Hostname: page-component-7bb8b95d7b-lvwk9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-09-17T08:29:04.906Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Essentialism and the Expression of Social Stereotypes: A Comparative Study of Spain, Brasil and England

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 January 2013

Marcos Emanoel Pereira*
Affiliation:
Universidade Federal da Bahia (Brazil)
José Luis Álvaro Estramiana
Affiliation:
Universidad Complutense (Spain)
Inge Schweiger Gallo
Affiliation:
Universidad Complutense (Spain)
*
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Marcos Emanoel Pereira. Universidade Federal da Bahia. Instituto de Psicologia. Campus Universitário. São Lázaro. Salvador, BA. (Brazil). E-mail: emanoel@terra.com.br

Abstract

Over the past few years, one of the most productive directions in the study of the activation and application of stereotypes has been provided by the essentialist concept of categorization. The research presented here studied the impact of two dimensions of essentialist beliefs - naturalism and entitativity-by using data collected from Brazil, Spain and England. The aim was to test whether there was a greater degree of essentialization among the naturalizable categories (sex, age and race) than among the entitative categories (economic condition, religion, political orientation, nationality and social condition). The results not only showed that participants hold more essentialist beliefs with regard to naturalistic categories but also showed the differences in the degree of essentialization across the three cultures. A discussion is conducted on the implications of the present findings, and on the heuristic value of the theoretical model (of the bidimensional nature of essentialism) adopted by this research.

En los últimos años, la concepción esencialista de la categorización se ha consolidado como una de las orientaciones teóricas más fecundas para el estudio de la activación y aplicación de los estereotipos. La presente investigación transcultural realizada en Brasil, España e Inglaterra estudió el impacto de dos dimensiones de las creencias esencialistas –el naturalismo y la entitatividad- sobre la expresión de los estereotipos sociales y comprobó si se produce una mayor esencialización de las categorías naturalizables de sexo, edad y raza, en comparación con el grado de esencialización de las categorías entitativas relativas a la condición económica, la religión, la orientación política, la nacionalidad y la condición social. Los resultados obtenidos mostraron que en las diferentes dimensiones comparativas las categorías naturalizables fueron esencializadas con mayor intensidad que las categorías entitativas. Asimismo, se observaron diferencias entre los tres países en el grado de esencialización. Se discuten las implicaciones de los resultados, así como el valor heurístico del modelo teórico adoptado en la presente investigación sobre la naturaleza bidimensional del esencialismo.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allport, G. (1962). La naturaleza del prejuicio. Buenos Aires: EudebaGoogle Scholar
Betancour, V., Rodríguez, A., Quiles, M., & Rodríguez, T. (2005). Relación de la infrahumanización del exogrupo con los procesos de inferencia y memoria. Psicothema, 17, 447452.Google Scholar
Blair, I., & Banaji, M. (1996). Automatic and controlled processes in stereotype priming. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 11421163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brewer, M., Hong, Y., & Li, Q. (2004). Dynamic entitivity: perceiving groups as actors. In Yzerbyt, V. C. Judd, C., & Corneille, O. (Eds.), The psychology of group perception: perceived variability, entitativity and essentialism (pp. 2538). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Campbell, D. (1958). Common fate, similarity, and other indices of the status of aggregates of persons as social entities. Behavioural Science, 3, 1425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Demoulin, S., Leyens, J.-P., & Yzerbyt, V. (2006). Lay theories of essentialism. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 9, 2542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denson, T., Lickel, B., Curtis, M., Stenstrom, D., & Ames, D. (2006). The roles of entitativity and essenciality in judgments of collective responsability. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 9, 4361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Devine, P. (1989) Stereotypes and prejudice: their automatic and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dweck, C., Chiu, C., & Hong, Y. (1995). Implicit theories and their role in judgements and reactions: a world from two perspectives. Psychological Inquiry, 6, 267285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Estrada, C., Yzerbyt, V., & Seron, E. (2004). Efecto del esencialismo psicológico sobre las teorías ingenuas de las diferencias grupales. Psicothema, 16, 181186.Google Scholar
Estrada, C., & Yzertbyt, V. (2007). Teorías implícitas y esencialismo psicológico: Herramientas conceptuales para el estudio de las relaciones entre y dentro de los grupos. Psykhe, 16, 111121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fazio, R., & Olson, M. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: their meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 297327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fishbein, H.D. (1996). Peer prejudice and discrimination: evolutionary, cultural and developmental dynamics. Boulder: Westview.Google Scholar
Gelman, S. (2004). Psychological essentialism in children. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 404409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gelman, S., & Wellman, H. (1991). Insides and essences: early understandings of the non-obvious. Cognition, 38, 213244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamilton, D., Sherman, S., & Rodgers, J. (2004). Perceiving the groupness of groups: Entitativity, homogeneity, essentialism, and stereotypes. In Yzerbyt, V. C., Judd, C., & Corneille, O. (Eds.), The psychology of group perception: perceived variability, entitativity and essentialism (pp. 3960). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Haslam, N. (1998). Natural kinds, human kinds, and essentialism. Social Research, 65, 110.Google Scholar
Haslam, N., Bain, P., Douge, L., Lee, M., & Bastian, B. (2005). More human than you: attributing humanness to self and others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 937950.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haslam, N., Bastian, B., Bain, P., & Kashima, Y. (2006). Psychological essentialism, implicit theories, and intergroup relations. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 9, 6376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haslam, N., Bastian, B., & Bisset, M. (2004). Essentialist beliefs about personality and their implications. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 16611673.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heider, F. (1965). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Hirschfeld, L. A. (2001). On a folk theory of society: children, evolution, and mental representation of social groups. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 107117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jost, J., & Major, B. (Eds.) (2001). The psychology of legitimacy: emerging perspectives in justice, ideology and intergroup relations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Keller, J. (2005). In genes we trust: the biological component of psychological essentialism and its relationship to mechanisms of motivated social cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 686702.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, S., Plaks, J., Hong, Y., Chiu, C., & Dweck, C. (2001). Static vs. dynamic theories and the perceptions of groups: different routes to different destinations. Personality and Social Psychological Review, 5, 156168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macrae, C., & Bodenhausen, G. (2000). Social cognition: thinking categorically about others. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 93120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mahalingam, R. (2003a). Essentialism, culture, and beliefs about gender among the Aravanis of Tamil Nadu, India. Sex Roles, 49, 489496.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mahalingam, R. (2003b). Essentialism, culture, and power: representations of social class. Journal of Social Issues, 59, 733749.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malle, B. (1999). How people explain behavior: a new theorethical framework. Personality and Psychology Social Review, 3, 2348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malle, B. (2006). How the mind explains behavior. Folk explanations, meaning and social interactions. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Medin, D. (1989). Concepts and conceptual structure. American Psychologist, 44, 14691481.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Medin, D., Goldstone, R., & Gentner, D. (1993). Respects for similarity. Psychological Review, 44, 14691481.Google Scholar
Pereira, M. (2002). Psicologia social dos estereótipos. São Paulo: EPU.Google Scholar
Picket, C. (2001). The effects of entitativity beliefs on implicit comparisons between group members. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 515525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prentice, D., & Miller, D. (2006). Essentializing differences between women and men. Psychological Science, 17, 129135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wegner, D., & Bargh, J. (1998). Control and automaticity in social life. In Gilbert, D. Fiske, S., & Lindzey, G.. (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (pp. 446496). Boston: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Yzerbyt, V., & Rogier, A. (2001). Blame it on the group: entitativity, subjective essentialism and social attribution. In Jost, J., & Major, B. (Eds.), The psychology of legitimacy: emerging perspectives in justice, ideology and intergroup relations (pp. 103134). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Yzerbyt, V., Rocher, S., & Schadron, G. (1997). Stereotype as explanations: a subjective esentialistic view of group perception. In Spears, R. Oakes, P. Ellemers, N., & Haslam, A. (Eds.), The Psychology of stereotyping and group life (pp.2050). London: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar