Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T18:43:37.809Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Environmental determinants of healthy eating: in need of theory and evidence

Symposium on ‘Behavioural nutrition and energy balance in the young’

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 August 2008

Johannes Brug*
Affiliation:
EMGO-Institute, VU University Medical Center, Van der Boechorststraat 7, 1081 BT Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Stef P. Kremers
Affiliation:
Department of Health Education and Promotion, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
Frank van Lenthe
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health, Erasmus UniversityMedical Center, Rotterdam, The Netherlands
Kylie Ball
Affiliation:
Centre for Physical Activity and Nutrition Research, Deakin University, Melbourne, Australia
David Crawford
Affiliation:
Centre for Physical Activity and Nutrition Research, Deakin University, Melbourne, Australia
*
*Corresponding author: Professor Johannes Brug, fax +31 20 4448181, email J.Brug@vumc.nl
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

In order to promote healthful nutrition, insight is needed into the determinants of nutrition behaviours. Behavioural determinant research and behavioural nutrition interventions have focused mostly on individual-level motivational factors. It has been argued that the individual's socio-cultural and physical environments may be the main determinants of nutrition behaviours. However, the theoretical basis and empirical evidence for environmental determinants of nutrition behaviours are not strong. The present paper is a narrative review informed by a series of systematic reviews and recent original studies on associations between environmental factors and nutrition behaviours to provide an overview and discussion of the evidence for environmental correlates and predictors of nutrition behaviour. Although the number of studies on potential environmental determinants of nutrition behaviours has increased steeply over the last decades, they include only a few well-designed studies with validated measures and guided by sound theoretical frameworks. The preliminary evidence from the available systematic reviews indicates that socio-cultural environmental factors defining what is socially acceptable, desirable and appropriate to eat may be more important for healthful eating than physical environments that define the availability and accessibility of foods. It is concluded that there is a lack of well-designed studies on environmental determinants of healthful eating behaviours. Preliminary evidence indicates that social environmental factors may be more important than physical environmental factors for healthful eating. Better-designed studies are needed to further build evidence-based theory on environmental determinants to guide the development of interventions to promote healthful eating.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2008

Abbreviations:
ANGELO

analysis grid for environments linked to obesity

EnRG

environmental research model for weight gain prevention

Diet and nutrition are important for population health, but large majorities of populations in many countries do not comply with recommendations for healthy eating(Reference Ezzati, Lopez, Rodgers, Vander Hoorn and Murray1, Reference Brug and Oenema2). Healthful diet promotion is mostly focused on reducing saturated fat and energy intake and on promoting fruit, vegetable and fibre intake.

According to a basic model for planned promotion of population health (Fig. 1) the identification of major health problems and its nutritional risk factors are the first steps in the development of healthful nutrition promotion interventions. These first two steps define why effective nutrition interventions should be implemented and what nutrition behaviours should be targeted. However, to further develop such interventions, insight is needed into why individuals engage in such risk behaviours(Reference Brug, Oenema and Ferreira3).

Fig. 1. A model for planned health education and health promotion.

Until recently, studies on determinants of eating behaviours primarily focused on individual-level factors, such as taste preferences, nutrition knowledge, attitudes and intentions, and insight into these motivational determinants have informed health education interventions promoting more healthful eating habits(Reference Brug, Oenema and Ferreira3). More recently, it has been argued that the individual's environment may be of crucial importance in shaping and maintaining nutrition and eating habits(Reference Ball, Timperio and Crawford4). In most countries with established market economies palatable energy-rich foods that are high in fat, sugar and salt are available anywhere and anytime, while fresh fruit and vegetables and other foods rich in fibre may be much more difficult to obtain. This factor, combined with the tendency to eat when food is available and eat more when more is on offer(Reference Raynor and Wing5, Reference Fisher and Kral6), may lead to overeating of some foods and lack of consumption of others. Poor eating habits increase the risk of overweight and obesity, high serum cholesterol levels and hypertension, and consequently an increased risk for metabolic syndrome, diabetes, CVD and some cancers. Such environmental determinants of nutrition behaviour warrant a health protection approach, i.e. policy measures to realise environmental changes to healthful nutrition promotion rather than simply health education.

The present paper will review the theory and evidence relating to environmental determinants of health behaviours. The main focus will be on nutrition behaviour, but reference will be made also to evidence from reviews on environmental determinants of physical activity, given the similarities between behavioural nutrition and physical activity(Reference Baranowski7). The present paper will build on a series of reviews on these topics that have been conducted in the past few years as well as recent original studies in which the authors have been involved.

Introducing theories on environmental determinants

Most current health behaviour theories hypothesise that health behaviour is determined by an interrelated set of personal and environmental factors. For example, it has been proposed that individuals' health behaviours are determined by three broad interrelated categories of determinants: motivation; abilities; opportunities(Reference Rothschild8). The chances for individuals to engage in health behaviours are greatest when they are motivated to act healthily, have the abilities to engage in the healthy behaviour and when their social and physical environment offers the right opportunities for engaging in the healthy behaviour. Motivation and abilities can be considered to be personal determinants of health behaviour, while opportunities are based on environmental determinants. However, these categories are not independent. In an environment that offers easy opportunities for healthy behaviours, an individual may need less motivation and fewer skills. Social cognitive theory explicitly highlights this interaction between the individual and the environment in predicting health behaviour(Reference Bandura9).

Just as personal factors have been further subdivided into more-specific determinant constructs such as intentions, attitudes, outcome expectancies, subjective norms, self-efficacy and perceived behaviour control(Reference Armitage and Conner10), the environment can also be further defined by distinguishing various environmental factors.

Different classifications of possible environmental determinants of health behaviours have been proposed that show overlap and similarities. So-called ecological models of health behaviour arguably put most emphasis on the environmental factors in shaping health behaviours(Reference Sallis, Owen, Glanz, Rimer and Lewis11). Social environmental influences (inter-individual influences), physical environmental influences (influences within community settings) and macrosystem influences (influences at the societal level) have been identified(Reference Story, Neumark-Sztainer and French12). Social and cultural environmental influences have been differentiated, and a further distinction made between ultimate, distal and proximal factors(Reference Flay and Petraitis13). Based on the distinctions between categories of environment factors combined with the proximity of these environmental factors, a matrix can be designed with cells that represent different classes of environmental influences.

Such a matrix or grid structure is explicitly proposed in the analysis grid for environments linked to obesity (ANGELO) framework(Reference Swinburn, Egger and Raza14). This framework enables the identification of potential intervention settings and strategies. ANGELO was developed for the investigation of environments that promote excess energy intake and sedentary lifestyles (i.e. ‘obesogenic’ environments).

The ANGELO framework is a grid with two axes (Fig. 2). On the first axis two ‘sizes’ of environment (micro and macro) are distinguished. Micro-environments are defined as environmental settings in which groups of individuals meet and gather. Such settings are usually geographically distinct and provide opportunities for direct interaction between individuals and the environment. Examples of micro-environments are homes, schools, workplaces, supermarkets, bars and restaurants, other recreational facilities and also neighbourhoods.

Fig. 2. The analysis grid for environments linked to obesity (ANGELO) grid. (Based on Swinburn et al. (Reference Swinburn, Egger and Raza14).)

Macro-environments encompass the more anonymous infrastructure that may support or hinder energy-balance behaviours. Examples of macro-environments are national or international food and nutrition policies, and how food products are marketed, taxed and distributed.

On the second axis four ‘types’ of environments are distinguished: physical; economic; political; socio-cultural. The physical environment refers to the availability of opportunities for healthy and unhealthy choices, such as points-of-purchase for different foods, availability of healthy food options in school canteens or worksite cafeterias etc. The economic environment refers to the costs related to healthy and unhealthy eating. The political environment refers to the rules and regulations that may influence food choice and eating behaviour. School nutrition policies, e.g. on bans on soft-drink vending machines or on what treats can and cannot be brought to school, and family food rules are examples of political environmental factors. The socio-cultural environment refers to the social and cultural subjective and descriptive norms and other social influences such as social support for the adoption of health behaviour or social pressure to engage in unhealthy habits; socio-economic position can also be regarded as a socio-cultural factor, but is sometimes included in the economic environment.

The ANGELO framework is very useful for categorising different environmental influences on nutrition or other health behaviours. However, ANGELO is not a theoretical framework in the sense that it describes (hypothetical) mediating or moderating pathways highlighting how and under what conditions the different environmental factors may influence engagement in health behaviours, and ANGELO, as well as most other socio-ecological health behaviour models, lacks a description of how individual-level factors, i.e. motivation- and ability-related factors and environmental factors, interact in shaping health behaviours(Reference Brug, Oenema and Ferreira3).

Recently, a conceptual framework to further study the interplay between individual factors and environmental factors in predicting energy-balance behaviours has been proposed (Fig. 3)(Reference Kremers, De Bruijn, Visscher, van Mechelen, De Vries and Brug15). A dual-process model is outlined that can be used to gain insight into the causal mechanisms that underlie the relationship between environmental influences and behaviours that may influence the energy balance.

Fig. 3. The environmental research model for weight gain prevention. (From Kremers et al. (Reference Kremers, De Bruijn, Visscher, van Mechelen, De Vries and Brug15).)

This so-called environmental research model for weight gain prevention (EnRG) builds on recent reviews that have shown a lack of consistent results relating to the impact of environmental factors on nutrition behaviours and physical activity(Reference Brug, van Lenthe and Kremers16). A meta-analysis based on sixteen studies has confirmed the ambivalence in current empirical evidence relating to physical activity, but could not identify a single ‘crude’ environmental factor as being consistently related to physical activity(Reference Duncan, Spence and Mummery17). It is argued that the evidence relating to environmental determinants of energy-balance-related nutrition and physical activity behaviours collected to date has often been the result of non-theoretical approaches, which do not provide any knowledge on causal mediating relationships between environmental factors and health behaviours(Reference Kremers, De Bruijn, Visscher, van Mechelen, De Vries and Brug15). The fact that a majority of research in the field to date has been ‘opportunistic’, drawing on existing data rather than being based on purpose-designed studies, has also been highlighted(Reference Ball, Timperio and Crawford4). Using the application of knowledge mapping techniques, a panel of experts from diverse professional fields has highlighted a particular need for research to document the extent of environmental influences and how such environmental factors influence behaviour(Reference Booth, Sallis and Ritenbaugh18). Thus, an outline is given of how environments might affect behaviour, with the inclusion of mediating pathways and potential moderators of the relationships between environment, personal behavioural determinants and health behaviour(Reference Kremers, De Bruijn, Visscher, van Mechelen, De Vries and Brug15). The main ingredients of EnRG are derived from the ANGELO framework and one of the most popular socio-cognitive behavioural determinants models, the theory of planned behaviour(Reference Ajzen19).

EnRG is a dual-process model(Reference Petty and Cacioppo20) in the sense that it presumes that on the one hand behaviour can be the result of direct ‘automatic’ responses to environmental cues(Reference Bargh and Chartrand21), and on the other hand individuals may invest time and effort in systematically building beliefs and decisions to guide their health behavioural choices. It is argued that the application of the dual-process view in the study of personal and environmental determinants of health behaviours will help to gain insight into the circumstances under which health behaviour is a conscious action or an action that is more automatically induced by environmental cues, and such insights may inform intervention strategies to change health behaviour(Reference Kremers, De Bruijn, Visscher, van Mechelen, De Vries and Brug15). In the case of mediation by individual cognitions a health education approach may be appropriate, while in the case of automatic responses to environmental cues a health protection approach aimed at changing the environment may be more effective. This strategy implies an ‘either/or’ approach, but it is likely that both pathways will operate and therefore both intervention approaches are important.

EnRG thus hypothesises that the environmental factors such as those categorised in the ANGELO framework can have a direct influence on health behaviour by triggering more or less automatic responses to environmental cues. For example, in a home environment in which good-quality fruit is easily accessible in a fruit bowl, the sight and smell of the fruit may trigger consumption. On the other hand, environmental factors may influence health behaviour via personal motivation. For example, a restaurant environment that offers an impressive salad bar may induce more-positive beliefs related to eating a salad, which will increase intentions to eat a salad and make actual consumption more likely. The model further hypothesises that factors such as habit strength, personality characteristics and awareness of personal health behaviours may moderate the direct and indirect influences of environmental factors on health behaviours.

Reviews of the evidence

Most publications arguing that environmental factors drive unhealthy eating habits are position papers or narrative reviews, and therefore do not provide systematic evidence in favour of a causal association between environmental factors and unhealthy eating habits or its consequences. Furthermore, most of these position papers focus primarily on the presumed importance of the physical environment; the availability, accessibility and affordability of foods that contribute to unhealthful eating patterns.

Recently, six systematic reviews of the scientific literature up to 2005 on environmental correlates (297 papers were included in the reviews) and interventions (112 papers were included) for nutrition behaviours and physical activity for children, adolescents and adults in countries with established market economies were conducted and published as a series of papers in different scientific journals(Reference Giskes, Kamphuis, van Lenthe, Kremers, Droomers and Brug22Reference Kamphuis, Giskens, de Bruijn, Wendel-Vos, Brug and van Lenthe26). In this series of reviews the potential determinants and interventions were categorised according to the cells of the ANGELO grid.

First, these reviews reveal that research on potential environmental correlates of nutrition and physical activity is becoming more and more popular, particularly among children and adolescents, given the sharply increasing number of publications on this issue in the last decades(Reference Brug, van Assema, Lenderink, Glanz and Kok27). Furthermore, the reviews indicate that micro-size environmental factors have been studied much more often than macro-level factors. In the studies reviewed socio-cultural and physical environmental factors were included most often.

More importantly, the reviews of observational studies do not yet strongly support the recent claims that the environment has an important influence on nutrition and physical activity behaviours. The reviews reveal that the evidence is inconsistent for all the categories of environmental factors; less than half the studies have reported significant associations between environmental factors and the health behaviours investigated. Furthermore, these observational studies were mostly cross-sectional. Such studies require less time and other resources than more rigorous research, but even the presence of significant associations would not have provided evidence that environmental factors predict or cause unhealthful behaviours. Longitudinal and experimental studies are necessary to identify true environmental predictors and determinants of nutrition or physical activity behaviours.

Although strongly outnumbered by observational studies, the intervention studies that were reviewed provide more consistent evidence relating to relevant environmental factors, but the range of environmental factors studied was much smaller.

The combined findings from 297 observational studies and 112 intervention studies that were included in the different reviews lead to the following overall conclusions: (1) social support and modelling appear to be important for physical activity, in youth as well as in adulthood; (2) parents play a crucial role in influencing the health behaviour of their children in terms of modelling behaviour and also by using parenting practices and styles that encourage and support healthy habits in their offspring; (3) availability and accessibility of healthy and less-healthy foods are important for nutrition behaviours in youth and adulthood; schools and worksites offer good opportunities to improve the availability of healthful foods; (4) from the reviews of intervention studies it appears that increasing physical activity opportunities makes a difference, and schools and worksites offer suitable settings. In particular, increasing the period of time spent participating in physical education and physical activity in schools can make a difference for children and adolescents. Improving opportunities for walking can make a difference in adults; (5) children and adolescents from more deprived families are likely to have unhealthier diets and less physical activity, and lower household income is associated with less-healthy diets in adults(Reference Brug, van Lenthe and Kremers16).

The number of sound evidence-based conclusions about important environmental factors that could be drawn from reviewing the >400 original studies is not that impressive. The relatively-weak evidence that has been found thus far may not be interpreted as the absence of a relationship between ‘the environment’ and nutrition or physical activity behaviour. Despite the large number of studies, there is still a lack of high-quality studies and study replications, and many potentially-relevant environmental factors have not been studied. The available research has focused on only part of the environment, in particular micro-level factors in the socio-cultural environment and the physical environment. These factors are typically home-environmental social factors and school–physical environmental factors (parental influences and school availability) for children and adolescents, and social support and home and worksite availability and accessibility factors for adults, with few studies on neighbourhood environmental factors. Studies on macro-size environmental factors are almost completely absent.

Furthermore, most studies have applied weak study designs and non-validated measurement instruments. For example, most of the available observational studies have used cross-sectional designs, thus providing evidence for associations, but not for prediction or causation. Most studies have only presented bivariate associations between a presumed correlate and the behaviour; few studies have used multivariate analyses, adjusting for other potential personal or environmental correlates of nutrition or physical activity behaviours. Most observational and intervention studies have not examined the differences in environmental correlates for distinct subgroups based on the proposed moderators in EnRG, such as gender, socio-economic status, ethnicity and habit strength. Many intervention studies have not included a control group. Only some more recent studies have used multi-level analyses to take into account the fact that potential environmental correlates are often studied in non-independent samples, such as individuals clustered within neighbourhoods, schools or school classes(Reference Brug, van Lenthe and Kremers16). Exposure to environmental factors and engagement in nutrition and physical activity behaviours have often been assessed with non-validated self-report measures.

Important issues and new insights from recent studies

The series of reviews has led to the identification of five important issues that require further exploration and investigation:

  1. 1. more insight is needed in the interplay between individual level and environmental factors in prediction and determination of health behaviour;

  2. 2. socio-cultural environments may be more important than physical environmental factors;

  3. 3. differences between objective and self-report assessments of the environment;

  4. 4. the use of observational studies or intervention studies to explore environmental determinants;

  5. 5. the specificity of behaviour and environments.

These issues will be discussed briefly and recent studies in which the authors have been involved that provide some preliminary evidence to help to resolve these issues will be reported.

Mediation and moderation between motivation, ability and opportunity

In most of the studies that were reviewed only direct associations between environments and nutrition behaviours were considered, and other categories of potential behavioural determinants such as motivational factors were not included. Some recent studies that have included both environmental factors and motivational factors have shown that motivational factors are more strongly associated with self-report behaviours than environmental factors, and that motivational factors mediate the environmental impact on dietary behaviour and physical activity(Reference de Bruijn, Kremers, de Vet, de Nooijer, van Mechelen and Brug28Reference van der Horst, Kremers, Ferreira, Singh and Brug32). The mediated route from environment to behaviour holds in various instances, individuals and behaviours studied. However, the cognitively-mediated route does not always take sufficient account of variations in behaviour. Since unmediated environmental effects are postulated to be important explanatory mechanisms in the field of dietary behaviour and physical activity(Reference Kremers, De Bruijn, Visscher, van Mechelen, De Vries and Brug15), future studies need to be aimed at the identification of the behaviours, individuals and circumstances under which environmental influences are or are not mediated by motivational factors.

The impact of parenting practices, i.e. a social environmental factor, on adolescent soft-drink consumption has been investigated in a study that also explored the potential moderating role of adolescent personality in relation to the parenting practices–adolescent soft drink consumption relationship(Reference de Bruijn, Kremers, de Vries, van Mechelen and Brug33). The results show that adolescents who perceive their parents to have stricter house rules on soft-drink consumption drink less soft drink. Importantly, the personality dimension ‘agreeableness’ moderates this relationship, with adolescents with medium levels of ‘agreeableness’ most likely to obey parental house rules.

The potentially moderating role of general parenting dimensions (i.e. strictness and involvement) on the association between parenting practices and adolescent sugar-sweetened beverage consumption has been further investigated(Reference van der Horst, Kremers, Ferreira, Singh and Brug32). Consistent with published theory(Reference Darling and Steinberg34), the impact of content-specific parenting practices on consumption behaviour was found to be stronger among adolescents who perceive their parents as being both strict and involved. Specific restrictive practices relating to sugar-sweetened beverages have less impact on the child's behaviour for those parents who are generally less strict or involved with their child.

Other studies have focused on the moderating role of habit in the motivational control of dietary intake and physical activity. Habits are considered to be more or less automatic behavioural responses to environmental cues, and when habit strength for a certain dietary behaviour or physical activity increases, this behaviour may be less guided by conscious intentions(Reference Verplanken and S35). Multiple studies have confirmed this notion. For example, the influence of intention on fruit consumption has been shown to be weak and non-significant for those individuals who have a strong habit towards fruit consumption(Reference de Bruijn, Kremers, de Vet, de Nooijer, van Mechelen and Brug28). In contrast, for those individuals with a low or medium habit strength towards fruit consumption, intention has been found to be a significant predictor of fruit consumption. Similar results have been found for adolescent sedentary behaviour(Reference Kremers and Brug36) and child physical activity(Reference Kremers, Dijkman, de Meij, Jurg and Brug37).

Social or physical environment?

Most position papers on the presumed importance of the ‘obesogenic’ environment explicitly or implicitly highlight the importance of the physical environment, referring to the overabundant availability and accessibility of foods that contribute to overeating, and the presumed poorer access to healthy foods(Reference Brug, van Lenthe and Kremers16). Support for a role of the physical environment comes from the USA. An increase in the number of supermarkets (with their larger variety of healthy foods) has been shown to be associated with an increase in fruit and vegetable consumption, and with a lower prevalence of overweight and obesity(Reference Morland, Diez Roux and Wing38, Reference Morland, Wing and Diez Roux39). In other studies, however, such as the Brisbane Food Study conducted in Australia, the ‘objective’ availability of recommended foods has not been found to be related to actual food-purchasing behaviour(Reference Giskes, Van Lenthe, Brug, Mackenbach and Turrell40). Similarly, the Socio-economic Status and Activity in Women Study in Australia has found that the density of supermarkets and fruit and vegetable stores in local neighbourhoods in Melbourne is not strongly related to fruit or vegetable consumption amongst women living in those neighbourhoods(Reference Ball, Timperio, Salmon, Giles-Corti, Roberts and Crawford41).

The potentially weak association between the availability of healthy food and its consumption can be illustrated by neighbourhood studies. While neighbourhood inequalities in food availability have been reported, evidence on the directions of effects is equivocal. Some studies have shown that there are fewer healthy choices available in stores in deprived areas compared with less-deprived areas(Reference Baker, Schootman and Barnidge42, Reference Horowitz, Colson, Hebert and Lancaster43) (K Ball, A Timperio and D Crawford, unpublished results), but others have shown few differences in healthy food availability between deprived and less-deprived neighbourhoods(Reference Apparicio, Cloutier and Shearmur44, Reference Winkler, Turrell and Patterson45) or differences favouring more-deprived rather than less-deprived neighbourhoods(Reference Cummins and Macintyre46, Reference Cummins and Macintyre47). Lack of associations between the availability of healthy foods may thus be attributed to lack of consistent variation in ‘availability’ between (groups of) individuals. The majority of individuals in many Western countries, including those with less-healthy diets, may have sufficient access to healthy foods. It cannot be excluded that there is a larger spatial segregation in availability of healthy foods in the USA that may have driven the associations that have been reported(Reference McIntyre48). This possibility suggests that other factors may determine diet. Indeed, the series of reviews point out that the evidence for important socio-cultural correlates of nutrition behaviours is more convincing than that for physical environmental factors.

Recent studies conducted in The Netherlands further support this viewpoint. Associations between a wide range of potential physical and socio-cultural environmental factors and consumption of fruits and vegetables have been examined(Reference Wind, De Bourdeaudhuij, te Velde, Sandvik, Due, Klepp and Brug49, Reference de Bourdeaudhuij, te Velde and Brug50) and the results indicate that family socio-cultural factors such as parental encouragement and modelling are stronger correlates of intakes than availability of fruits and vegetables. This finding is also supported by a systematic review of the literature(Reference Rasmussen, Krolner, Klepp, Lytle, Brug, Bere and Due51).

In relation to parenting as a socio-cultural environmental factor, a distinction can be made between general parenting styles and content-specific acts of parenting (i.e. ‘parenting practices’). Through the use of parenting practices, parents have been shown to have a large impact on their child's dietary behaviours. For example, extensive previous research has shown that there are several child feeding practices (e.g. encouraging children to eat beyond satiety, using food as a reward) that lead to an increased risk of childhood overweight, mediated via children's eating behaviour. Two recent Dutch studies among adolescents have shown that more-restrictive parenting practices are associated with lower consumption of sugar-sweetened beverages(Reference van der Horst, Kremers, Ferreira, Singh and Brug32, Reference de Bruijn, Kremers, de Vries, van Mechelen and Brug33). In relation to general parenting styles, it has been found that fruit consumption is higher among children who describe their parents as being authoritative (i.e. high levels of parental control or ‘strictness’ and high levels of parental warmth or ‘involvement’) compared with children who have been raised with another parenting style (i.e. indulgent, authoritarian or neglectful)(Reference Kremers, Brug, de Vries and Engels52).

It is important to note, however, that while socio-cultural forces are often mentioned in the literature as an important determinant of eating behaviours, the definition and range of socio-cultural influences are typically not well -elucidated and there is little empirical research on many potentially-important socio-cultural factors. It has been argued that further research is required to elucidate the range of socio-cultural factors that might impact on eating behaviours(Reference Ball, Crawford, Crawford and Jeffery53). It has been suggested that future work should separate socio-economic status into specific components such as education, occupation and income in order to provide further insight into the specific aspects of socio-economic status that are most important. While the majority of empirical studies to date have focused on socio-economic status, the fact that socio-economic status is certainly not the only important socio-cultural influence on eating has been highlighted(Reference Ball, Crawford, Crawford and Jeffery53). Additional research is required to examine the nature of associations of social roles and relationships, social institutions, social pressure and norms and more macro-level cultural factors. In particular, there is a need for studies of the pathways by which socio-cultural factors influence eating behaviours.

Objective or subjective environments?

There is a lack of validated measurement instruments to assess environmental factors, especially physical environments. From a methodological point of view, objective measures are usually valued more highly than subjective self-report measures, because the latter are subject to social desirability bias, same source bias (where respondents judge both the perception of the environment and their behaviour) or self-justification. Recent studies have shown poor agreement between measures of access to physical activity facilities obtained by self-report and by objective assessment (e.g. via geographic information systems or audits). In such a study in women a mismatch between perceived and objectively-assessed environments was found to be more common amongst younger and older women, those of low income, those with low self-efficacy for physical activity, those who were less active, and those who reported using fewer facilities(Reference Ball, Jeffery, Crawford, Roberts, Salmon and Timperio54). In a second study similar low agreement between perceived and objectively-assessed environments was found amongst adolescents (R Prins, A Oenema, K van der Horst and J Brug, unpublished results).

An exclusive focus on objective measures of physical environments neglects that individuals observe and review their environments, and this perception of the environment is likely to be more important for behavioural choices than the ‘objective environment’. For example, a US study that compared boundary maps drawn by residents and census-defined block groups has found differences in both the areas mapped and social indicators, as well as inconsistencies between resident-drawn maps(Reference Coulton, Korbin, Chan and Su55). Australian research with children has also shown that perceptions of the neighbourhood environment are highly individual(Reference Hume, Salmon and Ball56, Reference Veitch, Ball, Robinson and Salmon57).

The series of reviews discussed earlier indicate that associations between environments and behaviour are stronger when subjective self-report measures of environments are used, and recent research also indicates that perceptions of availability and price of healthful foods are more strongly associated with food choice than with objective availability and price data(Reference Giskes, Van Lenthe, Brug, Mackenbach and Turrell40).

Perceptions of the availability of healthy and less-healthy foods have been shown to be associated with eating such foods, rather than more objective assessments of availability based on audits (K van der Horst, A Oenema and J Brug, unpublished results). The perception of the environment has also been shown to be more strongly correlated with physical activities among Dutch adolescents than with measurements of the environment based on geographic information systems (R Prins, A Oenema, K van der Horst and J Brug, unpublished results).

This finding implies that it may be important to effectively intervene on perceptions of the environment, i.e. by making individuals more aware of the opportunities in their environments. In order to achieve such awareness, it is necessary to have a better insight into what influences an individual's perception of their environment. A recently-conducted study of the determinants of some perceptions of the environment (safety and area attractiveness) has shown that objective characteristics of the environment contribute substantially to perceptions of the environment, but that individual factors (such as self-perceived health and depression) additionally contribute to perceived neighbourhood environments (C Kamphuis, F van Lenthe, K Giskes and J Mackenbach, unpublished results).

Observational studies and intervention studies

In the majority of the studies that were included in the reviews of observational studies cross-sectional designs were used and thus associations were studied; however, associations do not prove causation.

It has been argued that intervention studies are now needed to identify environmental pathways to encourage healthful nutrition and physical activity behaviours(Reference Brug, van Lenthe and Kremers16, Reference Robinson and Sirard58). Such studies should use experimental and quasi-experimental research, including community trials and natural experiments, to test the effects of environmental change. Although strongly outnumbered by observational studies, the intervention studies that were included in the series of reviews have provided more consistent evidence in relation to relevant environmental factors.

In particular, in recent years school-based interventions to promote more healthful nutrition and/or physical activity behaviours among children and adolescents have been conducted using a more integrated health education and health protection approach, in which changes in the school or neighbourhood environments were an integral part of the intervention approach. Examples such as the Dutch Obesity Intervention in Teenagers Study and the European Pro Children Study indicate that programmes that include changes in the food and physical activity in school environments can lead to meaningful changes in nutrition and physical activity behaviours, and may contribute to better body compositions among children and adolescents(Reference te Velde, Twisk and Brug59, Reference Singh, Paw, Brug and van Mechelen60).

Many other interventions have successfully targeted the school environment in order to change dietary habits of children and adolescents in a more healthy direction. The most success intervention has been the improvement of food preparation in school cafeterias, such as decreasing the fat content of meals(Reference De Bruijn, Kremers, Wendel-Vos, Van Lenthe, Brug, Brug and van Lenthe61). Decreasing the price of healthy foods such as fruits and low-fat snacks, either in cafeterias or vending machines, is likely to increase sales and probably consumption of these products.

Specificity of behaviour and environments

An important difference between observational studies and intervention studies that may explain the discrepancy in the consistency of results in both types of studies is the extent of specificity, particularly, but not exclusively, in outcome variables. Intervention studies in which, for example, prompts direct individuals to use the stairs, will measure (the change in) stair climbing as the outcome and not overall physical activity. However, observational studies in which environmental determinants are measured in more detail have often used an overall behavioural outcome variable when a specific outcome variable would have been much more appropriate. For example, the quality of green space can only be expected to be associated with those specific activities that can be facilitated by the availability of green space, and not with total physical activity(Reference van Lenthe, Kremers and Brug62). Future studies on environmental determinants of behaviour should therefore recognise that ‘the angels are in the detail’(Reference Sallis63).

Recently, it has been found that the contribution of environmental characteristics to specific outcomes may differ between subgroups; physical and social neighbourhood factors have shown independent associations with participating in sports v. not participating in sports. However, no neighbourhood factors were significantly associated with meeting recommended sports activity levels v. not meeting these levels. It would seem that the importance of the environmental determinants for sports participation is higher for the most inactive individuals (C Kamphuis, F van Lenthe, K Giskes and J Mackenbach, unpublished results).

If progress is to be made in exploring the association between the environment and physical activity, a fundamental condition is linking specific environmental determinants to specific aspects of physical activity in well-defined populations.

Conclusions

Promoting healthy nutrition behaviours still relies mostly on health education techniques that try to motivate individuals to adopt more healthy lifestyles. Research indicates, however, that large majorities of populations are already motivated to prevent unnecessary weight gain, eat healthfully and to be physically active(Reference Wammes, French and Brug64), and that nutrition education often has minor and short-lived effects at best. It has been argued that this outcome is the result of the so-called obesogenic environment preventing individuals from acting on their positive intentions. This obesogenic environment is characterised by high availability and accessibility of palatable energy-dense foods, strong subjective and descriptive norms and social pressure to eat such foods, as well as abundant opportunities to minimise participation in physical activity at work, for transportation or during leisure time. The most-recent systematic reviews do not yet provide strong evidence for the crucial importance of the environment in causing overeating and lack of physical activity, because of the scarcity of well-designed studies at the time of the reviews. However, the most robust studies, i.e. longitudinal studies and ‘natural’ experiments looking at associations between changes in the environment and changes in health behaviours and more recent intervention studies, do confirm that environmental factors are important. Social environments may be more important than physical environments, and the impact of the environment may be mediated and moderated by different individual-level determinants of behaviour.

As the obesogenic environment is presumed to be important, it has therefore been argued that interventions to promote more healthful nutrition and physical activity practices should adopt a health protection paradigm instead of, or in addition to, health education. A health protection approach implies a focus on environmental changes that help to ‘protect’ the population against unhealthy nutrition and lack of physical activity, i.e. environmental changes that would make healthy nutrition and sufficient physical activity easier, more likely or even unavoidable. From a public health perspective such ‘upstream’ approaches are particularly appealing because of their potential to impact on large segments of the population. This health promotion and protection approach, i.e. creating environmental opportunities for healthful behaviours but also protecting the population against opportunities for unhealthful behaviours, has been successful in the major achievements in the history of public health, such as the reduction and sometimes eradication of infectious diseases, promotion of road safety and reducing the prevalence of smoking. In particular, recently conducted school-based interventions to promote more healthful nutrition and/or physical environments indicate that such more-health protection-orientated approaches can lead to changes in health behaviours and body composition(Reference te Velde, Twisk and Brug59, Reference Singh, Paw, Brug and van Mechelen60).

Acknowledgements

This research is being carried out with financial support from the EC, SP5A-CT-2006-044128 Health-promotion through Obesity Prevention across Europe (HOPE): an integrated analysis to support European health policy. The paper does not necessarily reflect the EC's views and in no way anticipates the future policy of the EC in this area. K.B. is supported by an Australian National Health and Medical Research Council Senior Research Fellowship (ID 479513). D.C. is supported by a Victoria Health Promotion Foundation Senior Research Fellowship.

There is no conflict of interest.

Contribution of the authors: J.B. planned the paper, prepared a first draft and compiled the final manuscript. All other authors provided comments on the draft and contributed specific paragraphs or subparagraphs to the manuscript.

References

1.Ezzati, M, Lopez, AD, Rodgers, A, Vander Hoorn, S & Murray, CJ (2002) Selected major risk factors and global and regional burden of disease. Lancet 360, 13471360.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2.Brug, J & Oenema, A (2006) Healthful nutrition promotion in Europe: goals, target populations, and strategies. Patient Educ Couns 63, 255257.Google ScholarPubMed
3.Brug, J, Oenema, A & Ferreira, I (2005) Theory, evidence and intervention mapping to improve behavioral nutrition and physical activity interventions. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 2, 2.Google Scholar
4.Ball, K, Timperio, AF & Crawford, DA (2006) Understanding environmental influences on nutrition and physical activity behaviors: where should we look and what should we count? Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 3, 33.Google Scholar
5.Raynor, HA & Wing, RR (2007) Package unit size and amount of food: do both influence intake? Obesity 15, 23112319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6.Fisher, JO & Kral, TV (2007) Super-size me: Portion size effects on young children's eating. Physiol Behav; Epublication 22 November 2007.Google Scholar
7.Baranowski, T (2004) Why combine nutrition and physical activity in the same international research society? Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 1, 2.Google Scholar
8.Rothschild, ML (1999) Carrots, sticks, and promises: a conceptual framework for the management of public health and the social issue behaviors. J Mark 63, 2437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
9.Bandura, A (1986) Social Foundations of Thought and Action. A Social Cognitive Theory. New Jersey: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
10.Armitage, CJ & Conner, M (2000) Social cognition models and health behaviour: A structured review. Psychol Health 15, 173189.Google Scholar
11.Sallis, JF & Owen, N (2003) Ecological models of health behavior. In Health Behavior and Health Education, 3rd ed., pp. 462485 [Glanz, K, Rimer, BK and Lewis, FM editors]. San Francisco, CA: Jossey Bass.Google Scholar
12.Story, M, Neumark-Sztainer, D & French, S (2002) Individual and environmental influences on adolescent eating behaviors. J Am Diet Assoc 102, S40S51.Google Scholar
13.Flay, BR & Petraitis, J (1994) The theory of triadic influence: a new theory of health behavior with implications for preventive interventions. Adv Med Sociol 4, 419.Google Scholar
14.Swinburn, B, Egger, G & Raza, F (1999) Dissecting obesogenic environments: the development and application of a framework for identifying and prioritizing environmental interventions for obesity. Prev Med 29, 563570.Google Scholar
15.Kremers, SPJ, De Bruijn, GJ, Visscher, TLS, van Mechelen, W, De Vries, NK & Brug, J (2006) Environmental influences on energy balance-related behaviors: A dual-process view. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 3, 9.Google Scholar
16.Brug, J, van Lenthe, FJ & Kremers, SPJ (2006) Revisiting Lewin: how to gain insight in environmental correlates of obesogenic behaviors. Am J Prev Med 31, 525529.Google Scholar
17.Duncan, MJ, Spence, JC & Mummery, WK (2005) Perceived environment and physical activity: a meta-analysis of selected environmental characteristics. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 2, 11.Google Scholar
18.Booth, SL, Sallis, JF, Ritenbaugh, C et al. (2001) Environmental and societal factors affect food choice and physical activity: rationale, influences, and leverage points. Nutr Rev 59, 2164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19.Ajzen, I (1991) The theory of planned behaviour. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 50, 179211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
20.Petty, RE & Cacioppo, JT (1986) Communication and Persuasion, Central and Peripheral Routes to Attitude Change. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
21.Bargh, JA & Chartrand, TL (1999) The unbearable automaticity of being. Am Psychol 54, 462479.Google Scholar
22.Giskes, K, Kamphuis, CB, van Lenthe, FJ, Kremers, SPJ, Droomers, M & Brug, J (2007) A systematic review of associations between environmental factors, energy and fat intakes among adults: is there evidence for environments that encourage obesogenic dietary intakes? Public Health Nutr 10, 10051017.Google Scholar
23.Ferreira, I, van der Horst, K, Wendel-Vos, W, Kremers, SPJ, van Lenthe, FJ & Brug, J (2007) Environmental correlates of physical activity in youth – a review and update. Obes Rev 8, 129154.Google Scholar
24.van der Horst, K, Oenema, A, Ferreira, I et al. (2007) A systematic review of environmental correlates of obesity-related dietary behaviors in youth. Health Educ Res 22, 203226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25.Wendel-Vos, W, Droomers, M, Kremers, SPJ, Brug, J & van Lenthe, FJ (2007) Potential environmental determinants of physical activity in adults: a systematic review. Obes Rev 8, 425440.Google Scholar
26.Kamphuis, CB, Giskens, K, de Bruijn, GJ, Wendel-Vos, W, Brug, J & van Lenthe, FJ (2006) Environmental determinants of fruit and vegetable consumption among adults: a systematic review. Br J Nutr 96, 620635.Google ScholarPubMed
27.Brug, J, van Assema, P, Lenderink, T, Glanz, K & Kok, GJ (1994) Self-rated dietary fat intake: association with objective assessment of fat, psychosocial factors and intention to change. J Nutr Educ 26, 218223.Google Scholar
28.de Bruijn, GJ, Kremers, SPJ, de Vet, E, de Nooijer, J, van Mechelen, W & Brug, J (2007) Does habit strength moderate the intention-behavior relationship? The case of fruit consumption. Psychol Health 22, 899916.Google Scholar
29.de Bruijn, GJ, Kremers, SP, Lensvelt-Mulders, G, de Vries, H, van Mechelen, W & Brug, J (2006) Modeling individual and physical environmental factors with adolescent physical activity. Am J Prev Med 30, 507512.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
30.Kamphuis, CB, van Lenthe, FJ, Giskes, K, Huisman, M, Brug, J & Mackenbach, JP (2008) Socioeconomic status, environmental and individual factors, and sports participation. Med Sci Sports Exerc 40, 7181.Google Scholar
31.de Bruijn, GJ, Kremers, SPJ, van Mechelen, W, Schaalma, H & Brug, J (2005) Determinants of adolescent bicycle use for transportation and snacking behavior. Prev Med 40, 658667.Google Scholar
32.van der Horst, K, Kremers, S, Ferreira, I, Singh, A & Brug, J (2007) Perceived parenting style and practices and the consumption of sugar-sweetened beverages by adolescents. Health Educ Res 22, 295304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33.de Bruijn, GJ, Kremers, SPJ, de Vries, H, van Mechelen, W & Brug, J (2007) Associations of social-environmental and individual-level factors with adolescent soft drink consumption: results from the SMILE study. Health Educ Res 22, 227237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34.Darling, N & Steinberg, L (1993) Parenting style as context: an integrative model. Psych Bull 113, 487496.Google Scholar
35.Verplanken, B & S, Orbell (2003) Reflections on past behavior: A self-report index of habit strength. J Appl Soc Psychol 33, 13131330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
36.Kremers, SPJ & Brug, J (2008) Habit strength of physical activity and sedentary behavior among children and adolescents. Pediatr Exerc Sci 20, 517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
37.Kremers, SPJ, Dijkman, MAM, de Meij, JSB, Jurg, ME & Brug, J (2008) Awareness and habit: important factors in physical activity in children. Health Educ (In the Press).Google Scholar
38.Morland, K, Diez Roux, AV & Wing, S (2006) Supermarkets, other food stores, and obesity: the atherosclerosis risk in communities study. Am J Prev Med 30, 333339.Google Scholar
39.Morland, K, Wing, S & Diez Roux, AV (2002) The contextual effect of the local food environment on residents' diets: the atherosclerosis risk in communities study. Am J Public Health 92, 17611767.Google Scholar
40.Giskes, K, Van Lenthe, FJ, Brug, J, Mackenbach, JP & Turrell, G (2007) Socioeconomic inequalities in food purchasing: The contribution of respondent-perceived and actual (objectively measured) price and availability of foods. Prev Med 45, 4148.Google Scholar
41.Ball, K, Timperio, A, Salmon, J, Giles-Corti, B, Roberts, R & Crawford, D (2007) Personal, social and environmental determinants of educational inequalities in walking: A multilevel study. J Epidemiol Comm Health 61, 108114.Google Scholar
42.Baker, EA, Schootman, M & Barnidge, E (2006) The role of race and poverty in access to foods that enable individuals to adhere to dietary guidelines. Prev Chronic Dis 3; available at http://www.cdc.gov/pcd/issues/2006/jul/05_0217.htmGoogle Scholar
43.Horowitz, CR, Colson, KA, Hebert, PL & Lancaster, K (2004) Barriers to buying healthy foods for people with diabetes: Evidence of environmental disparities. Am J Public Health 94, 15491554.Google Scholar
44.Apparicio, P, Cloutier, M & Shearmur, R (2007) The case of Montreal's missing food deserts: Evaluation of accessibility to food supermarkets. Int J Health Geogr 6, 4.Google Scholar
45.Winkler, E, Turrell, G & Patterson, C (2006) Does living in a disadvantaged area mean fewer opportunities to purchase fresh fruit and vegetables in the area? Findings from the Brisbane food study. Health Place 12, 306319.Google Scholar
46.Cummins, S & Macintyre, S (1999) The location of food stores in urban areas: a case study in Glasgow. British Food Journal 101, 545.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
47.Cummins, S & Macintyre, S (2002) A systematic study of an urban foodscape: The price and availability of food in Greater Glasgow. Urban Studies 39, 21152130.Google Scholar
48.McIntyre, S (2007) Deprivation amplification revisited; or, is it always true that poorer places have poorer access to resources for healthy diets and physical activity? Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 4, 34.Google Scholar
49.Wind, M, De Bourdeaudhuij, I, te Velde, SJ, Sandvik, C, Due, P, Klepp, KI & Brug, J (2006) Correlates of fruit and vegetable consumption among 11-year-old Belgian-Flemish and Dutch schoolchildren. J Nutr Educ Behav 38, 211221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
50.de Bourdeaudhuij, I, te Velde, SJ, Brug, J et al. (2007) Personal, social and environmental predictors of daily fruit and vegetable intake in 11-year-old children in nine European countries. Eur J Clin Nutr; Epublication 16 May 2007.Google Scholar
51.Rasmussen, M, Krolner, R, Klepp, KI, Lytle, L, Brug, J, Bere, E & Due, P (2006) Determinants of fruit and vegetable consumption among children and adolescents: systematic review of the literature. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 3, 22.Google Scholar
52.Kremers, SPJ, Brug, J, de Vries, H & Engels, RCME (2003) Parenting style and adolescent fruit consumption. Appetite 41, 4350.Google Scholar
53.Ball, K & Crawford, D (2005) The role of socio-cultural factors in the obesity epidemic. In Obesity Prevention and Public Health, pp. 3754 [Crawford, D and Jeffery, RW editors]. New York: Oxford University Press Inc.Google Scholar
54.Ball, K, Jeffery, RW, Crawford, D, Roberts, R, Salmon, J & Timperio, A (2008) Mismatch between perceived and objective measures of physical activity environments. Prev Med (In the Press).Google Scholar
55.Coulton, CJ, Korbin, J, Chan, T & Su, M (2001) Mapping residents' perceptions of neighbourhood boundaries: a methodological note. Am J Community Psychol 29, 371383.Google Scholar
56.Hume, C, Salmon, J & Ball, K (2005) Children's perceptions of their home and neighbourhood environments, and their association with objectively measured physical activity: a qualitative and quantitative study. Health Educ Res 20, 113.Google Scholar
57.Veitch, J, Ball, K, Robinson, S & Salmon, J (2006) Where do children play? A qualitative study of parents' perceptions of influences on children's active free-play. Health Place 12, 383393.Google Scholar
58.Robinson, TN & Sirard, JR (2006) Preventing childhood obesity: a solution-oriented research paradigm. Am J Prev Med 28, 194201.Google Scholar
59.te Velde, SJ, Twisk, JWR & Brug, J (2007) Tracking of fruit and vegetable consumption from adolescence into adulthood and its longitudinal association with overweight. Br J Nutr 98, 431438.Google Scholar
60.Singh, AS, Paw, MJ, Brug, J & van Mechelen, W (2007) Short-term effects of school-based weight gain prevention among adolescents. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 161, 565571.Google Scholar
61.De Bruijn, GJ, Kremers, S, Wendel-Vos, W, Van Lenthe, F & Brug, J (2005) Environmental interventions for selected dietary behaviours in youth. In Environmental Determinants and Interventions for Physical Activity, Nutrition and Smoking: A Review, pp. 204239 [Brug, J and van Lenthe, F editors]. Zoetermeer, The Netherlands: Speed-Print BV.Google Scholar
62.van Lenthe, FJ, Kremers, S & Brug, J (2007) Exploring environmental determinants of physical activity – the road to the future is always under construction. Public Health 122, 329.Google Scholar
63.Sallis, JF (2008) Angels in the details: Comment on ‘The relationship between destination proximity, destination mix and physical activity behaviors’. Prev Med 46, 67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
64.Wammes, B, French, S & Brug, J (2007) What young Dutch adults say they do to keep from gaining weight: self-reported prevalence of overeating, compensatory behaviours and specific weight control behaviours. Public Health Nutr 10, 790798.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. A model for planned health education and health promotion.

Figure 1

Fig. 2. The analysis grid for environments linked to obesity (ANGELO) grid. (Based on Swinburn et al.(14).)

Figure 2

Fig. 3. The environmental research model for weight gain prevention. (From Kremers et al.(15).)