Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-s2hrs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-18T11:26:47.430Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Modeling Health Event Impact on Smoking Cessation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2024

Edwin D. Boudreaux*
Affiliation:
Departments of Emergency Medicine, Psychiatry, And Quantitative Health Sciences, University of Massachusetts Medical School, Worcester MA, USA
Erin O’Hea
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Stonehill College, Easton MA, USA
Bo Wang
Affiliation:
Department of Quantitative Health Sciences, University of Massachusetts Medical School, Worcester MA, USA
Eugene Quinn
Affiliation:
Department of Mathematics, Stonehill College, Easton MA, USA
Aaron L. Bergman
Affiliation:
Departments of Emergency Medicine and Psychiatry, University of Massachusetts Medical School, Worcester MA, USA
Beth C. Bock
Affiliation:
The Miriam Hospital, Department of Psychiatry & Human Behavior, Warren Alpert School of Medicine, Brown University, Providence RI, USA
Bruce M. Becker
Affiliation:
Behavioral and Social Science, The School of Public Health, Brown University, Providence RI, USA
*
Correspondence should be addressed to Edwin D. Boudreaux; edwin.boudreaux@umassmed.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background. This study examined how cognitive and affective constructs related to an acute health event predict smoking relapse following an acute cardiac health event. Methods. Participants were recruited from emergency departments and completed cognitive and emotional measures at enrollment and ecological momentary assessments (EMA) for 84 days postvisit. Results. Of 394 participants, only 35 (8.9%) remained abstinent 84 days postvisit. Time to relapse was positively associated with age, actual illness severity, self-efficacy, and quit intentions. Conclusions. Older, seriously ill patients with strong confidence and intentions to quit smoking remain abstinent longer after discharge, but most still relapse within three months.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © 2022 Edwin D. Boudreaux et al.

1. Introduction

Despite the remarkable decreases in cigarette smoking rates since the 1960s, smoking remains one of the most significant preventable causes of death among Americans, accounting for around 480,000 deaths annually [1]. Unfortunately, the rate of smoking cessation has recently decreased, and smoking prevalence has stabilized at around 15.5% of the United States adult population, with no measurable changes since 2015 [Reference Jamal, Phillips and Gentzke2]. Moreover, remaining smokers disproportionally represent minority, uninsured, impoverished, and nonmetropolitan populations [3]. Continued research into understanding and promoting smoking cessation remains one of the most important public health priorities for the United States [Reference Jha, Ramasundarahettige and Landsman4].

Studies have confirmed that both initial quit attempts and sustained smoking cessation occur at higher-than-expected rates after an individual experiences an acute health event, like myocardial infarction, or is diagnosed with a major medical illness, like cancer [Reference Bernstein, Boudreaux and Cabral5Reference Tofler, May, Bartrop, Kirkness, Glinatsis and de Burgh12]. However, these studies also confirm that the positive effect of such a sentinel health event does not occur with every person, and, all too often, the change is short lived, with many smokers relapsing within only a few days or weeks after the event. Understanding the drivers of initial and long-term smoking cessation after an acute health event is important to inform interventions designed to maximize the “teachable moment” to get more smokers to attempt, to quit, and to help quitters transition to sustained abstinence. The sentinel event method [Reference Boudreaux, Bock and O’Hea6] describes a systematic process for developing conceptual models to explain how health events can trigger positive behavior change. It is not a health behavior theory itself; rather, it provides a framework for identifying and testing cognitive, affective, and event-related constructs, typically by selecting and adapting good fit constructs from existing health behavior theories, and studying their associations with behavior change longitudinally.

Using the sentinel event method, O’Hea and colleagues [Reference O’Hea, Abar, Bock, Chapman and Boudreaux10] studied short-term smoking cessation over the 7 days after an ED visit for cardiac-related symptoms by selecting and testing constructs adapted from the theory of planned behavior [Reference Ajzen13] and self-regulation theory [Reference Rothman, Baldwin, Hertel, Fuglestad, Vohs and Baumeister14]. Their model emphasized the importance of behavioral intentions as a mediator between event-related constructs, such as perceived illness severity and smoking-related causal attributions, and smoking cessation, as well as teasing out the role of both cognitive and affective variables. Their results confirmed that intentions to quit smoking shortly before discharge were strongly associated with sustained abstinence over the 7 days after discharge and fully or partially mediated the associations between other predictors and abstinence. Furthermore, several novel findings were reported. Perceived illness severity, measured by assessing how serious the individual believed his/her illness to be, was positively associated with 7-day abstinence, even after controlling for actual illness severity, exhibiting a direct association with 7-day abstinence of nearly the same magnitude as quit intentions. However, the relationship was complex and depended heavily on the time anchor used for the perception ratings. Perceptions at initial symptom onset were more strongly associated with abstinence than perceptions upon initial arrival to the hospital or toward the end of the encounter. Moreover, the interaction between perceived illness severity and smoking-related causal attributions approached significance, suggesting that illness severity may have been more likely to inspire behavior change when the individual strongly believed the health problem was caused or aggravated by smoking. These findings, using more nuanced measurement of perceived illness severity over time than previous studies using aggregate measures [Reference Baird and Eliasziw15, Reference Weinstein16], may help explain the ambiguity in the literature surrounding this construct. Some studies have found it is important in predicting smoking cessation [Reference O’Hea, Abar, Bock, Chapman and Boudreaux10, Reference Boudreaux, Abar and O’Hea17], whereas others have not [Reference Boudreaux, Moon, Baumann, Camargo, O’Hea and Ziedonis18]. Severity perceptions are not static, and aggregating severity perceptions may hide or confound associations with intentions and behavior.

In sum, when a sentinel health event occurs, there is a complex relationship between actual illness severity, perceived severity, emotional reactions to the event, causal attributions, quit intentions, and subsequent smoking behavior. Further, the importance of perceived severity and illness-related emotions at different time anchors merits further evaluation, as these are not static constructs. The present study sought to expand the literature by examining constructs previously identified as a good fit for the population and research question [Reference Boudreaux, Bock and O’Hea6, Reference O’Hea, Abar, Bock, Chapman and Boudreaux10] using a sample of ED patients being evaluated for acute coronary syndrome. This study extended the predictor pool used by O’Hea and colleagues [Reference O’Hea, Abar, Bock, Chapman and Boudreaux10] by adding a measure of self-efficacy from social learning theory [Reference Bandura19]. In this context, self-efficacy reflects the confidence in one’s ability to quit smoking. Finally, the study design was improved by following the patients for a longer time period than reported by O’Hea and colleagues [Reference O’Hea, Abar, Bock, Chapman and Boudreaux10], extending the monitoring period to 12 weeks (84 days) rather than 7 days postdischarge. Smoking was tracked in a more precise way using ecological momentary assessment (EMA) six times/day for 28 days after the visit and a single end of day diary for the remaining 56 days, leading to daily tobacco use measures for 84 days. The feasibility of using EMA for tracking smoking in this opportunistic, heterogeneous sample was established in a pilot study, and the protocols were refined prior to study initiation [Reference Webber, O’Hea, Abar, Bock and Boudreaux20]. Using EMA allowed for more precise measurement of time to lapse after discharge to create survival curves.

2. Method

2.1. Procedure

From September 2011 through March 2013, adult cigarette smokers with symptoms of acute coronary syndrome who presented to one of four hospitals between two health systems were considered for enrollment. To be eligible, patients had to be 18 years or older; have smoked at least 100 cigarettes; have chest pain, chest pressure, shortness of breath, or syncope as primary presenting symptoms; undergo a cardiac evaluation consisting of, at minimum, an electrocardiogram and cardiac enzyme test (troponin); and be an active smoker, defined as smoking 1 or more cigarettes per day. Patients were ineligible if they presented with illicit drug use or alcohol abuse, had chest pain resulting from trauma, or were unable to be interviewed (e.g., severe medical illness, cognitive insufficiency, or insurmountable language barrier).

Participants were recruited during their ED or cardiac inpatient stay as close to the end of their medical visit as possible. This ensured a majority of the acute health event had already occurred prior to the interview, which was consistent with study interest in how perceptions and emotions changed over the course of the event. The range in actual illness severity in the patients who enrolled in the study was extensive, with some patients being discharged home from the ED with no evidence of cardiac disease (less serious) and others being diagnosed with an acute myocardial infarction requiring multivessel cardiac arterial bypass graft (CABG) surgery (very serious).

In addition to the baseline interview, participants were given a study cellphone and were expected to complete up to six random, interactive voice response (IVR) EMA calls per day, with each survey lasting one to two minutes, for 28 days, followed by a single end of day diary call for 56 additional days totaling 84 days of monitoring. Participants were trained on using the cellphone for EMA calls before discharge. A research assistant completed a structured follow-up telephone interview at 28 days, three months, and six months, though for the purposes of this paper, the focus will be entirely on the EMA data for the 3-month window.

Participants could keep the cellphone (or $40 if using their own cellphone) and had a free nationwide cell service for the duration of the study. They were compensated $0.25 per completed call for the EMA and $1.00 per completed end of day diary call in the last 56 days, for a total of up to $98.00. Withdrawn participants returned the cellphone but were paid for completed surveys. Additionally, participants received $15.00 compensation for completing research assistant telephone interviews, up to $45.00. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of the hospitals, and all patients signed informed consent.

2.2. Measures

At the time of enrollment, participants completed measures developed to assess a range of cognitive and affective constructs as described below. The psychometric properties of these scales are published by Boudreaux and colleagues [Reference Boudreaux, O'Hea and Moon21], and they are the same used by O’Hea and colleagues [Reference O’Hea, Abar, Bock, Chapman and Boudreaux10]. For two of these constructs, perceived illness severity and illness-related emotions, participants were asked to provide a rating for three specific time anchors: time anchor 1, when they first started experiencing symptoms; time anchor 2, when they first arrived at the hospital; and time anchor 3, currently, at the time of enrollment. The purpose of measuring items at each time anchor was to assess whether perceived illness severity and illness-related emotions fluctuated over the course of the healthcare encounter as hypothesized in previous papers [Reference Boudreaux, Bock and O’Hea6, Reference Boudreaux, Abar and O’Hea17, Reference Boudreaux, Moon, Baumann, Camargo, O’Hea and Ziedonis18]. For the entire health event to be chronicled, participants were enrolled as close to discharge as possible. Therefore, the third anchor rating, “right now,” typically occurred after most of the health event was over and within 24 hours of the patient’s discharge. The first two anchors, by necessity, were retrospectively rated, the limitations of which are described under limitations.

2.2.1. Actual Illness Severity

Enrolled participants’ medical diagnoses were highly heterogeneous in severity. A categorical disease severity rating, consistent with common clinical distinctions, was assigned to each participant. These categorical ratings were coded as follows: (0) treated in the ED and discharged, (1) admitted to an inpatient floor but received no intervention, and (2) admitted with an intervention, including percutaneous stent, balloon angioplasty, or CABG.

2.2.2. Perceived Illness Severity

Perceived illness severity was measured using three different time anchors, describe above. Participants’ responses to four questions that assessed perceived illness severity were rated on a five-point Likert-type scale: 1 = “strongly disagree,” 2 = “disagree,” 3 = “neither agree or disagree,” 4 = “agree,” and 5 = “strongly agree.” Sample items include “Something is seriously wrong with me,” and “I have a life-threatening illness.”

2.2.3. Illness-Related Negative Emotions

Illness-related negative emotions were measured using three different time anchors. Participants’ responses to eight items were rated on a five-point Likert-type scale: 1 = “not at all,” 2 = “a little,” 3 = “moderately,” 4 = “quite a bit,” and 5 = “extremely.” The emotions rated were anxiety, fear, frustration, nervousness, sadness, hopelessness, stress, and anger.

2.2.4. Smoking-Related Causal Attribution

Participants’ beliefs about the association between smoking and their current health problem were measured using four items rated on a five-point Likert-type scale: 1 = “strongly disagree,” 2 = “disagree,” 3 = “neither agree or disagree,” 4 = “agree,” and 5 = “strongly agree.” Sample items include “My current illness is due to a health problem caused by smoking,” and “Quitting smoking could improve my health.”

2.2.5. Self-Efficacy

Self-efficacy was assessed using a 0- to 10-point confidence ruler assessing confidence in quitting smoking within the next month, with 0 = not at all confident and 10 = 100% confident [Reference Abar, Baumann, Rosenbaum, Boyer, Ziedonis and Boudreaux22].

2.2.6. Nicotine Dependence

Participants’ nicotine addiction severity was measured using the Heaviness of Smoking Index [Reference Fagerstrom and Schneider23, Reference Heatherton, Kozlowski, Frecker and Fagerstrom24].

2.2.7. Intention to Quit

Using two different time anchors (time anchor 2 and 3), participants rated five questions that assessed intention to quit smoking using a five-point Likert-type scale: 1 = “strongly disagree,” 2 = “disagree,” 3 = “neither agree or disagree,” 4 = “agree,” and 5 = “strongly agree.” Example items include “I intend to quit smoking sometime within the next 30 days,” and “I have decided to quit smoking today.”

2.2.8. Tobacco Cessation Outcomes

Tobacco use was measured by EMA. At each assessment, participants rated whether or not he/she had smoked that day, and, if so, provided an estimate of the number of cigarettes smoked since awakening. The tobacco outcome data was transformed to hours to relapse, defined as the hours from discharge from the ED or inpatient unit to the time when the participant smoked his/her first cigarette. This variable was then transformed to form two categorical variables: (1) a dichotomous variable denoting sustained abstinence for the first 7 days after discharge (yes or no), which allowed comparison with extant literature; and (2) a 6-category ordinal variable based on the timing of the relapse: 1 (within 24 hours), 2 (between 1 and 7 days), 3 (between 8 and 14 days), 4 (between 15 and 28 days), 5 (between 29 and 56 days), and 6 (did not lapse during the 84 day window, i.e., sustained abstinence throughout the 3-month monitoring period).

2.2.9. Other Variables

We measured age, sex, race, ethnicity, and state of residence.

2.3. Data Analyses

Data analyses proceeded in several stages. First, the association between all of the bivariate predictors and 7-day abstinence status was calculated. Second, Pearson (for continuous variables) or Spearman (for ordinal variables) correlation analyses were conducted to examine the associations among the predictors, interactions (multiplicative) between causal attribution and perceived severity and illness-related negative emotions, and time to lapse expressed as the 6-point ordinal variable. Correlation analysis was performed using SAS 9.4 statistical software package [25].

Third, a path analysis was conducted to test the hypothesized model presented in Figure 1 that specifies relationships between all observed variables, interaction variables, and the 6-point ordinal outcome, using Mplus [Reference Muthén and Muthén26].

Figure 1: Hypothesized initial model of the relationships between the constructs of theory of planned behavior and quit intention and time to relapse.

Mediation effects were tested using the Sobel test, a commonly used method for testing the significance of the mediation effect [Reference Sobel27]. Standardized regression coefficients for all paths were estimated using maximum likelihood (ML) estimation. Missing data was handled using full information maximum likelihood (FIML). Goodness of model fit was assessed using absolute and comparative fit indices, including chi-square to degrees-of-freedom ratio (χ 2/df), root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), Bentler’s comparative fit index (CFI), and the Tucker Lewis index (TLI) [Reference Hooper, Coughlan and Mullen28]. Acceptable model fit is determined by an RMSEA less than 0.08 and values of CFI and TLI greater than 0.90 [Reference Hu and Bentler29].

3. Results

3.1. Descriptives

Of the 434 patients enrolled, 40 withdrew from the study or provided insufficient EMA compliance to determine the date and time of first lapse, leaving 394 participants for analysis (see Table 1). Participants’ ages ranged from 18 to 84 years old, with an average age of 53, and 200 (51%) were male. Most (354; 91%) were white, 35 (9.0%) were black, and 31 (8.0%) identified as Hispanic/Latino. Median compliance with the EMA was 69.9% (IQR: 46.1% and 85.3%). Figures 2 and 3 plot survival curves for the entire sample and actual severity subgroups.

Table 1: Individual characteristics of study sample (n = 394).

Figure 2: Kaplan-Meier survival curve of the entire sample.

Figure 3: Kaplan-Meier survival curve of subsamples based on actual illness severity.

Of those who lapsed, average time to lapse was 146.6 hours (SD = 283.0 hours), or roughly 6 days. Sustained abstinence 7 days postdischarge was achieved by 112/394 (28.4%). Using the 6-category lapse variable, 135 (34.3%) lapsed within 24 hours of discharge, 147 (37.3%) lapsed between one and seven days, 33 (8.4%) lapsed between seven and 14 days, 22 (5.6%) lapsed within 15 and 28 days, 16 (4.1%) lapsed between 29 and 56 days, 6 (1.5%) lapsed between 56 and 84 days, and 35 (8.9%) remained abstinent the entire 84 days.

3.2. Predicting 7-Day Abstinence

Table 1 shows the bivariate relations between the predictors and initial 7-day sustained abstinence. The strongest predictors were as follows: actual severity (χ 2 = 32.94, p < 0.001), self-efficacy (t = 8.34, p < 0.001), current intentions to quit (t = 7.77, p < 0.001), state of site (χ 2 = 7.70, p = 0.006), smoking-related causal attribution (t = 4.45, p < 0.001), and age (t = 3.52, p < 0.001).

3.3. Bivariate Correlations with Time to Relapse

The bivariate associations between the predictors, quit intention, and time to relapse expressed as the 6-category ordinal variable were examined using Pearson or Spearman correlation coefficients (see Table 2). Time to relapse was most strongly correlated with current quit intention (r = 0.34, p < 0.001), self-efficacy (r = 0.34, p < 0.001), actual severity (r = 0.30, p < 0.001), the interaction between smoking-related causal attribution and perceived severity (r = 0.20, p < 0.001), age (r = 0.20, p < 0.001), smoking-related causal attribution (r = 0.18, p < 0.001), highest value of perceived illness severity (r = 0.11, p < 0.05), and negative emotions at illness onset (r = −0.11, p < 0.05). Current quit intention was associated with the same variables as the time to relapse, with the addition of prior quit intentions (r = 0.51, p < 0.001), the interaction between causal attribution and illness-related negative emotions (r = 0.25, p < 0.001), average perceived severity (r = 0.21, p < 0.001), perceived severity at hospital presentation (r = 0.18, p < 0.001), and perceived severity at discharge (r = 0.10, p < 0.05).

Table 2: Correlation coefficients between perceived severity, negative emotions, causal attributions, social norms, heavy smoking index, intentions to quit smoking, and time to relapse (n = 394).

p < 0.05. p < 0.01. p < 0.001.

* Time to lapse expressed as 6 point ordinal variable.

3.4. Path Analyses of the Hypothesized Relationships

The initial hypothesized model is displayed in Figure 1. We hypothesized that actual severity would have a direct effect on perceived illness severity, illness-related negative emotions, and self-efficacy, which in turn would have an effect on current quit intention and time to relapse. Age, smoking-related causal attribution, and prior quit intention were hypothesized to have direct effect on current quit intention, which in turn would have an effect on time to relapse. The interactions of causal attribution × perceived severity and causal attribution × negative emotions were hypothesized to have a direct effect on current quit intention. Estimation of this initial model revealed an unacceptable model fit which suggested the need for modification of the initial model.

In modifying the initial path model, we included perceived illness severity, illness-related negative emotions, and self-efficacy as exogeneous variables and allowed these variables to covary with actual severity. Rather than all three time anchors for perceived illness severity and illness-related negative emotions, we used each individual’s highest rating from the three time anchors for each construct. The overall fit of the revised model (Figure 4) was excellent.

Figure 4: Revised structural model showing direct effects of actual and perceived severity, negative emotions, self-efficacy, casual attributions, and prior quit intention on current quit intention, which in turn has direct effect on time to lapse. Standardized path coefficients are shown. The highest values of perceived severity and negative emotions across times 1, 2, and 3 were used. p < 0.05, p < 0.01, and p < 0.001.

The chi-square/df ratio was 1.93, the RMSEA was 0.05, the SRMR was 0.03, the CFI was 0.99, and the TLI was 0.99. Illness-related negative emotions, self-efficacy, the interaction between causal attributions and perceived severity, and prior quit intention predicted a high level of current quit intention, which in turn predicted longer time to relapse. In addition, age, self-efficacy, and actual severity had a direct effect on time to relapse. The analysis revealed an R 2 value of 0.57 for current quit intention and of 0.20 for time to relapse.

The Sobel test of mediation effects indicated that current quit intention mediated the relationship between self-efficacy and time to relapse (z = 2.96, p = 0.003) and the relationship between prior quit intention and time to relapse (z = 2.88, p = 0.004). In addition, the Sobel test suggested that current quit intention mediated the relationship of the interaction between smoking-related causal attributions and perceived severity with time to relapse (z = 2.78, p = 0.005).

3.5. Perceived Illness Severity and Illness-Related Negative Emotions at Times 1, 2, and 3

Because measuring constructs across different time anchors is an innovative and emerging research area, a reduced model including only perceived severity and emotions at times 1, 2, and 3, current quit intention, and time to relapse was constructed (Figure 5).

Figure 5: Revised structural model showing direct effects of perceived severity and negative emotions at times 1, 2, and 3 on current quit intention, which in turn has direct effect on time to lapse. Standardized path coefficients are shown. Solid lines represent statistically significant relations; dotted lines represent nonstatistically significant relations. p < 0.05, p < 0.01, and p < 0.001.

The overall fit of this model was excellent. The chi-square/df ratio was 1.31 (p = 0.256), the RMSEA was 0.03, the SRMR was 0.02, the CFI was 0.98, and the TLI was 0.95. Perceived severity and illness-related negative emotions at time 2 predicted a high level of current quit intention, which in turn predicted longer time to relapse. Illness-related negative emotions at time 3 were negatively associated with current quit intention, and illness-related negative emotions at time 1 have a direct negative effect on time to relapse. The analysis revealed an R 2 value of 0.07 for current quit intention and of 0.13 for time to relapse, respectively.

Our structural equation model includes 9 manifest variables and two interaction terms. Based in the rule/recommendations of n = 10–20 observations per variable [Reference Hu and Bentler29, Reference Bentler and Chou30], our study with a sample size of 394 achieves adequate statistical power to detect true relationships in the data. This is also evidenced by our excellent model fit.

4. Discussion

Our study is the most comprehensive evaluation of the cognitive and affective factors associated with an acute cardiac health event and their relationship with smoking one week and three months after the event. Our findings confirmed many previously identified patterns and extended our understanding by evaluating new associations. Consistent with the literature, most of our sample of acute cardiac patients initiated smoking cessation upon discharge, but the vast majority relapsed back to smoking relatively soon after the visit. Specifically, 66% went at least 24 hours immediately after discharge without smoking, but only 28% went 7 days without smoking, and around 9% remained continuously abstinent for 84 days. It is challenging to directly compare these rates with rates among smokers in the general community, because comparable methods of identifying a given day when a person is not having a health event and following them for a similar period have not been used; however, large epidemiological studies suggest that only 3–5% of community-based smokers typically initiate smoking cessation in a given year [31Reference Hughes, Keely and Naud33], which is markedly less than those in our sample with a quit attempt. Our data confirm that an acute healthcare encounter can be a sentinel event that inspires changes in smoking, but it also confirms that relapsing back to smoking remained the pronounced norm.

Importantly, while relapsing back to smoking was the norm, this did not occur equally across our sample; instead, there were important predictors of short-term and longer abstinence. Greater actual illness severity was one of the most powerful predictors of abstinence, as shown in Figure 3. Those suffering a myocardial infarction who were admitted and received a cardiac intervention, like a percutaneous stent or CABG, were more likely to be abstinent than those who were less ill. Approximately 44% of such seriously ill patients achieved 7-day abstinence and 30% remained abstinent at 84 days. In contrast, those discharged home from the ED, i.e., the less severe group, revealed a very steep relapse curve, with only 4% achieving 7-day abstinence and 100% back to smoking by 28 days. Those who were admitted for observation but received no intervention fared somewhere in between. This pattern replicates the literature on illness severity and smoking cessation [Reference Bock, Becker, Niaura, Partridge, Fava and Trask34, Reference Rigotti, McKool and Shiffman35].

Quit intentions prior to discharge and self-efficacy both exhibited strong bivariate associations with 7-day abstinence (Table 1) and time to relapse (Table 2). In the final structural equation model (Figure 4), quit intentions continued to have significant direct associations with time to relapse, and self-efficacy had both a direct effect on time to relapse as well as a mediated effect through increased intentions to quit. These findings are consistent with the corpus of literature relating these two constructs to smoking-related behavior change [Reference Diemert, Bondy, Brown and Manske36Reference Zhu, Yang and Jiang39] and reinforces their fundamental importance for understanding both short-term and medium-term abstinence after an acute health event. Despite their preeminence in theory-based research and their conceptual incorporation into motivational interventions [Reference Heckman, Egleston and Hofmann40], empirical support for whether such interventions actually lead to increased intentions and self-efficacy and whether these changes, in turn, drive greater likelihood of abstinence are surprisingly rare [Reference Klemperer, Hughes, Callas and Solomon41]. Such mechanisms of action studies are crucial to testing whether quit intentions and self-efficacy can be influenced by intervention and are causal drivers behind improved abstinence.

Other illness-related variables demonstrated complex patterns with abstinence. Smoking-related causal attributions showed strong bivariate associations with intentions to quit, 7-day abstinence, and time to relapse. Generally speaking, those individuals who believed their illness was due to their smoking reported stronger intentions to quit and were more likely to be abstinent at 7 days and had longer time to relapse. Moreover, the interaction between causal attribution and the highest perceived illness severity was strongly associated with quit intentions and time to relapse at the bivariate level. In the final structural equation model, the main effect of smoking-related causal attribution no longer predicted quit intentions or time to relapse but the interaction term remained a predictor of quit intentions. Put differently, after all variables were entered into the model, it was not simply whether the individual thought his/her illness was related to smoking that drove quit intentions; instead, those that perceived they had a very serious illness and who thought this serious illness was due to smoking were most likely to intend to quit. This was similar to findings reported by O’Hea and colleagues [Reference O’Hea, Abar, Bock, Chapman and Boudreaux10] who found the interaction between smoking-related causal attribution and perceived severity at illness onset predicted 7-day abstinence after discharge in a similar sample of cardiac patients. Our results generally support other studies that have established a positive association between perceived severity and subsequent smoking cessation [Reference O’Hea, Abar, Bock, Chapman and Boudreaux10, Reference Boudreaux, Abar and O’Hea17, Reference Leventhal, Nerenz, Steele, Baum, Revernson and Singer42, Reference Scott and Lamparski43]. However, some studies have not found this association [Reference Boudreaux, Moon, Baumann, Camargo, O’Hea and Ziedonis18, Reference Schofield, Kerr and Tolson44]. Future studies should continue to pay close attention to perceived illness severity, including assessing these perceptions at different time anchors and evaluating the interaction between smoking-related causal attributions and perceived severity.

Illness-related negative emotional reactions, or what colloquially might be considered a “health scare,” were expected to promote abstinence. While previous studies have suggested negative affect may instigate relapses back to smoking among quitters [Reference Shiffman and Waters45], other studies of acute health events have suggested illness-related negative emotions might instigate increased motivation to quit, which in turn leads to increase probability of smoking cessation [Reference O’Hea, Abar, Bock, Chapman and Boudreaux10, Reference Boudreaux, Abar and O’Hea17, Reference Boudreaux, Moon, Baumann, Camargo, O’Hea and Ziedonis18]. However, the associations in our data were complex and, in some cases, contradictory. At the bivariate level, illness-related negative emotions did not demonstrate strong associations with 7-day abstinence or with time to lapse, with the exception of one negative association between initial illness-related negative emotions and time to lapse, which was the opposite of what was expected. The more detailed model examining ratings from each time anchor (Figure 5) demonstrates further complexity. Illness-related negative emotions at illness onset were negatively associated with time to relapse, and, similarly, illness-related negative emotions at discharge were negatively associated with current quit intentions, while, in contrast, negative emotions at hospital arrival were positively associated with quit intentions. When all variables were entered into the final full model, the highest value of illness-related negative emotions modestly predicted higher quit intentions. It is difficult to reconcile these seemingly contradictory associations with the extant literature, because most studies have not included measures of emotions related to the health event. O’Hea and colleagues [Reference O’Hea, Abar, Bock, Chapman and Boudreaux10] generally found that illness-related negative emotions were associated with stronger quit intentions, with illness-related negative emotions at hospital presentation also being associated with greater abstinence over the 7-day postdischarge period. Further study is required to fully understand the strength and direction of the association between illness-related negative emotions, quit intentions, and abstinence.

There were several limitations associated with this study. It is impossible to collect ratings of perceived illness severity, emotions, or quit intentions at the time when symptoms first began since these invariably occur hours to days prior to presenting to the emergency department. It is likewise not possible to collect ratings in real time immediately upon presentation, because the patient must be triaged and stabilized before research staff approach the patient. As a result, we must invariably rely on patient recall for these initial time anchors. This limitation is partially mitigated by the fact we approached patients during their healthcare encounter, minimizing the recall bias that might occur if we had obtained ratings days or weeks after they were discharged.

Patients who are admitted to an inpatient unit are forced to quit smoking. As a result, these participants would have already had up to several days of abstinence prior to discharge. This forced abstinence is not the same as voluntary cessation, so we framed the intentions to quit smoking within the context of their discharge home. It is possible, however, that this period of abstinence influenced their ratings. In addition, pinpointing exactly when the individual had their first cigarette was important, but compliance with EMA was variable. Some individuals were removed from the analysis because of lack of compliance, which may have reduced the sample’s representativeness. In addition, for some individuals, hours or days may have transpired between the actual lapse and their recording of the lapse. Because of the frequency of the assessments, it was unlikely this was longer than 24 hours. While it would be preferable to have precise measures immediately after the lapse, our data remains much more precise than most traditional studies that evaluate abstinence using time period separated by weeks or months. Finally, we did not collect biochemical verification of abstinence, because it was not feasible to do so. This is partially mitigated by the fact that the pressure to exaggerate abstinence is low in noninterventional studies like this one.

5. Conclusion

Our results confirmed the pattern of high lapse back to smoking after an acute health event and the preeminence of several key constructs, including actual illness severity, intentions to quit smoking, and smoking-related self-efficacy, in predicting abstinence for the 7 days after discharge, as well as time to lapse over the 84 days after discharge. Our results also revealed complex associations between illness-related cognitive factors and emotions, quit intentions, and tobacco use outcomes. The patterns reinforced the importance of taking measures of these variables at different time anchors and exploring their interactions.

Data Availability

Upon request submitted to the corresponding author (), the complete deidentified raw data, syntax file, and log files for analysis will be shared.

Disclosure

The sponsor had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare that there is no conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to acknowledge the valuable support lent by Brianna L. Haskins, M.S. in the copy editing and submission of this manuscript. The study was funded by a grant from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (R01DA023170).

References

US Department of Health and Human Services, The Health Consequences of Smoking—50 Years of Progress. A Report of the Surgeon General, US Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, Atlanta, GA, 2014.Google Scholar
Jamal, A., Phillips, E., Gentzke, A. S. et al., “Current cigarette smoking among Adults — United States, 2016,Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, vol. 67, no. 2, pp. 5359, 2018.10.15585/mmwr.mm6702a1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (SAMHSA), Behavioral Health Barometer: United States, 2015. HHS Publication No. SMA–16–Baro–2015, Center for Behavioral Health Statistics and Quality, Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, Rockville, MD, 2015.Google Scholar
Jha, P., Ramasundarahettige, C., Landsman, V. et al., “21st-century hazards of smoking and benefits of cessation in the United States,New England Journal of Medicine, vol. 368, no. 4, pp. 341350, 2013.10.1056/NEJMsa1211128CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernstein, S. L., Boudreaux, E. D., Cabral, L. et al., “Nicotine dependence, motivation to quit, and diagnosis among adult emergency department patients who smoke: a national survey,Nicotine & Tobacco Research, vol. 10, no. 8, pp. 12771282, 2008.10.1080/14622200802239272CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boudreaux, E. D., Bock, B., and O’Hea, E., “When an event sparks behavior change: an introduction to the sentinel event method of dynamic model building and its application to emergency medicine,Academic Emergency Medicine, vol. 19, no. 3, pp. 329335, 2012.10.1111/j.1553-2712.2012.01291.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glasgow, R. E., Hollis, J. F., Ary, D. V., and Boles, S. M., “Results of a year-long incentives-based worksite smoking-cessation program,Addictive Behaviors, vol. 18, no. 4, pp. 455464, 1993.10.1016/0306-4603(93)90063-FCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McBride, C. M., Emmons, K. M., and Lipkus, I. M., “Understanding the potential of teachable moments: the case of smoking cessation,Health Education Research, vol. 18, no. 2, pp. 156170, 2003.10.1093/her/18.2.156CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McBride, C. M., Blocklin, M., Lipkus, I. M., Klein, W. M. P., and Brandon, T. H., “Patient's lung cancer diagnosis as a cue for relatives' smoking cessation: evaluating the constructs of the teachable moment,Psychooncology, vol. 26, no. 1, pp. 8895, 2017.10.1002/pon.4011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Hea, E., Abar, B., Bock, B., Chapman, G., and Boudreaux, E. D., “Understanding smoking after acute illness: an application of the sentinel event method,Psychology & Health, vol. 30, no. 8, pp. 879896, 2015.10.1080/08870446.2014.997732CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rigotti, N. A., Munafo, M. R., and Stead, L. F., “Smoking cessation interventions for hospitalized smokers: a systematic review,Archives of Internal Medicine, vol. 168, no. 18, pp. 19501960, 2008.10.1001/archinte.168.18.1950CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tofler, G. H., May, R., Bartrop, R., Kirkness, A., Glinatsis, H., and de Burgh, S., “Acute coronary syndrome as a teachable moment for smoking cessation,Journal of Smoking Cessation, vol. 10, no. 1, pp. 511, 2015.10.1017/jsc.2013.35CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ajzen, I., “The theory of planned behavior,Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, vol. 50, no. 2, pp. 179211, 1991.10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-TCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothman, A. J., Baldwin, A. S., Hertel, A. W., and Fuglestad, P. T.Self-regulation and behavior change: disentangling behavioral initiation and behavioral maintenance,” in Handbook of Self-Regulation: Research, Theory, and Applications, Vohs, K. D., Baumeister, R. F., Eds., pp. 106122, Guilford Press, New York, NY, 2011.Google Scholar
Baird, D. L. and Eliasziw, M., “Disparity in perceived illness intrusiveness and illness severity between cardiac patients and their spouses,Journal of Cardiovascular Nursing, vol. 26, no. 6, pp. 481486, 2011.10.1097/JCN.0b013e3182092c11CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinstein, N. D., “Perceived probability, perceived severity, and health-protective behavior,Health Psychology, vol. 19, no. 1, pp. 6574, 2000.10.1037/0278-6133.19.1.65CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boudreaux, E. D., Abar, B., O’Hea, E. et al., “Cognitive and affective predictors of smoking after a sentinel health event,Psychology, Health & Medicine, vol. 19, no. 4, pp. 402409, 2014.10.1080/13548506.2013.832781CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boudreaux, E. D., Moon, S., Baumann, B. M., Camargo, C. A., O’Hea, E., and Ziedonis, D. M., “Intentions to quit smoking: causal attribution, perceived illness severity, and event-related fear during an acute health event,Annals of Behavioral Medicine, vol. 40, no. 3, pp. 350355, 2010.10.1007/s12160-010-9227-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bandura, A., Social Learning Theory, Prentice-Hall, Oxford, England, 1977.Google Scholar
Webber, C. J., O’Hea, E. C., Abar, B., Bock, B., and Boudreaux, E. D., “Ecological momentary assessment and first smoking cessation lapse after an acute cardiac event: a pilot study,Journal of Health Psychology, vol. 25, no. 8, pp. 10761081, 2020.10.1177/1359105317746731CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boudreaux, E. D., O'Hea, E., Moon, S. et al., “Measuring cognitive and affective constructs in the context of an acute health event,Psychology, Health & Medicine, vol. 18, no. 4, pp. 398411, 2013.10.1080/13548506.2012.720378CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abar, B., Baumann, B. M., Rosenbaum, C., Boyer, E., Ziedonis, D., and Boudreaux, E. D., “Profiles of importance, readiness and confidence in quitting tobacco use,Journal of Substance Use, vol. 18, no. 2, pp. 7581, 2013.10.3109/14659891.2011.606351CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fagerstrom, K. O. and Schneider, N. G., “Measuring nicotine dependence: a review of the Fagerstrom Tolerance Questionnaire,Journal of Behavioral Medicine, vol. 12, no. 2, pp. 159182, 1989.10.1007/BF00846549CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heatherton, T. F., Kozlowski, L. T., Frecker, R. C., and Fagerstrom, K. O., “Measuring the heaviness of smoking: using self-reported time to the first cigarette of the day and number of cigarettes smoked per day,British Journal of Addiction, vol. 84, no. 7, pp. 791800, 1989.10.1111/j.1360-0443.1989.tb03059.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
SAS Institute Inc., Base SAS 9.4 procedures guide, SAS Help Center, seventh edition, 2020, Base SAS 9.4 Procedures Guide, Seventh Edition.Google Scholar
Muthén, L. K. and Muthén, B. O., Mplus. The Comprehensive Modelling Program for Applied Researchers: User’s Guide, Muthén & Muthén, Los Angeles, CA, 2017.Google Scholar
Sobel, M. E., “Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural equation models,Sociological Methodology, vol. 13, pp. 290312, 1982.10.2307/270723CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hooper, D., Coughlan, J., and Mullen, M. R., “Structural equation modelling: Guidelines for determining model fit,Electronic Journal of Business Research Methods, vol. 6, no. 1, pp. 5360, 2007.Google Scholar
Hu, L. and Bentler, P. M., “Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives,Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, vol. 6, no. 1, pp. 155, 1999.10.1080/10705519909540118CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bentler, P. M. and Chou, C.-P., “Practical issues in structural modeling,Sociological Methods & Research, vol. 16, no. 1, pp. 78117, 1987.10.1177/0049124187016001004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC), “Smoking cessation during previous year among adults--United States, 1990 and 1991,Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, vol. 42, no. 26, pp. 504507, 1993.Google Scholar
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC), “Cigarette smoking among adults---United States, 2000,Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, vol. 51, no. 29, pp. 642645, 2002.Google Scholar
Hughes, J. R., Keely, J., and Naud, S., “Shape of the relapse curve and long-term abstinence among untreated smokers,Addiction, vol. 99, no. 1, pp. 2938, 2004.10.1111/j.1360-0443.2004.00540.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bock, B. C., Becker, B. M., Niaura, R. S., Partridge, R., Fava, J. L., and Trask, P., “Smoking cessation among patients in an emergency chest pain observation unit: outcomes of the Chest Pain Smoking Study (CPSS),Nicotine & Tobacco Research, vol. 10, no. 10, pp. 15231531, 2008.10.1080/14622200802326343CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rigotti, N. A., McKool, K. M., and Shiffman, S., “Predictors of smoking cessation after coronary artery bypass graft surgery. Results of a randomized trial with 5-year follow-up,Annals of Internal Medicine, vol. 120, no. 4, pp. 287293, 1994.10.7326/0003-4819-120-4-199402150-00005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diemert, L. M., Bondy, S. J., Brown, K. S., and Manske, S., “Young adult smoking cessation: predictors of quit attempts and abstinence,American Journal of Public Health, vol. 103, no. 3, pp. 449453, 2013.10.2105/AJPH.2012.300878CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tzilos, G. K., Strong, D. R., Abrantes, A. M., Ramsey, S. E., and Brown, R. A., “Quit intention as a predictor of quit attempts over time in adolescents with psychiatric disorders,The American Journal on Addictions, vol. 23, no. 1, pp. 8489, 2014.10.1111/j.1521-0391.2013.12067.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vangeli, E., Stapleton, J., Smit, E. S., Borland, R., and West, R., “Predictors of attempts to stop smoking and their success in adult general population samples: a systematic review,Addiction, vol. 106, no. 12, pp. 21102121, 2011.10.1111/j.1360-0443.2011.03565.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhu, W. H., Yang, L., Jiang, C. Q. et al., “Characteristics of smokers and predictors of quitting in a smoking cessation clinic in Guangzhou, China,Journal of Public Health, vol. 32, no. 2, pp. 267276, 2010.10.1093/pubmed/fdp107CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckman, C. J., Egleston, B. L., and Hofmann, M. T., “Efficacy of motivational interviewing for smoking cessation: a systematic review and meta-analysis,Tobacco Control, vol. 19, no. 5, pp. 410416, 2010.10.1136/tc.2009.033175CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klemperer, E. M., Hughes, J. R., Callas, P. W., and Solomon, L. J., “A mediation analysis of motivational, reduction, and usual care interventions for smokers who are not ready to quit,Nicotine & Tobacco Research, vol. 19, no. 8, pp. 916921, 2017.10.1093/ntr/ntx025CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leventhal, H., Nerenz, D., and Steele, D.Illness representations and coping with health threats,” in Handbook of Psychology and Health, Baum, A., Revernson, T., Singer, J., Eds., pp. 219252, Erlbaum, Hillsdale NJ, 1984.Google Scholar
Scott, R. R. and Lamparski, D., “Variables related to long-term smoking status following cardiac events,Addictive Behaviors, vol. 10, no. 3, pp. 257264, 1985.10.1016/0306-4603(85)90006-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schofield, I., Kerr, S., and Tolson, D., “An exploration of the smoking-related health beliefs of older people with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease,Journal of Clinical Nursing, vol. 16, no. 9, pp. 17261735, 2007.10.1111/j.1365-2702.2007.01701.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shiffman, S. and Waters, A. J., “Negative affect and smoking lapses: a prospective analysis,Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, vol. 72, no. 2, pp. 192201, 2004.10.1037/0022-006X.72.2.192CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1: Hypothesized initial model of the relationships between the constructs of theory of planned behavior and quit intention and time to relapse.

Figure 1

Table 1: Individual characteristics of study sample (n = 394).

Figure 2

Figure 2: Kaplan-Meier survival curve of the entire sample.

Figure 3

Figure 3: Kaplan-Meier survival curve of subsamples based on actual illness severity.

Figure 4

Table 2: Correlation coefficients between perceived severity, negative emotions, causal attributions, social norms, heavy smoking index, intentions to quit smoking, and time to relapse (n = 394).

Figure 5

Figure 4: Revised structural model showing direct effects of actual and perceived severity, negative emotions, self-efficacy, casual attributions, and prior quit intention on current quit intention, which in turn has direct effect on time to lapse. Standardized path coefficients are shown. The highest values of perceived severity and negative emotions across times 1, 2, and 3 were used. p < 0.05, p < 0.01, and p < 0.001.

Figure 6

Figure 5: Revised structural model showing direct effects of perceived severity and negative emotions at times 1, 2, and 3 on current quit intention, which in turn has direct effect on time to lapse. Standardized path coefficients are shown. Solid lines represent statistically significant relations; dotted lines represent nonstatistically significant relations. p < 0.05, p < 0.01, and p < 0.001.