Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-wg55d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-01T05:17:36.478Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Introduction to the special issue on cross-cultural neuropsychology

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 November 2023

Mathew Staios
Affiliation:
Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health & School of Psychological Sciences, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia
Sanne Franzen
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology & Alzheimer Center, Erasmus MC University Medical Center, Rotterdam, The Netherlands
John L. Woodard*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Wayne State University, Detroit, MI, USA
*
Corresponding author: John L. Woodard; Email: john.woodard@wayne.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Introduction
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of International Neuropsychological Society

Over the last few decades, cross-cultural neuropsychology, with its origins in anthropology and linguistics, has developed from a niche area of interest to a considerable field of study within neuropsychology. Cross-cultural neuropsychology “analyzes the influence of cultural variables on cognition from a neurological perspective” (Ardila, Reference Ardila2020, p. 64), examining how and why cultural and linguistic factors influence neuropsychological test results. It also involves the study of differences in brain organization across different cultures and how brain pathology manifests in different cultures.

Some of the earliest seeds of cross-cultural neuropsychology were sown by the early 20th-century concepts put forth by Vygotsky, in addition to the work of Luria, who both proposed that “mind is the product of the material conditions of culture” (Nell, Reference Nell1999, p. 45). Over a half-century later, Alfredo Ardila laid the foundations for much of the contemporary work being done today. He wrote extensively on the cultural values underlying neuropsychological testing and the applicability of specific test procedures, elements, and strategies developed in Western Europe and North America to populations in other regions of the world.

Initial studies of cross-cultural neuropsychology mainly focused on capturing how the global diversity in languages, cultures, education, and other considerations impacts performance on neuropsychological tests – a line of research still fruitful today. These studies launched a plethora of new hypotheses about possible underlying mechanisms to explain performance differences on cognitive tests, as well as spurring the development of new testing paradigms, cross-cultural neuropsychological measures, and guidelines aimed at providing the field with the right tools and inspiration to improve assessment in cross-cultural settings.

The first comprehensive handbooks on cross-cultural neuropsychology were published at the turn of the century (e.g., Fletcher-Jansen et al., Reference Fletcher-Jansen, Strickland and Reynolds2000). The number of publications since then slowly increased over the next 20 years. However, a surge of research in this domain has recently occurred. A search of the literature to date shows that only 41 publications explicitly mention “cross-cultural neuropsychology” in their title or as a keyword. Still, fully half of these publications have appeared within the last three years.

This special issue contributes nine state-of-the-art studies to the literature on cross-cultural neuropsychology. The papers in this volume focus on three overall themes: (1) the development of novel tests and testing paradigms to facilitate cross-cultural neuropsychological assessment; (2) linguistic considerations and adaptations for existing neuropsychological assessment procedures; and (3) cross-cultural self-report and informant report measures.

Development of novel cross-cultural measures and testing paradigms

Nielsen et al., developed and validated a brief naming test, the Copenhagen Cross-Linguistic Naming Test (C-CLNT), for use with culturally, linguistically, and educationally diverse older adults in Europe. The C-CLNT was based on a set of standardized color drawings and selected by considering name agreement and frequency across five European and two non-European languages. The C-CLNT displayed acceptable scale reliability and good construct validity, with moderate to strong correlations with traditional language tests. Diagnostic accuracy for dementia was good and significantly better than that of the Boston Naming Test but was poor for mild cognitive impairment. A different approach to circumventing language barriers is used by Ampofo et al. In an elegant study, the authors demonstrated that the use of semantically meaningless consonant-vowel-consonant trigrams, such as VAB, could be used as stimuli in a memory test with minimal changes to accommodate for language differences between American and Italian participants. Such trigrams may be a promising aid in conducting cross-linguistic memory studies (at least, for studies with a Latin/Roman alphabet or similar phonology of words).

Linguistic considerations and adaptations for existing neuropsychological procedures

McHenry et al., evaluated the psychometric properties of the culturally adapted NIH Toolbox African Languages for use with Swahili and Dholuo-speaking children in western Kenya. Most tests showed acceptable psychometric properties for use within these specific language groups. However, issues related to shape identification and trading speed for accuracy limited the utility of the Dimensional Change Card Sort for many participants, with approximately 25% of children unable to match based on shape. While this test battery showed promise for use with these populations, further research is needed to establish these measures' clinical utility and acceptability across culturally diverse populations.

Salonen et al., showed that the use of Swedish WPPSI-IV normative data for assessing young children belonging to the Swedish-speaking minority in Finland (Finland-Swedes) resulted in, on average, 1/3 SD higher than the Scandinavian norms, a difference which was statistically significant with medium-sized effects. In contrast, Staios et al., showed that the use of majority English language normative data resulted in significantly underestimating performances across a range of neuropsychological measures within a sample of healthy, older, educationally disadvantaged Greek Australian immigrants, including the WAIS-IV, verbal and visual memory tests, language and naming, and executive functions. However, the newly developed representative Greek Australian normative data were shown to be superior in comparison to existing English language norms and was also capable of sensitively and explicitly distinguishing between the healthy controls and those with dementia.

Porsselvi et al., adapted the Oxford Cognitive Screen for use with Tamil speakers and demonstrated the importance of considering language and education in test design/adaptation. The authors ultimately decided to incorporate both aspects of formal, “high variety” Tamil, as spoken in educational settings, and “low variety” Tamil, informal Tamil, spoken by many in everyday life but not used in writing.

Finally, Crisan et al., administered a neuropsychological battery to native speakers of Romanian and Arabic (who have linguistically and orthographically dissimilar languages). Animal fluency and the Emotion Word Fluency Test (Abeare et al., Reference Abeare, Freund, Kaploun, McAuley and Dumitrescu2017) were administered in English and in their native language in a counterbalanced fashion to assess the effects of limited English proficiency. They compared the performance of these two groups with a Canadian sample of native English speakers tested only in English to investigate the effect of limited English proficiency on neuropsychological performance. Measures with high verbal mediation were predictably associated with lower performance in participants with limited English proficiency. In contrast, tests with minimal verbal mediation tended to be more resistant to the effects of limited English proficiency. However, this pattern showed unexpected differences between the two limited English proficiency groups. For example, on the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System version of the Stroop test (Delis et al., Reference Delis, Kaplan and Kramer2001), which tends to be less verbally mediated, the Romanian sample continued to improve across trials, while the Arabic group declined. In contrast, on the highly verbally mediated Animal fluency task, the Romanian participants improved their performance when tested in their native language, while the Arabic sample showed the opposite pattern, performing better when tested in English. This performance disparity was unexpected and suggests that simply evaluating an individual in their native language is sometimes not effective in minimizing the impact of language proficiency on neuropsychological test performance. It may occasionally produce inadvertent effects, resulting in unexpected or distorted cognitive results. In addition to these thought-provoking findings regarding the impact of language proficiency on neurocognitive performance, the authors identified several measures that appear to be resistant to limited English proficiency.

Impact of culture on self- and informant report of neuropsychiatric symptoms

Two studies examined measures relying on the patient’s and caregiver’s perception of symptoms. Nguyen et al., studied how the commonly used Neuropsychiatric Inventory can be adapted to Vietnamese and subsequently be used to assess neuropsychiatric symptoms after traumatic brain injury. Their findings add to a growing literature, mainly from the United States, Europe, and Asia (Lai, Reference Lai2014; Cummings, Reference Cummings2020), indicating that some neuropsychiatric abnormalities after brain injury likely have a sizeable biological component and are potentially less culturally determined. The study by Statucka et al., addresses a topic that has received little attention in the international cross-cultural literature so far: the assessment of subjective complaints and functional impairment in diverse populations. The authors found that immigration status in their sample had a limited effect on reporting cognitive change and the degree to which such change limited daily functioning, suggesting that such measures may be more robust than cognitive tests in these populations. Future studies may examine this topic in more detail by evaluating different instruments and conducting differential item functioning analysis. For example, in a study of eight European countries by Dubbelman et al. (2020), differential item functioning was identified for items such as using a washing machine, making appointments, or playing card or board games, indicating that (subtle) cultural bias may be present in these instruments.

Challenges for cross-cultural neuropsychology

Although the studies included in this special issue provide some next steps and inspiration for future research, conducting studies in cross-cultural neuropsychology is challenging due to the many factors that must be considered. For example, differences between diverse groups may easily be attributed to cultural factors, while differences in language or education actually drive these effects. For example, Manly et al., (Reference Manly, Jacobs, Touradji, Small and Stern2002) demonstrated that expected differences between different ethnoracial groups were attenuated if the reading level, a proxy of quality of education, was taken into consideration. Therefore, researchers must measure many aspects of diversity to identify how cultural groups differ on tests. These factors, outlined in the ECLECTIC framework (Fujii, Reference Fujii D.E.2018), include education quantity and quality, literacy, acculturation, language proficiency, bilingualism, comfort with the testing situation, and test wiseness. Even for a “simple” translation of an existing test, many linguistic factors should ideally be considered, such as word frequency, age of acquisition, familiarity, word length, phonemic complexity, imageability, and many other factors (for an informative overview, see, e.g., Ivanova & Hallowell, Reference Ivanova and Hallowell2013). The study by Crisan et al., in this issue, provides an additional perspective on the issue of the effects of language proficiency on neuropsychological performance.

Another challenge to research in cross-cultural neuropsychology is that there are numerous ecological, sociological, and economic factors related to culture that could affect early life experiences and can profoundly influence the development and differentiation of cognitive functions and neural structures (Chua et al., Reference Chua, Liu and Nisbett2005; Goh & Park, Reference Goh and Park2005). The relative impact of each of these factors on cognitive or neural systems may be challenging to disentangle. Research in this domain should consider the differential effects of factors such as socialization, environmental and ecological demands and supports, education (including curriculum and learning environment), language (e.g., mono- versus multilingualism, proficiency in the language tested), value systems, family constellation, and theory of mind (e.g., Sabbagh et al., Reference Sabbagh, Xu, Carlson, Moses and Lee2006).

Finally, culture is constantly changing. With such changes, the familiarity with and relevance of testing procedures and test stimuli can also change over time. As a result, the structure of neuropsychological assessment procedures and stimuli may not be applicable, familiar, or even behaviorally appropriate across all cultures (Ardila, Reference Ardila2018) or even over time. Basic strategies used by examinees on various tasks have also been shown to differ across cultures (Wang, Reference Wang2021). Future studies must assess the impact of examinees' strategy use (e.g., methods for imposing meaning on stimuli), the applicability of neuropsychological test stimuli, and the structure and design of cognitive tests across cultures on cognitive performance. The effects of cultural differences on the perceived interpersonal dynamics inherent in the assessment procedure must also be considered. Across the spectrum of cultural differences, it is also essential to keep in mind that the meaning and relevance of cognitive constructs varies across cultures (e.g., memory (Wang, Reference Wang2021), executive functioning (Cho et al., Reference Cho, Hosseini-Kamkar, Song and Morton2023)). For example, Western concepts of memory emphasize processes such as recall and recognition, while Buddhist ideas of memory focus on aspects such as apperception, perception, insight, and mindfulness (Cassaniti, Reference Cassaniti2018, Deshpande, Reference Deshpande1996, Wang, Reference Wang2021).

Recommendations for research in cross-cultural neuropsychology

We are currently at the point of being able to formulate and propose testable theoretical models that account for cultural influences on the growth and deterioration of cognitive abilities across the lifespan. We must go beyond simple descriptions of cultural characteristics and differences at the phenotypic level. We have the capacity to explain what accounts for them from an ontological perspective and should consider these issues in the future design of novel neuropsychological instruments and assessment approaches.

Future studies of cross-cultural neuropsychology should implement and systematically report demographic variables and consensually adopted, clear, and specific standards in their research articles. O’Bryant et al. (Reference O’Bryant, O’Jile and McCaffrey2004) noted that information on race, ethnicity, native language and acculturation are often not reported in research articles, which likely limits progress in cross-cultural neuropsychology. Medina et al. (Reference Medina, Torres, Gioia, Ochoa Lopez, Wang and Cirino2021) provided a recent update on these trends, and while reporting of these variables in research papers is slowly improving, greater systematic reporting of these variables is needed. They present specific recommendations by Rad et al. (Reference Rad, Martingano and Ginges2018) as a possible path forward. All researchers (even those outside cross-cultural neuropsychology) should strongly consider implementing these recommendations to enhance the transparency and representation of research in the field as a whole.

At the most basic level, direct translation of instruments from one language to another should be avoided. For example, the original Auditory Verbal Learning Test (Rey, Reference Rey1958) used two-syllable words for all test stimuli (Woodard, Reference Woodard and Poreh2006). Taylor's (Reference Taylor1959) primarily direct translation of Rey’s list of French words into English resulted in a loss of the bisyllabic list structure and changes in word frequency of the items across the two languages. If translation is used, researchers should systematically report and clarify the procedure used to translate the material, the background and training of the translators, and other relevant factors. In clinical studies, standards for using interpreters should also be implemented. For example, researchers should clarify how interpreters were trained and prepared to assist with the evaluation.

We would also advocate for developing and using cross-cultural neuropsychological evaluation procedures rather than race-based norms (Franzen et al., Reference Franzen, Pomati and Papma2022). The examinee is much more than a number corresponding to a score on a test. To again borrow from Luria and Vygotsky, the score represents a phenotype. We must seek to identify the ontological constructs underlying cognitive performance. Advanced psychometric procedures are also available to establish neuropsychological measures' measurement equivalence and diagnostic validity (Pedraza & Mungas, Reference Pedraza and Mungas2008). For example, Item Response Theory techniques can be used for test construction and identifying differential item functioning that may contribute to item bias on cognitive tests. In addition, Bayesian methods have much more to offer over their frequentist counterparts for the development of normative data and interpretation of the performance of an individual relative to a normative group (Crawford & Garthwaite, Reference Crawford and Garthwaite2007; Huygelier et al., Reference Huygelier, Gillebert and Moors2022). Bayesian approaches can also be used to differentiate the impact of cultural traits associated with previous generations and in adjacent populations (transmitted culture) and ecological factors (evoked culture) on cognitive performance (Hyafil & Baumard, Reference Hyafil and Baumard2022).

Finally, much existing cross-cultural research is fraught with methodological concerns, including the use of small sample sizes, which are typically underpowered for detecting meaningful outcomes, and convenience samples rather than random sampling to obtain groups representative of the cultures they purport to reflect. Even if random selection cannot be fully accomplished, comparing critical sample characteristics to known or expected population characteristics is possible to characterize the sample’s representativeness. Finally, the use of Bayesian techniques, as mentioned earlier, can be used to establish and interpret normative data more accurately. Other advanced methodological innovations that could clarify factors related to the growth or decline in neuropsychological functions over the lifespan are also readily available. For example, longitudinal modeling of developmental trajectories using latent growth curve modeling, latent change score modeling, or linear mixed effects modeling can be used to characterize the rate and extent of development or decline of cognitive abilities and cultural factors that might be related to such change.

In summary, the papers in this Special Issue present cutting-edge, novel approaches and methodologies for studying important issues in cross-cultural neuropsychology. As noted earlier, exploring the impact of culture is challenging, given varying definitions of culture, the changing nature of culture, and the numerous culturally-related factors that can influence cognitive and neural development. Notwithstanding these challenges, research on cross-cultural neuropsychology has enjoyed unprecedented growth within just the last few years. We hope that this Special Issue will provide a solid contribution to the knowledge base in this field and will stimulate much more innovative research to provide new insights into the effects of culture on neuropsychological functioning

