Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-vsgnj Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-18T05:19:11.191Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Induced steady color shifts from temporally varying surrounds

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 September 2006

ANTHONY D. D'ANTONA
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois
STEVEN K. SHEVELL
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois Department of Opthalmology and Visual Science, University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois

Abstract

The color appearance of a physically steady central region can appear to vary over time if a surrounding chromatic light varies in time. The induced temporal variation, however, is strongly attenuated at surround temporal frequencies above ∼3 Hz. At these higher temporal frequencies, the central region appears steady (De Valois et al., 1986). The posited explanation is a cortical low-pass temporal filter. Here, we investigate whether higher temporal-frequency surrounds induce color shifts in the steady appearance of the central test. Surrounds modulated in time along the l or s chromatic direction of MacLeod-Boynton color space were symmetric around equal-energy white (EEW). The temporal frequency of the surround was varied. If observers perceived the central test to be temporally modulating between two points in time, they set two separate matches to the extreme points of this modulation. If the central test appeared steady in time, then color matches were made to this steady appearance. Corroborating previous reports, measurements showed that surround temporal frequencies below ∼3 Hz induced temporal modulation. At higher temporal frequencies, however, the surround induced steady color shifts, compared to a steady surround at its time average (EEW). The measurements imply that a nonlinear neural process affects chromatic induction from time-varying context.

Type
TEMPORAL FACTORS
Copyright
© 2006 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Cavanagh, P., MacLeod, D.I., & Anstis, S.M. (1987). Equiluminance: spatial and temporal factors and the contribution of blue-sensitive cones. Journal of the Optical Society of America A 4, 14281438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Valois, R.L., Webster, M.A., De Valois, K.K., & Lingelbach, B. (1986). Temporal properties of brightness and color induction. Vision Research 26, 887897.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kinney, J.S. (1965). Effect of exposure duration on induced color. Journal of the Optical Society of America A 55, 731766.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kinney, J.S. (1967). Color induction using asynchronous flashes. Vision Research 7, 299318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krauskopf, J., Zaidi, Q., & Mandler, M.B. (1986). Mechanisms of simultaneous color induction. Journal of the Optical Society of America A 3, 17521757.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacLeod, D.I. & Boynton, R.M. (1979). Chromaticity diagram showing cone excitation by stimuli of equal luminance. Journal of the Optical Society of America A 69, 11831186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Monnier, P. & Shevell, S.K. (2003). Large shifts in color appearance from patterned chromatic backgrounds. Nature Neuroscience 6, 801802.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Monnier, P. & Shevell, S.K. (2004). Chromatic induction from S-cone patterns. Vision Research 44, 849856.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schirillo, J.A. & Shevell, S.K. (2000). Role of perceptual organization in chromatic induction. Journal of the Optical Society of America A 17, 244254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shevell, S.K. & Wei, J. (1998). Chromatic induction: Border contrast or adaptation to surrounding light? Vision Research 38, 15611566.Google Scholar
Singer, B. & D'Zmura, M. (1994). Color contrast induction. Vision Research 34, 31113126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, V.C., Jin, P.Q., & Pokorny, J. (2001). The role of spatial frequency in color induction. Vision Research 41, 10071021.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, V.C. & Pokorny, J. (1975). Spectral sensitivity of the foveal cone photopigments between 400 and 500 nm. Vision Research 15, 161171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stockman, A. & Plummer, D.J. (1998). Color from invisible flicker: A failure of the Talbot-Plateau law caused by an early “hard” saturating nonlinearity used to partition the human short-wave cone pathway. Vision Research 38, 37033728.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xian, S.X. & Shevell, S.K. (2004). Changes in color appearance caused by perceptual grouping. Visual Neuroscience 21, 383388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yeh, T., Lee, B.B., & Kremers, J. (1995). Temporal response of ganglion cells of the macaque retina to cone-specific modulation. Journal of the Optical Society of America A 12, 456464.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zaidi, Q., Yoshimi, B., Flanigan, N., & Canova, A. (1992). Lateral interactions within color mechanisms in simultaneous induced contrast. Vision Research 32, 16951707.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zele, A.J. & Vingrys, A.J. (2005). Cathode-ray-tube monitor artefacts in neurophysiology. Journal of Neuroscience Methods 141, 17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar