Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-n9wrp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-19T03:53:02.787Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The social defeat hypothesis of schizophrenia: a parsimonious explanation for multiple psychosis risk factors?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 November 2020

Rik Schalbroeck*
Affiliation:
Rivierduinen Institute for Mental Healthcare, Leiden, The Netherlands School for Mental Health and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
*
Author for correspondence: Rik Schalbroeck, E-mail: rschalbroeck@gmail.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Correspondence
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

In their recent meta-analysis, Selten, van der Ven, and Termorshuizen (Reference Selten, van der Ven and Termorshuizen2020) report an increased risk of psychotic disorder in first- and second-generation migrants, and show that this risk is associated with skin colour and region of origin and destination. The authors argue that the social defeat hypothesis of schizophrenia offers an explanation for these findings. However, this hypothesis has limitations related to the definition and measurement of social defeat, which hinder its use as a scientific explanation for the risks of psychosis in migrants and other groups.

The social defeat hypothesis states that the negative experience of being excluded from the majority group (i.e. social exclusion) leads to sensitization and/or increased baseline activity of the mesolimbic dopamine system and thereby to an increased risk of schizophrenia or psychosis (Selten, van der Ven, Rutten, & Cantor-Graae, Reference Selten, van der Ven, Rutten and Cantor-Graae2013). The hypothesis originally aimed to provide a parsimonious explanation for four psychosis risk factors, including migration, low intelligence quotient (IQ), illicit drug use, and urban upbringing (Selten & Cantor-Graae, Reference Selten and Cantor-Graae2005), and was later also applied to groups such as individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and lesbian, gay, and bisexual individuals.

At first glance, it might seem defensible that social defeat is a common risk factor between these groups. However, since social defeat has been defined inconsistently, it is difficult to evaluate whether it truly is associated with the risk of psychosis in these individuals. That is, social defeat is defined in some publications (including the present meta-analysis) as social exclusion (e.g. Selten et al., Reference Selten, van der Ven, Rutten and Cantor-Graae2013, Reference Selten, van der Ven and Termorshuizen2020), and in other publications as subordinate position or outsider status (e.g. Selten, Booij, Buwalda, & Meyer-Lindenberg, Reference Selten, Booij, Buwalda and Meyer-Lindenberg2017; Selten & Cantor-Graae, Reference Selten and Cantor-Graae2005). Although these constructs can be related, their interchangeable use is unjustified since they are not equivalent (e.g. in contrast, a person can be in a subordinate position within a majority group).

As a result, it is unclear whether the hypothesis proposes that subordinate position, outsider status, social exclusion, or some combination of them accounts for the observed risks of psychosis in migrants and other groups (this issue is complicated even further when definitions from animal and depression research are taken into account, which define defeat in terms of losing and/or a sense of failed struggle; Gilbert, Allan, Brough, Melley, and Miles, Reference Gilbert, Allan, Brough, Melley and Miles2002). This lack of uniform definition of defeat also demonstrably results in conflicting interpretations of the hypothesis. For example, social defeat was operationalized in one study as loneliness as this resembled the subjective experience of social exclusion (Gevonden et al., Reference Gevonden, Booij, van den Brink, Heijtel, van Os and Selten2014), and in another as low social rank, which was contrasted with loneliness (Jaya, Ascone, & Lincoln, Reference Jaya, Ascone and Lincoln2017).

A second limitation of the hypothesis is that the authors argue that defeat can be difficult to measure because people do not want to admit having this experience (e.g. Selten et al., Reference Selten, van der Ven, Rutten and Cantor-Graae2013). This statement is problematic for several reasons. First, it is unclear what people do not want to admit since social defeat has not been defined uniformly. Second, one can question whether this statement is true, as other studies did, for instance, measure experiences of outsider status and reduced value (van Nierop et al., Reference van Nierop, van Os, Gunther, van Zelst, De Graaf, Ten Have and van Winkel2014). Third, this statement limits the hypothesis' falsifiability, as it provides researchers with an excuse in the case of null-findings, allowing them to claim that they found no relationship between reports of social defeat and psychosis because their subjects did not want to admit feeling defeated.