Competing interests

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

Abeare, C. A., Freund, S., Kaploun, K., McAuley, T., & Dumitrescu, C. (2017). The emotion word fluency test (EWFT): Initial psychometric, validation, and physiological evidence in young adults. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 39(8), 738752. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2016.1259396 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ardila, A. (2018). Historical development of human cognition: A cultural-historical neuropsychological perspective. Springer Nature Private Limited.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ardila, A. (2020). Cross-cultural neuropsychology: History and prospects. RUDN Journal of Psychology and Pedagogics, 17 (1), 6478. https://doi.org/10.22363/2313-1683-2020-17-1-64-78 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cassaniti, J. (2018). Remembering the present: Mindfulness in buddhist Asia. Cornell Univ. Press.Google Scholar
Cho, I., Hosseini-Kamkar, N., Song, H., & Morton, J. B. (2023). Culture, executive functions, and academic achievement. Frontiers in Psychology, 14, 1100537. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1100537 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chua, H. F., Liu, J., & Nisbett, R. E. (2005). Culture and diverging views of social events. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31 (7), 925934.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crawford, J. R., & Garthwaite, P. H. (2007). Comparison of a single case to a control or normative sample in neuropsychology: Development of a Bayesian approach. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24 (4), 343372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cummings, J. (2020). The neuropsychiatric inventory: Development and applications. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 33 (2), 7384. https://doi.org/10.1177/0891988719882102 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J. H. (2001). Delis-Kaplan executive function system: Examiner’s manual. The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Deshpande, C. G. (1996). Memory as depicted in eastern and western thoughts. Psycho-Lingua, 26 (2), 5765.Google Scholar
Fletcher-Jansen, E, Strickland, T, Reynolds, C (2000). Handbook of cross-cultural neuropsychology. Kluwer Academic.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franzen, S., Pomati, S., Papma, J. M., on behalf of the European Consortium on Cross-Cultural Neuropsychology (2022). Cross-cultural neuropsychological assessment in Europe: Position statement of the european consortium on cross-cultural neuropsychology (ECCroN). The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 36 (3), 546557. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2021.1981456 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fujii D.E., M. (2018). Developing a cultural context for conducting a neuropsychological evaluation with a culturally diverse client: The ECLECTIC framework. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 32 (8), 13561392. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2018.1435826 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goh, J. O., & Park, D. C. (2005). Culture sculpts the perceptual brain. Progress in Brain Research, 178, 95111.10.1016/S0079-6123(09)17807-XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huygelier, H., Gillebert, C. R., & Moors, P. (2022). The value of Bayesian methods for accurate and efficient neuropsychological assessment. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 28 (9), 984995.10.1017/S1355617721001120CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hyafil, A., & Baumard, N. (2022). Evoked and transmitted culture models: Using Bayesian methods to infer the evolution of cultural traits in history. PLoS ONE, 17 (4), e0264509. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0264509 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ivanova, M. V., & Hallowell, B. (2013). A tutorial on aphasia test development in any language: Key substantive and psychometric considerations. Aphasiology, 27 (8), 891920. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2013.805728 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lai, C. K. (2014). The merits and problems of neuropsychiatric inventory as an assessment tool in people with dementia and other neurological disorders. Clinical Interventions in Aging, 9, 10511061. https://doi.org/10.2147/CIA.S63504 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manly, J. J., Jacobs, D. M., Touradji, P., Small, S. A., & Stern, Y. (2002). Reading level attenuates differences in neuropsychological test performance between African American and White elders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 341348. doi: 10.1017/s1355617702813157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Medina, L. D., Torres, S., Gioia, A., Ochoa Lopez, A., Wang, J., & Cirino, P. T. (2021). Reporting of demographic variables in neuropsychological research: An update of O'Bryant, etal’s trends in the current literature. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 27 (5), 497507. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617720001083 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nell, V. (1999). Luria in Uzbekistan: The vicissitudes of cross-cultural neuropsychology. Neuropsychology Review, 9 (1), 4552. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1025643004782 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Bryant, S. E., O’Jile, J. R., & McCaffrey, R. J. (2004). Reporting of demographic variables in neuropsychological research: Trends in the current literature. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 18 (2), 229233. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854040490501439 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pedraza, O., & Mungas, D. (2008). Measurement in cross-cultural neuropsychology. Neuropsychological Review, 18 (3), 184193. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-008-9067-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rad, M. S., Martingano, A. J., & Ginges, J. (2018). Toward a psychology of Homo sapiens: Making psychological science more representative of the human population. Proceedings of The National Academy of Sciences of The United States of America, 115 (45), 1140111405. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1721165115 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rey, A. (1958). L’examen clinique en psychologie. Presses Universitaires de France.Google Scholar
Sabbagh, M. A., Xu, F., Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Lee, K. (2006). The development of executive functioning and theory of mind: A comparison of Chinese and U.S. pre-schoolers. Psychological Science, 17 (1), 7481. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01667.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, E. M. (1959). Psychological appraisal of children with cerebral defects. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, Q. (2021). The cultural foundation of human memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 72, 151179. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-070920-023638 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodard, J. L. (2006). Memory performance indexes for the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test. In Poreh, A. (Eds.), The quantified process approach (pp. 105141). Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar