, sports centers, workplaces and restricting mass public gatherings, while protective against infectious diseases, may result in highly negative psychological consequences (Ellwardt & Präg, Reference Ellwardt and Präg2021). Conspiracy theories in the media, rumors of increased containment measures, and rapidly circulating images of sick or dying people on social media fuel consequences such as panic attacks and anxiety (Saadat et al., Reference Saadat, Izadi, Jonaidi Jafari, Abolghasemi, Jamalimoghadamsiyahkali, Jamalian and Javanbakht2020; The Lancet, 2020; Yoon, Feyissa, & Suk, Reference Yoon, Feyissa and Suk2021). While for some people the consequences of negative information trigger a brief emotional response latency, a weak information filter allows for a fear response to panic reactions that are sustained by new negative information (Bushuyev, Bushuieva, Onyshchenko, & Bondar, Reference Bushuyev, Bushuieva, Onyshchenko and Bondar2021; Sharma et al., Reference Sharma, Ghura, Mahendru, Erkut, Kaur and Bedi2020).

Depression and worthlessness

Considering the general trends in search parameters only for the period before and after the outbreak of the pandemic, our results show significant changes in the frequency of the search term ‘depression’ in the unweighted dataset and no significant changes in the weighted dataset. A depression refers to a mental illness characterized by dejection, loss of interest, exhaustion, and listlessness. It is accompanied by other secondary symptoms, such as feelings of guilt, lack of concentration, self-doubt, or insomnia. Severe courses of this disease can lead to suicide. The causes are genetic-physiological factors, as well as psychosocial strain, stress load or a combination of these.

Contrary to expectations, our study shows a significant decrease in the frequency of the search term ‘depression’ since the pandemic outbreak in at least one dataset. In studies assessing associations between a pandemic and depression, an increase in depression rates was found following a pandemic outbreak. Thus, in a 2021 study, it was found that after the spread of COVID-19 in Wuhan, one in five (19.2%) suffered from moderate to severe depression (Zhong, Huang, & Liu, Reference Zhong, Huang and Liu2021). A representative survey in the United Kingdom found that the pandemic significantly increased the number of people with a depressive disorder, identifying the following risk factors as favoring the depressive disorder development during a pandemic: female gender, younger age, socioeconomic problems and disabled individuals (Vizard et al., Reference Vizard, Davis, White and Beynon2020). A meta-analysis shows a world-wide 7-fold increase in the prevalence of depression since the pandemic outbreak (Bueno-Notivol et al., Reference Bueno-Notivol, Gracia-García, Olaya, Lasheras, López-Antón and Santabárbara2021). Of 15 000 people examined who had been in quarantine for 10 days, 29% showed post-traumatic stress disorder and 31% experienced symptoms of depression (Huremović, Reference Huremović2019).

It is not explainable with the previous theoretical concepts, however, why our result does not show the expected trend. A possible explanation may be found in the variability of languages; as symptoms increase or decrease, other languages may use different search terms whose nuances are lost in translation. Online Supplementary Appendix B lists up to 4 different translations each for the noun stems depression (depressive, etc.) and sadness (sad, etc.). Furthermore, it cannot be ruled out whether depressed individuals show different online search behavior due to a pandemic than pre-pandemic, whether depression prevented them from conducting ‘online searches’, nor whether multiple factors may have influenced this outcome. Additional studies are needed to investigate this particular trend.

Our study further shows that the search term ‘depression’ appeared as a search parameter in the following containment measures: ‘interventions in place to limit all indoor-outdoor mass public gatherings’, ‘closure of entertainment venues’, ‘closure of educational institutions: higher education’, ‘closure of educational institutions: secondary schools’, ‘closure of educational institutions: primary schools’, ‘stay at home orders for the general population (lock-down)’, ‘closure of places of worship’, ‘closure of public transport’.

A component of depression, the feeling of worthlessness, emerged at second position, being the most frequently associated search term among the different measurement procedures. Depressed people often experience feelings of worthlessness and guilt. Individuals are quick to assign unpleasant incidents to themselves and blame themselves, even if these feelings are of an irrational nature. The feeling of worthlessness as a search parameter emerged in the following containment measures: ‘closure of entertainment venues’, ‘teleworking recommendations’, ‘closure of institutions: daycare’, ‘stay at home orders for the general population (lock-down)’, ‘closure of restaurants and cafes’.

The strong prominence of the psychological terms ‘depression’ and ‘worthlessness’ that accompanied the above-mentioned containment measures during the pandemic can be attributed to several possible explanations. One of these involves the fact that the pandemic appears to have altered the entire social structure worldwide, from employment status to educational situation to outdoor activities (Foley & Cooper, Reference Foley and Cooper2021). Containment is a very efficient way of interrupting chains of infection, as they separate infected persons from non-infected ones. Not being able to deal with everyday matters, experiencing dramatic events such as the loss of a loved one or an acquaintance, or constant negative news about deaths or numbers of infected person, can lead to increased anxiety in isolation as well as in a pandemic, and can further trigger serious illnesses such as post-traumatic stress disorder or depression. Studies show that younger people were severely affected psychologically by the pandemic and the lockdown measures. As a result, children and adolescents lost the opportunity to participate in school or extracurricular activities, which gave them a sense of structure or daily rhythm (Courtney, Watson, Battaglia, Mulsant, & Szatmari, Reference Courtney, Watson, Battaglia, Mulsant and Szatmari2020; Davy et al., Reference Davy, Scheuermaier, Roden, Christie, Bentley, Gomez-Olive and Rae2021; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Pan, Wan, Tan, Xu, Ho and Ho2020). Furthermore, because of the pandemic and following the lockdown measures, people faced restrictions in their daily activities, which were likely associated with insecurity and a sense of threat. Also, during the pandemic, many people had experiences that threatened their livelihoods, such as losing their jobs, which had economic, social, and ultimately life consequences (Kaushik & Guleria, Reference Kaushik and Guleria2020).

In the context of depressive consequences of the pandemic, social networks and media may also play another major role. The information contained in these areas can be a useful tool for gaining more knowledge about pathogens and treatment methods, while on the other hand, they provide a breeding ground for health impairment (Zhong et al., Reference Zhong, Huang and Liu2021).

Looking for psychological support options

Considering searches of support for a possible psychological burden, illness, or disorder, analyses were also conducted in our study. We analyzed to what extent the corresponding search terms have changed significantly since the pandemic. Among them, our results revealed that the search parameters ‘psychologist’, ‘psychotherapist’, ‘psychotherapy’ have increased highly significantly (p < 0.01) since the COVID-19 pandemic.

As a result of the psychological distress caused by the pandemic, we assume that people are significantly more likely to search for psychological and psychotherapeutic support on the Internet.

The more general search term ‘psychology’ became significantly less frequently searched since the pandemic (p < 0.05) in the unweighted dataset. However, there is a possible interpretation that this search query refers to more general terminology such as psychology studies, psychology books or other general interests in the field of ‘psychology’ that have received less interest since the pandemic outbreak and its containment measures.

Limitations

The individual data sources have some limitations. The data on non-pharmaceutical interventions and response measures have been collected by the European Centre for Disease Prevention and Control. The European Centre for Disease Prevention and Control uses multiple information sources per country such as Ministries of Health or National Public Health Institutes, and the obtained data is systematically cross checked with data from the World Health Organization. The limitations of this data source are described in detail by the ECDC (2021).

The data provided by Google Trends have a sampling error (Steegmans, Reference Steegmans2021) which has been compensated by the data acquisition strategy implemented in this study. However, there is certainly an interpretive limitation: searching for a term on the Internet does not and should not equate to a diagnosis of a given condition; nevertheless, previous studies showed connections and correlations between the users’ Internet queries and their well-being (Ginsberg et al., Reference Ginsberg, Mohebbi, Patel, Brammer, Smolinski and Brilliant2009).

Nevertheless, it must be clearly emphasized that (a) the reach of search engines is limited in market coverage and therefore they are probably not representative and (b) intentions are not stored for the search queries; for example, someone who googles psychology may be looking for psychological help, or looking for information on studying psychology, or may simply not know the word. All these intentions lead to the same search query and cannot simply be traced back to the corresponding reason.

Conclusions

Our study uses trends in the Internet search parameters of psychological aspects according to the ICD-10 classification in order to determine how they changed during the pandemic due to COVID-19 and the containment measures. For this purpose, a comprehensive dataset was compiled to identify links and significant correlations. As a result, the study reveals three psychological aspects extremely prominent due to the pandemic and its containment measures: anxiety, worthlessness, and panic attack. These three psychological aspects vary in severity depending on the measures implemented.

Furthermore, the results show that the frequency of seeking psychological support measures has increased significantly since the pandemic.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291723001563.

Competing interest

None.

References

Ahmed, M. Z., Ahmed, O., Aibao, Z., Hanbin, S., Siyu, L., & Ahmad, A. (2020). Epidemic of COVID-19 in China and associated psychological problems. Asian Journal of Psychiatry 51, 102092. doi:10.1016/j.ajp.2020.102092.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alfano, V., & Ercolano, S. (2020). The efficacy of lockdown against COVID-19: A cross-country panel analysis. Applied Health Economics and Health Policy 18, 509517. doi:10.1007/s40258-020-00596-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alshareef, R., Al Zahrani, A., Alzhrani, M., Suwaidi, A., & Alamry, B. (2020). Impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on residents’ clinical training and psychosocial well-being in Saudi Arabia's western region. Journal of Family Medicine and Primary Care 10, 25802586. doi:10.4103/jfmpc.jfmpc_2173_20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnold, M. B. (1950). An excitatory theory of emotion. In Reymert, M. L. (Ed.), Feelings and emotions (pp. 1133). New York, Toronto, London: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Asmundson, G. J., & Taylor, S. (2020). How health anxiety influences responses to viral outbreaks like COVID-19: What all decision-makers, health authorities, and health care professionals need to know. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 71, 102211. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2020.102211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atalan, A. (2020). Is the lockdown important to prevent the COVID-19 pandemic? Effects on psychology, environment and economy-perspective. Annals of Medicine and Surgery 56, 3842. doi:10.1016/j.amsu.2020.06.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baloch, S., Baloch, M. A., Zheng, T., & Pei, X. (2020). The coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic. The Tohoku Journal of Experimental Medicine 250, 271278. doi:10.1620/tjem.250.271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bäuerle, A., Teufel, M., Musche, V., Weismüller, B., Kohler, H., Hetkamp, M.,Skoda, E.M. (2020). Increased generalized anxiety, depression and distress during the COVID-19 pandemic: A cross-sectional study in Germany. Journal of Public Health 42, 672678. doi:10.1093/pubmed/fdaa106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bhatia, M. S., Goyal, S., Singh, A., & Daral, A. (2020). COVID-19 pandemic-induced panic disorder. The Primary Care Companion for CNS Disorders 22, 26. doi:10.4088/PCC.20l02626.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowen, J. D. (2021). Psychological distance and the pandemic: Insights from Construal Level Theory and relationship science. Social and Personality Psychology Compass 15, e12594. doi:10.1111/spc3.12594.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brooks, S. K., Webster, R. K., Smith, L. E., Woodland, L., Wessly, S., & Greenberg, N. (2020). The psychological impact of quarantine and how to reduce it: Rapid review of the evidence. The Lancet 395, 912920. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30460-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bueno-Notivol, J., Gracia-García, P., Olaya, B., Lasheras, I., López-Antón, R., & Santabárbara, J. (2021). Prevalence of depression during the COVID-19 outbreak: A meta-analysis of community-based studies. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology 21, 100196. doi:10.1016/j.ijchp.2020.07.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bushuyev, S., Bushuieva, V., Onyshchenko, S., & Bondar, A. (2021). Modeling the dynamics of information panic in society. COVID-19 case. Computer Modeling and Intelligent Systems 4, 400408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cao, W., Fang, Z., Hou, G., Han, M., Xu, X., Dong, J., & Zheng, J. (2020). The psychological impact of the COVID-19 epidemic on college students in China. Psychiatry Research 287, 112934. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2020.112934.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Capdevila, I. B. i. (2004). A view of the linguistic situation in Malta. Noves SL. Revista de Sociolingüística, 14, 3.13.8.Google Scholar
Choi, E. P. H., Hui, B. P. H., & Wan, E. Y. F. (2020). Depression and anxiety in Hong Kong during COVID-19. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17, 3740. doi:10.3390/ijerph17103740.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Confederatione Svizerra, Federal Statistical Office (2021). Languages. Retrieved from https://www.bfs.admin.ch/bfs/de/home/statistiken/bevoelkerung/sprachen-religionen/sprachen.html, last access: December 2022.Google Scholar
Courtney, D., Watson, P., Battaglia, M., Mulsant, B. H., & Szatmari, P. (2020). COVID-19 impacts on child and youth anxiety and depression: Challenges and opportunities. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 65, 688691. doi:10.1177/0706743720935646.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
data.world (2022). covid-19 datasets. Retrieved from https://data.world/datasets/covid-19, last access: November 2022.Google Scholar
Davy, J. P., Scheuermaier, K., Roden, L. C., Christie, C. J., Bentley, A., Gomez-Olive, F. X., … Rae, D.E. (2021). The COVID-19 lockdown and changes in routine-oriented lifestyle behaviors and symptoms of depression, anxiety, and insomnia in South Africa. Journal of Physical Activity & Health 18, 10461057. doi:10.1123/jpah.2020-0863.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deci, E., & Ryan, M. R. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. Berlin: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dozois, D. J. (2020). Anxiety and depression in Canada during the COVID-19 pandemic: A national survey. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne 62, 136142. doi:10.1037/cap0000251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
ECDC, European Centre for Disease Prevention and Control (2021). Data on country response measures to COVID-19. Retrieved from https://www.ecdc.europa.eu/en/covid-19/data, last access: December 2022.Google Scholar
Ellwardt, L., & Präg, P. (2021). Heterogeneous mental health development during the COVID-19 pandemic in the United Kingdom. Scientific Reports 11, 17. doi:10.1038/s41598-021-95490-w.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elm, E. v, Altman, D. G., Egger, M., Pocock, S. J., Gøtzsche, P. C., & Vandenbroucke, J. P. (2007). The strengthening the reporting of observational studies in epidemiology (STROBE) statement: Guidelines for reporting observational studies. Annals of Internal Medicine 147, 573577.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernández-de-las-Peñas, C., Palacios-Ceña, D., Gómez-Mayordomo, V., Florencio, L. L., Cuadradob, M. L., Plaza-Manzano, G., & Navarro-Santana, M. (2021). Prevalence of post-COVID-19 symptoms in hospitalized and non-hospitalized COVID-19 survivors: A systematic review and meta-analysis. European Journal of Internal Medicine 92, 5570. doi:10.1016/j.ejim.2021.06.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foley, M., & Cooper, R. (2021). Workplace gender equality in the post-pandemic era: Where to next? Journal of Industrial Relations 63, 463476. doi:10.1177/00221856211035173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ginsberg, J., Mohebbi, M. H., Patel, R. S., Brammer, L., Smolinski, M. S., & Brilliant, L. (2009). Detecting influenza epidemics using search engine query data. Nature 457, 10121014. doi:10.1038/nature07634.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Google LLC (2020). Google COVID-19 Search Trends symptoms dataset. Retrieved from http://goo.gle/covid19symptomdataset, last access: November 2022.Google Scholar
Government of Cyprus, Statistical Service Cyprus (2022). Statistical Service Cyprus. Retrieved from https://www.cystat.gov.cy/en/default, last access: December 2022.Google Scholar
Huremović, D. (2019). Psychiatry of pandemics. Cham: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Javelot, H., & Weiner, L. (2020). Panic and pandemic: Review of the literature on the links between panic disorder and the SARS-CoV-2 epidemic. L'encephale 46, 9398. doi:10.1016/j.encep.2020.05.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaushik, M., & Guleria, N. (2020). The impact of pandemic COVID-19 in workplace. European Journal of Business and Management 12, 110. doi: 10.7176/EJBM/12-15-02.Google Scholar
King, J. D., McQuaid, A., Leeson, V. C., Samuel, O., Grant, J., Azeem, M. S. I., … Crawford, M. J. (2022). The association of severe COVID anxiety with poor social functioning, quality of life, and protective behaviours among adults in United Kingdom: A cross-sectional study. Research Square, PREPRINT (Version 1), 115. doi:10.21203/rs.3.rs-2064928/v1.Google Scholar
Landi, G., Pakenham, K. I., Boccolini, G., Grandi, S., & Tossani, E. (2020). Health anxiety and mental health outcome during COVID-19 lockdown in Italy: The mediating and moderating roles of psychological flexibility. Frontiers in Psychology 11, 2195. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York, Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pigeron-Piroth, I., & Fehlen, F. (2015). Les langues dans les offres d'emploi au Luxembourg (1984–2014). Walferdange: Université du Luxemboug.Google Scholar
Planchuelo-Gómez, Á., Odriozola-González, P., Irurtia, M. J., & de Luis-García, R. (2020). Longitudinal evaluation of the psychological impact of the COVID-19 crisis in Spain. Journal of Affective Disorders 277, 842849. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2020.09.018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Qiu, J., Zhao, R., Yang, S., & Dong, K. (2017). Informetrics: Theory, methods and applications. Singapore: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saadat, S. H., Izadi, M., Jonaidi Jafari, N., Abolghasemi, H., Jamalimoghadamsiyahkali, S., Jamalian, A., … Javanbakht, M. (2020). Fear and panic of COVID-19. International Journal of Travel Medicine and Global Health 8, 9192. doi:10.34172/IJTMGH.2020.15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sharma, G. D., Ghura, A. S., Mahendru, M., Erkut, B., Kaur, T., & Bedi, D. (2020). Panic during COVID-19 pandemic! A qualitative investigation into the psychosocial experiences of a sample of Indian people. Frontiers in Psychology 11, 575491. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2020.575491.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Statistics Finland (2021). Official Statistics of Finland: Population structure. Retrieved from https://www.tilastokeskus.fi/tup/suoluk/suoluk_vaesto_en.html, last access: December 2022.Google Scholar
Steegmans, J. (2021). The pearls and perils of Google Trends: A housing market application. Big Data 9, 443453. doi:10.1089/big.2020.0183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stillman, P. E., Fujita, K., Sheldon, O., & Trope, Y. (2018). From “me” to “we”: The role of construal level in promoting maximized joint outcomes. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 147, 1625. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2018.05.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Szilagyi, I., Ullrich, T., Lang-Illievich, K., Klivinyi, C., Schittek, G. A., Simonis, H., & Bornemann-Cimenti, H. (2021). Google Trends for pain search terms in the world's most populated regions before and after the first recorded COVID-19 case: Infodemiological study. Journal of Medical Internet Research 23(4):e27214. doi:10.2196/27214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tang, H., & Ng, J. H. K. (2006). Googling for a diagnosis—use of Google as a diagnostic aid: Internet based study. The BMJ 333, 11431145. doi:10.1136/bmj.39003.640567.AE.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
The Lancet (2020). COVID-19: Fighting panic with information. Lancet (London, England) 395(10224), 537. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30379-2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Parys, J. (2012). Languages in Ireland. Retrieved from https://www.languageknowledge.eu/countries/ireland, last access: December 2022.Google Scholar
Vizard, T., Davis, J., White, E., & Beynon, B. (2020). Coronavirus and depression in adults, Office for National Statistics. Retrieved from https://www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/wellbeing/articles/coronavirusanddepressioninadultsgreatbritain/julytoaugust2021, last access: December 2022.Google Scholar
Wang, C., Pan, R., Wan, X., Tan, Y., Xu, L., Ho, C. S., & Ho, R. C. (2020). Immediate psychological responses and associated factors during the initial stage of the 2019 coronavirus disease (COVID-19) epidemic among the general population in China. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17, 1729. doi:10.3390/ijerph17051729.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization (2021 a). Coronavirus – Global Situation. Retrieved from https://covid19.who.int, last access: December 2022.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (2021 b). World Health Organization announces COVID-19 outbreak a pandemic. Retrieved from https://www.who.int/europe/emergencies/situations/covid-19 last access: December 2022.Google Scholar
Worldometers.info. (2021). COVID-19 CORONAVIRUS PANDEMIC. Retrieved from https://www.worldometers.info/coronavirus/, last access: December 2022.Google Scholar
Yan, H., Zhu, Y., Gu, J., Huang, Y., Sun, H., Zhang, X., … Chen, S. X. (2021). Better strategies for containing COVID-19 pandemic: A study of 25 countries via a vSIADR model. Proceedings of the Royal Society A 477, 20200440. doi:10.1098/rspa.2020.0440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yoon, M. S., Feyissa, I. F., & Suk, S. W. (2021). Panic and trust during COVID-19: A cross-sectional study on immigrants in South Korea. Healthcare 9, 199. doi:10.3390/healthcare9020199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yossi, M. (2012). Insights into what the world is searching for – the new Google Trends. Inside Search. The official Google Search Blog. September 27, 2012.Google Scholar
Zheng, L., Miao, M., Lim, J., Li, M., Nie, S., & Zhang, X. (2020). Is lockdown bad for social anxiety in COVID-19 regions?: A national study in the SOR perspective. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17, 4561. doi:10.3390/ijerph17124561.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhong, B., Huang, Y., & Liu, Q. (2021). Mental health toll from the coronavirus: Social media usage reveals Wuhan residents’ depression and secondary trauma in the COVID-19 outbreak. Computers in Human Behavior 114, 106524. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2020.106524.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Data flow overview of the process of data acquisition, preparation, and consolidation.

Figure 1

Figure 2. The data acquisition provides a time series for each country and each search term. The upper visualization shows the data on “anxiety/anxious” searches in Germany; the lower one accordingly for Portugal. Each graph shows in red the relative number of searches scaled to 100%, with the uncertainty due to sampling and rounding plotted as the estimated standard deviation for each time point as an additional thin red line; the black line shows the moving average of a two-month time window. The temporal relation to the pandemic is based on the number of nationally reported infection cases, which is also scaled and shown as a dark gray plot. The counter measures in force at the national level at that time are plotted below the time series.

Figure 2

Table 1. Each European country implemented countermeasures independently (Yan et al., 2021)

Figure 3

Table 2. Each countermeasure is tested statistically on its influence on Internet search terms of psychological disorders and symptoms

Figure 4

Table 3. Analogous to Table 2, each countermeasure is tested statistically on its influence on Internet search terms of psychological disorders and symptoms

Figure 5

Table 4. The overall impact of the pandemic on individual search parameters before and during the pandemic in Europe

Figure 6

Figure 3. The overall distribution of frequencies in the population-weighted data set of all countries (in a range between 0 and 100) of most search terms related to psychological aspects in the time ranging three years before and since the official pandemic declaration by the WHO.

Supplementary material: File

Szilagyi et al. supplementary material 1

Szilagyi et al. supplementary material
Download Szilagyi et al. supplementary material 1(File)
File 420.9 KB
Supplementary material: File

Szilagyi et al. supplementary material 2

Szilagyi et al. supplementary material
Download Szilagyi et al. supplementary material 2(File)
File 229.2 KB
Supplementary material: File

Szilagyi et al. supplementary material 3

Szilagyi et al. supplementary material
Download Szilagyi et al. supplementary material 3(File)
File 31.3 KB