Several solutions to the problem of immeasurability have been proposed. First, the authors state that the hypothesis can be tested by examining risks of psychosis and/or dopaminergic functioning in groups presumed to be socially defeated (Selten et al., Reference Selten, van der Ven, Rutten and Cantor-Graae2013, Reference Selten, van der Ven and Termorshuizen2020). However, often it is nearly impossible to unequivocally establish whether (or to what extent) a group is socially defeated, since the experience of defeat is subjective and does not necessarily vary in proportion to exposure to social adversity (Selten & Cantor-Graae, Reference Selten and Cantor-Graae2005). For example, one researcher might argue that individuals with ASD experience more social adversity and thus feel more defeated, whereas another might argue the opposite by stating that these individuals do not care about or notice their social adversity. Subsequent empirical observations then still cannot test the hypothesis. Moreover, groups presumed to be socially defeated usually share exposure to other factors, such as threat and negative affect, which could alternatively underlie observed risks of psychosis.

Second, it has been suggested that alternative measurements (e.g. of certain cognitive schemas or self-esteem) can be used to (indirectly) assess social defeat (Selten et al., Reference Selten, van der Ven, Rutten and Cantor-Graae2013). However, these measurements (and/or complex interactions between them) might be associated with the development of psychosis even if they are unrelated to social defeat, and such unique contributions are likely underestimated or missed if they are solely interpreted as being indicative of social defeat.

In the past 15 years the social defeat hypothesis of schizophrenia has provided an intuitive and influential explanation for the role of the social environment in the development of psychosis. It has had demonstrable predictive power and seems to grasp some aspect of the social world that is important for understanding the development of psychosis. However, its use as a scientific hypothesis is hindered by the unclear definition and presumed immeasurability of social defeat. As a result, at this time it cannot be convincingly argued that the social defeat hypothesis provides a parsimonious explanation for the risk of psychosis in migrants and other groups.

Financial support

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Conflict of interest

None.

References

Gevonden, M., Booij, J., van den Brink, W., Heijtel, D., van Os, J., & Selten, J. P. (2014). Increased release of dopamine in the striata of young adults with hearing impairment and its relevance for the social defeat hypothesis of schizophrenia. JAMA Psychiatry, 71(12), 13641372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilbert, P., Allan, S., Brough, S., Melley, S., & Miles, J. (2002). Relationship of anhedonia and anxiety to social rank, defeat and entrapment. Journal of Affective Disorders, 71(1–3), 141151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jaya, E. S., Ascone, L., & Lincoln, T. M. (2017). Social adversity and psychosis: The mediating role of cognitive vulnerability. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 43(3), 557565.Google ScholarPubMed
Selten, J. P., Booij, J., Buwalda, B., & Meyer-Lindenberg, A. (2017). Biological mechanisms whereby social exclusion may contribute to the etiology of psychosis: A narrative review. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 43(2), 287292.Google Scholar
Selten, J. P., & Cantor-Graae, E. (2005). Social defeat: Risk factor for schizophrenia? The British Journal of Psychiatry, 187(2), 101102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Selten, J. P., van der Ven, E., Rutten, B. P., & Cantor-Graae, E. (2013). The social defeat hypothesis of schizophrenia: An update. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 39(6), 11801186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Selten, J. P., van der Ven, E., & Termorshuizen, F. (2020). Migration and psychosis: A meta-analysis of incidence studies. Psychological Medicine, 50(2), 303313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Nierop, M., van Os, J., Gunther, N., van Zelst, C., De Graaf, R., Ten Have, M., … van Winkel, R. (2014). Does social defeat mediate the association between childhood trauma and psychosis? Evidence from the NEMESIS-2 study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 129(6), 467476.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed