Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-wg55d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-08T18:09:30.784Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Stress, anxiety and other emotions in Japanese modern dance performance

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 May 2015

J. H. Kerr*
Affiliation:
University of Tsukuba, Japan
C. Kawaguchi
Affiliation:
University of Tsukuba, Japan
M. Oiwa
Affiliation:
University of Tsukuba, Japan
Y. Terayama
Affiliation:
University of Tsukuba, Japan
A. Zukawa
Affiliation:
University of Tsukuba, Japan
*
Institute of Health and Sport Sciences, University of Tsukuba, Tsukuba-shi, Ibaraki-ken, Japan 305, kerr@taiiku.tsukuba.ac.jp

Abstract

Two reversal theory-based studies investigated dancers' psychological experience at final rehearsal, and at three performances in an annual competition. In Study 1, 23 Japanese modern dance specialists completed the Tension and Effort Stress Inventory immediately before and after performance. Pre-study expectations that dancers would experience higher levels of unpleasant emotions (including performance anxiety or stage fright) and stress pre-competition than pre-final rehearsal were not confirmed. Dancers experienced significantly higher levels of unpleasant emotions before final rehearsal than before competition. Also contrary to expectations, competing was found not to be more stressful and not to require greater efforts to cope, than rehearsing. Study 2 set out to investigate dancers' basic motives for dancing and to show whether the anxiety-to-excitement reversal phenomena could be discerned during dance performance. Using the same experimental procedure as Study 1, a similar group of 15 dancers completed a dance performance experience questionnaire. The results indicated that, for the majority of dancers, the primary reason for dancing was either the paratelic, telic, arousal-seeking, or alloic-sympathy meta-motivational categories, and that many dancers experienced anxiety to excitement reversals.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © University of Papua New Guinea and the Centre for Southeast Asian Studies, Northern Territory University, Australia 1999

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abel, J. L., & Larkin, K. T. (1990). Anticipation of performance among musicians: Physiological arousal, confidence and state anxiety. Psychology of Music, 18, 171182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Apter, M. J. (1982). The experience of motivation: The theory of psychological reversals. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Apter, M. J. (1989). Reversal theory: Motivation, emotion and personality. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Apter, M. J. (1992). The dangerous edge. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Apter, M. J., & Batler, R. (1996). Gratuitous risk: A study of parachuting. In Svebak, S., & Apter, M. J. (Eds.), Stress and health: A reversal theory perspective (pp. 119129). Washington, DC: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Bru, E., Mykletun, R. J., & Svebak, S. (1997). Back pain, dysphoric versus euphoric moods and the experience of stress and efforts in female hospital staff. Personality and Individual Differences, in press.Google Scholar
Campbell, D. T., & Stanley, J. C. (1963). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for research. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (1979). Quasi-experimentation: Design and analysis issues for field settings. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Cox, T. (1978). Stress. London: Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craske, M. G., & Craig, K. D. (1984). Musical performance anxiety: The three-systems model and self-efficacy theory. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 22, 267280.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edwards, C. P., & Whiting, B. B. (1980). Differential socialization of girls and boys in the light of cross-cultural research. In Super, C. & Harkness, S. (Eds.), Anthropological perspectives on child development. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Fredrikson, M., & Gunnarsson, R. (1992). Psychobiology of stage fright: The effect of public performance on neuroendocrine, cardiovascular and subjective ratings. Biological Psychology, 33, 5162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamann, D. L. (1982). An assessment of anxiety in instrumental and vocal performances. Journal of Research in Musical Education, 30, 7790.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kerr, J.H., & Cox, T. (1991). Arousal and individual differences in sport. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 10751085.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kerr, J. H., & Svebak, S. (1994). The acute effects of participation in sports on mood. Personality and Individual Differences, 16, 159166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Konijn, E. A. (1991). What's on between the actor and his audience? Empirical analysis of emotion processes in the theatre. In Wilson, G. D. (Ed.), Psychology and performing arts (pp. 5973). Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
Males, J.R., & Kerr, J.H. (1996). Stress, emotion and performance in elite slalom canoeists. The Sport Psychologist, 10, 1737.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marchant-Haycox, & Wilson, J. (1992). Personality and stress in performing artists. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 10611068.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steptoe, A. (1989). Stress, coping and stage fright in professional musicians. Psychology of Music, 77, 311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steptoe, A., & Fidler, H. (1987). Stage fright in orchestral musicians: A study of cognitive and behavioural strategies in performance anxiety. British Journal of Psychology, 75, 241249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steptoe, A., Malik, F., Pay, C., Pearson, P., Price, C., & Win, Z. (1995). The impact of stage fright on student actors. British Journal of Psychology, 86, 2739.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Svebak, S. (1993). The development of the Tension and Effort Stress Inventory (TESI). In Kerr, J. H., Murgatroyd, S., & Apter, M. J. (Eds.), Advances in reversal theory (pp. 189204). Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
Svebak, S., Ursin, H., Endresen, I., Hjelmen, A. M., & Apter, M. J. (1991). Back pain and the experience of stress, efforts and moods. Psychology and Health, 5, 307314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1996). Using multivariate statistics (3rd edn). New York: Harper Collins.Google Scholar
Tharp, T. (1992). Push comes to shove. New York: Bantam.Google Scholar
Whiting, B. B., & Edwards, C. P. (1988). Children of different worlds: The formation of social behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar