Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-17T11:35:58.908Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

No association between breast-feeding and adult psychosis in two national birth cohorts

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

S. J. Leask*
Affiliation:
University of Nottingham Department of Psychiatry
D. J. Done
Affiliation:
Psychology Division, University of Hertfordshire
T. J. Crow
Affiliation:
Oxford University Department of Psychiatry
M. Richards
Affiliation:
National Survey of Health & Development, University College, London
P. B. Jones
Affiliation:
University of Nottingham Department of Psychiatry
*
Dr Stuart J. Leask, School of Community Health Sciences, University of Nottingham, Duncan Macmillan House, Porchester Road, Nottingham NG3 6AA
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

It has been proposed that breast-feeding might have a protective effect against the development of adult schizophrenia.

Aims

To test this hypothesis.

Method

Using prospective data from two UK national birth cohorts, the feeding histories of those who later developed schizophrenia were compared with the remaining population at risk. Analyses in each cohort were considered to be independent tests of the hypothesis.

Results

There were no differences in feeding histories. In the 1946 birth cohort (n=4447) 30 cases of DSM–III–R schizophrenia arose by age 43; 24.1% of cases v. 23.6% of controls were entirely bottle-fed; 17.3% v. 12.3% were breast-fed for under I month; 58.6% v. 64.1% were breast-fed beyond I month. In the 1958 cohort (n=18 856), 40 cases of CATEGO nuclear schizophrenia arose by age 28; 24.1% of cases v. 31.7% of controls were entirely bottle-fed; 27.6% v. 24.9% were breast-fed for under 1 month; 48.3% v. 43.4% were breast-fed beyond 1 month.

Conclusions

These findings provide no evidence of any effect of breast-feeding in protecting against the risk of later schizophrenia.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © 2000 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

Breast-feeding has been suggested as protective of cognitive development (Reference Lucas, Morley and ColeLucas et al, 1992; but see Reference Gale and MartynGale & Martyn, 1996; Reference Malloy and BerendesMalloy & Berendes, 1998). Schizophrenia is preceded by childhood cognitive impairments (Reference Jones, Rodgers and MurrayJones et al, 1994; Reference Crow, Done and SackerCrow et al, 1995), and McCreadie (Reference McCreadie1997) thought that breast-feeding might be a factor in the aetiology of schizophrenia. His figures seemed to show that those who were breast-fed as babies were two to three times less likely to develop schizophrenia in later life as those who were not, in a geographically defined population, using maternal recall of feeding histories in cases compared with their siblings, and published norms. Similar findings emerged from a case-control study by Peet et al (Reference Peet, Poole and Laugharne1997).

A plausible mechanism exists. Unlike cow's milk or most formula feeds, breastmilk contains large amounts of long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids, such as docosahexaenoic acid (DHA), an important constituent of neuronal membranes. Lack of DHA might have adverse effects on development (Reference Makrides, Neumann and SimmerMakrides et al, 1995; but see Reference Innis, Nelson and LwangaInnis et al, 1996), so that the lower postnatal DHA intake from formula feeds could explain the deviant cognitive and neurological development seen premorbidly in schizophrenia (Reference Done, Johnstone and FrithDone et al, 1991; Reference Dworkin, Cornblatt and FriedmannDworkin et al, 1993; Reference Jones, Rodgers and MurrayJones et al, 1994).

METHOD

We tested McCreadie's hypothesis that breast-feeding protects against later schizophrenia, using data gathered prospectively in two national birth cohorts.

Samples

The 1946 British National Survey of Health and Development (NSHD) originated as a survey of all births in mainland Britain during the week 3-9 March 1946 (n=13 687), to investigate the birth rate, which had been falling in the pre-war years, and to plan for the needs of maternity services in the National Health Service due to be introduced 2 years later (Reference WadsworthWadsworth, 1991). A random sample, stratified by social class, formed a cohort of 5362 individuals who have been followed to date: data have been collected on 20 occasions up to the age of 43, 11 times before the age of 16, and 9 times since.

The 1958 National Child Development Study (NCDS) began life as The Perinatal Mortality Survey (Reference ShepherdShepherd, 1985). It included 98% of births in mainland Britain during the week 3-9 March 1958. Five subsequent attempts have been made to trace and gather data on its members at age 7, 11, 16, 23 and 33 (NCDS 1-5).

Exposure: breast-feeding

In both cohorts, details on perinatal feeding were obtained by the health visitor in an interview with the mother. In the NSHD this took place when the child was 2 years old. Breast-feeding was classified as not attempted, or stopped in the 1st, 2nd, 3rd, 4th, 5th-7th, 8th-10th, or after the 10th month. In the NCDS the interview took place when the child was 7 years old, and breast-feeding was classified as not attempted, stopped in the first, or continued beyond the first month.

Data from the NSHD cohort were recoded to match the categories in the NCDS. The social class of the father at birth, and the gender of the child, were considered as potential confounders.

Ooutcome: ascertainment of subjects with schizophrenia

NSHD (Reference Jones, Rodgers and Murray Jones et al, 1994 )

By the age of 43 years 8 months, 81 of the members of the survey showed some evidence of a diagnosis of schizophrenia, use of regular neuroleptic medication, undefined severe mental illness or a psychiatric admission for unknown cause. Admissions were identified through a now discontinued national register, the Mental Health Enquiry, and reporting by survey members themselves. All available clinical material was scrutinised in order to determine whether these individuals met DSM-III-R criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 1987) for schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder.

NCDS (Reference Done, Johnstone and Frith Done et al, 1991 )

Patients discharged from psychiatric hospitals between 1974 and 1986 were identified from the Mental Health Enquiry, and where possible their case notes were obtained. These were used to fill in a Present State Examination (PSE) schedule, and from this input a CATEGO diagnosis (Reference Wing, Cooper and SartoriusWing et al, 1974). Cohort members were identified using NCDS data, and cohort members achieving a PSE diagnosis of ‘S+’ (‘narrow schizophrenia’) were included in the analysis.

Statistics

For the NSHD, a risk set of all the survey members alive in the UK at age 16 (n=4746) was drawn from the stratified sample of 5362, as used in previous studies of schizophrenia and affective disorder in this sample (Reference Jones, Rodgers and MurrayJones et al, 1994; Reference van Os, Jones and Lewisvan Os et al, 1997).

For the NCDS, a risk set of all the survey members, with the recorded data on childhood breast-feeding and social class of the father at birth (n=14 501), was drawn from the total cohort of 18 856, and used in the analysis, in the same way as in previous studies in this data-set (Reference Done, Johnstone and FrithDone et al, 1991; Reference Crow, Done and SackerCrow et al, 1995).

In both cohorts we used a nested case-control design. We calculated odds ratios (ORs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) for exposure to breast-feeding in cases compared with controls, adjusted and unadjusted for confounding by gender and social class. Hypothesis tests used the χ2 statistic. All analyses were performed using SPSS for Windows, version 8.0.

RESULTS

In the NSHD, information on perinatal feeding was available for 4703 (88%) subjects; 30 subjects (20 male) met DSM-III-R criteria for schizophrenia or schizo-affective disorder, and for 29 of these data had been recorded on perinatal feeding, gender and social class.

The total number of births from 3-9 March 1958 in mainland Britain was 17414, and information on perinatal feeding was available for 14 501 (83%) of these. Forty cohort members were identified who achieved a diagnosis of ‘narrow schizophrenia’ (PSE S+), of whom 29 (73%) (14 male) had data recorded on perinatal feeding, and 27 on gender and social class as well.

Breast-feeding differed with gender and social class in both cohorts (Table 1). Associations between these two variables and schizophrenia have already been documented (Reference Done, Johnstone and FrithDone et al, 1991; Reference Jones, Rodgers and MurrayJones et al, 1994), indicating that they were potential confounders of any association between breast-feeding and schizophrenia.

Table 1 Breast-feeding status of children born in 1946 (National Survey of Health and Development (NSHD) cohort) and 1958 (National Child Development Study (NCDS) cohort), by gender and social class

NSHD NCDS
Bottle-fed Breast-fed < 1 month Breast-fed > 1 month Bottle-fed Breast-fed < 1 month Breast-fed > 1 month
n (%) n (%) n (%) n (%) n (%) n (%)
Gender
Male 555 (52.6) 1142 (53.7) 632 (48.8) 2404 (52.3) 1842 (51.0) 3199 (50.8)
Female 500 (47.4) 984 (46.3) 663 (51.2) 2191 (47.7) 1769 (49.0) 3096 (49.2)
Social class
I-II 214 (20.5) 447 (21.2) 333 (25.8) 575 (13.9) 503 (15.0) 1254 (21.3)
III 529 (50.8) 1121 (53.2) 653 (50.6) 2498 (60.4) 2066 (61.5) 3565 (60.6)
IV-V 299 (28.7) 540 (25.6) 304 (23.6) 1066 (25.8) 790 (23.5) 1060 (18.0)

Numbers and percentages of cases and controls are shown in Table 2, stratified by cohort. The crude odds ratios showed no evidence of a protective effect of breast-feeding in either cohort; indeed, trends were in the other direction.

Table 2 Breast-feeding status of cases and controls

1946 NSHD Feeding Unadjusted odds ratio (95% CI) Adjusted odds ratio (95% CI)
Bottle-fed Breast-fed < 1 month Breast-fed > 1 month Breast-fed < 1 month Bread-fed > 1 month Breast-fed < 1 month Breast-fed > 1 month
n (%) n (%) n (%)
Cases (n=29) 7 (24.1) 5 (17.3) 17 (58.6) 1.36 (0.43-4.3) 0.89 (0.37-2.2) 1.35 (0.43-4.3) 0.86 (0.36-2.1)
Controls (n=4447) 1048 (23.6) 549 (12.3) 2850 (64.1)
Adjusted for gender and social class: χ2 (6, n=29)=5.8, P=0.44.
Complete data available for 4476 individuals.
1958 NCDS Feeding Unadjusted odds ratio (95% CI) Adjusted odds ratio (95% CI)
Bottle-fed Breast-fed < 1 month Breast-fed > 1 month Breast-fed < 1 month Bread-fed > 1 month Breast-fed < 1 month Breast-fed > 1 month
n (%) n (%) n (%)
Cases (n=29) 7 (24.1) 8 (27.6) 14 (48.3) 1.46 (0.53-4.0) 1.46 (0.59-3.6) 1.99 (0.65-6.1) 2.03 (0.73-5.7)
Controls (n=14 482) 4588 (31.7) 3603 (24.9) 6281 (43.4)
Adjusted for gender and social class: χ2 (5, n=27)=3.46, P=0.63.
Complete data available for 13 377 individuals.

DISCUSSION

No protective effect

We have demonstrated no significant effect of breast-feeding as a protection against adult schizophrenia: if anything, our results contradict those of McCreadie (Reference McCreadie1997). The two analyses share low statistical power, but we are impressed by the consistency of our findings from two independent birth cohorts.

Cohort v. case-control studies

Our study benefits from two clearly-defined populations, with breast-feeding data collected in the same way for cases and controls during the same epoch in terms of feeding styles and fashions. In both populations, the data on breast-feeding were collected from mothers when their children were some years old (2 years in the NSHD, 7 years in the NCDS). However, this is preferable to the situation in a conventional case-control study, which requires accurate recall not only over 20-30 years, but also with knowledge of the adult outcome; our data were gathered blind to this potential bias.

Comparing subjects' feeding histories from different epochs, as McCreadie was obliged to do, albeit at the same chronological age, is problematic. The differences in overall habits in the two cohorts (Table 1) provide evidence of a secular change. We used the definitions of schizophrenia in the two cohorts that were available to us. The similarity of the results from the two cohorts suggests that the difference in diagnosis is not relevant.

Methodological issues

Case ascertainment appears relatively unbiased in both cohorts. The total number of cases identified in each sample lie within the numbers which would be expected given their ages (Reference Done, Johnstone and FrithDone et al, 1991; Reference Jones, Rodgers and MurrayJones et al, 1994). Thus, we do not think that we have preferentially identified groups with ‘severe’ schizophrenia and biased our findings.

Breast-feeding data were missing for a higher proportion of cases than of controls in the 1958 sample. However, we doubt that there is any serious, systematic bias, because of the similarity of the overall result compared with that for the earlier cohort, where exposure data were more complete.

Differences in national feeding patterns have already been noted (Reference McCreadieMcCreadie, 1997), and might account for differences between studies; the majority of babies born in England in 1946 were initially breast-fed, even if this stopped within days, whereas data from Scotland in 1946 suggests that only half received any breast-milk at all. McCreadie compared his cases with national data derived from the NSHD before stratified sampling, whereas we used the stratified NSHD sample. This will have led to differences in the prevalence of breast-feeding but will not have biased our effects, because cases and controls came from the same population at risk, with equal baseline risk of inclusion. Another difference between our study and that of McCreadie is the fact that cases in both cohorts are effectively incident cases, whereas the Nithsdale (McCreadie's) sample is prevalent, biased towards chronic illness. Thus, while we can exclude a protective effect in terms of causation, the possibility remains of an effect on outcome. The positive associations are not without precedent (cf. Reference Torrey, Rawlings and YolkenTorrey et al, 1999), but lack significance in these data.

In summary, on the basis of an investigation in two national cohorts, the hypothesis that absence of breast-feeding contributes to the aetiology of schizophrenia is not supported. Significant findings in previous case-control studies are therefore likely to be Type I errors, due to methodological factors.

Clinical Implications and Limitations

CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS

  • Choice of feeding method should not be made out of concern over adverse psychiatric consequences.

  • The consistency of results over two epidemiological samples strengthens this conclusion.

  • Findings from case-control studies remain vulnerable to selection and recall bias.

LIMITATIONS

  • Differences between the two population samples were not examined in any detail.

  • Details of exposure (e.g. types of non-breast milk) were unavailable.

  • Small numbers of cases; possible type 2 error (lack of power to detect small effects).

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We gratefully acknowledge discussion with, and encouragement from, Professor Robin McCreadie, the support of Professor Mike Wadsworth and the NSHD team at University College, London, Dr Tony Gordon, Dr Peter Shepherd and the Social Statistics Research Unit at City University, London, and financial support from the Stanley Foundation.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

Funded by grants from the Stanley Foundation.

References

American Psychiatric Association (1987) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd edn, revised) (DSM–III–R). Washington, DC: APA.Google Scholar
Crow, T. J., Done, D. J. & Sacker, A. (1995) Childhood precursors of psychosis as clues to its evolutionary origins. European Archives of Psychiatry & Clinical Neuroscience, 245, 6169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Done, D. J., Johnstone, E. C., Frith, C. D., et al (1991) Complications of pregnancy and delivery in relation to psychosis in adult life: data from the British Perinatal Mortality Survey. British Medical Journal, 302, 15761580.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dworkin, R. H., Cornblatt, B. A., Friedmann, R., et al (1993) Childhood precursors of affective vs. social deficits in adolescents at risk for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 19, 563577.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gale, C. R. & Martyn, C. N. (1996) Breast feeding, dummy use, and adult intelligence. Lancet, 347, 1057; comments, 1072–1075.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Innis, S. M., Nelson, C. M., Lwanga, D., et al (1996) Feeding formula without arachidonic acid and docosahexaenoic acid has no effect on preferential looking acuity or recognition memory in healthy full-term infants at 9 months of age. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 64, 4046.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, P., Rodgers, B., Murray, R., et al (1994) Child developmental risk factors for schizophrenia in the 1946 birth cohort. Lancet, 344, 13981402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lucas, A., Morley, R., Cole, T. J., et al (1992) Breast milk and subsequent intelligence quotient in children born pre-term. Lancet, 339, 261264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Makrides, M., Neumann, M., Simmer, K., et al (1995) Are long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids essential nutrients in infancy? Lancet, 345, 14631468.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malloy, M. H. & Berendes, H. (1998) Does breast feeding influence intelligence quotients at 9 and 10 years of age? Early Human Development, 50, 209217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCreadie, R. G. (1997) The Nithsdale Schizophrenia Surveys 16. Breast-feeding and schizophrenia: preliminary results and hypotheses. British Journal of Psychiatry, 170, 334337.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peet, M., Poole, J. & Laugharne, J. (1997) Infant feeding and the development of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 24, 255256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shepherd, P. M. (1985) The National Child Development Study. An Introduction to the Background to the Study and the Methods of Data Collection (Working Paper No. 1). London: Social Statistics Research Unit, City University.Google Scholar
Torrey, E. F., Rawlings, R. & Yolken, R. H. (1999) The antecedents of psychoses: a case-control study of selected risk factors. Schizophrenia Research, 36, 57.Google Scholar
van Os, J., Jones, P. B., Lewis, G., et al (1997) Developmental precursors of affective illness in a general population birth cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54, 625631.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wadsworth, M. E. J. (1991) The Imprint of Time: Childhood, History and Adult Life. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Wing, J. K., Cooper, J. E. & Sartorius, N. (1974) The Measurement and Classification of Psychiatric Symptoms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Breast-feeding status of children born in 1946 (National Survey of Health and Development (NSHD) cohort) and 1958 (National Child Development Study (NCDS) cohort), by gender and social class

Figure 1

Table 2 Breast-feeding status of cases and controls

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.