Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-18T08:24:35.110Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Rationalisation of meat consumption in New Zealand adolescents

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 August 2021

Kelly R Latimer
Affiliation:
Department of Human Nutrition, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
Meredith C Peddie
Affiliation:
Department of Human Nutrition, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
Tessa Scott
Affiliation:
Department of Human Nutrition, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
Jillian J Haszard*
Affiliation:
Division of Sciences, University of Otago, P.O. Box 56, Dunedin 9054, New Zealand
*
*Corresponding author: Email jill.haszard@otago.ac.nz
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

This study aimed to describe meat consumption rationalisation and relationships with meat consumption patterns and food choice motivations in New Zealand adolescents.

Design:

This was a cross-sectional study of adolescents from high schools across New Zealand. Demographics, dietary habits, and motivations and attitudes towards food were assessed by online questionnaire and anthropometric measurements taken by researchers. The 4Ns questionnaire assessed meat consumption rationalisation with four subscales: ‘Nice’, ‘Normal’, ‘Necessary’ and ‘Natural’.

Setting:

Nineteen secondary schools from eight regions in New Zealand, with some purposive sampling of adolescent vegetarians in Otago, New Zealand.

Participants:

Questionnaires were completed by 385 non-vegetarian and vegetarian (self-identified) adolescents.

Results:

A majority of non-vegetarian adolescents agreed that consuming meat was ‘nice’ (65 %), but fewer agreed that meat consumption was ‘necessary’ (51 %). Males agreed more strongly than females with all 4N subscales. High meat consumers were more likely to agree than to disagree that meat consumption was nice, normal, necessary and natural, and vegetarians tended to disagree with all rationalisations. Adolescent non-vegetarians whose food choice was motivated more by convenience, sensory appeal, price and familiarity tended to agree more with all 4N subscales, whereas adolescents motivated by animal welfare and environmental concerns were less likely to agree.

Conclusions:

To promote a reduction in meat consumption in adolescents, approaches will need to overcome beliefs that meat consumption is nice, normal, necessary and natural.

Type
Research paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

The consumption of meat has attracted considerable attention in recent years(Reference Aiking1) due to numerous negative effects of meat production and consumption on both population and planetary health(Reference Aiking1Reference Joy3). Research has highlighted strong correlations between consumption of red and processed meats and the risk of developing non-communicable diseases, such as CVD, type 2 diabetes and colorectal cancer(Reference Bendinelli, Palli and Masala4Reference Wada, Oba and Tsuji6). Increasing awareness of climate change has also raised concerns surrounding the sustainability of meat production and the short- and long-term effects on our environment(Reference Curry2,Reference de Boer and Aiking7) . Alongside these concerns, ethical and animal welfare concerns have been raised, because the increasing industrialisation of meat production has caused a substantial deterioration in welfare(Reference Curry2).

Experts are therefore calling for a global shift to a plant-based diet, defined as the habitual increased consumption of fruits, vegetables, legumes, nuts, seeds and a variety of grains (including wholegrains), alongside the decreased or minimal consumption of meat, eggs and dairy products(Reference Lea, Crawford and Worsley8). It is hypothesised that a shift to a plant-based diet would be beneficial in terms of reducing the risk of non-communicable diseases, reducing the unethical treatment of animals and maintaining environmental sustainability(Reference de Boer and Aiking7,Reference Thompson9Reference Grant11) . A Summary Report of the EAT-Lancet Commission(Reference Willett, Rockström and Loken10) identified that the consumption of fruits, vegetables, legumes and nuts needed to more than double, while the consumption of red meat needed to more than halve in order to meet their Scientific Targets for Healthy Diets and Sustainable Food Production by 2050.

To be able to develop consumer-orientated strategies for a transition to a more plant-based diet, personal determinants of behaviour, including habits, attitudes, knowledge and barriers to change, must be recognised(Reference Sallis, Pinski and Grossman12). One key barrier to changing meat consumption habits in the population will be overcoming the rationalisations that people make when consuming meat. Many omnivores face a ‘meat paradox’ where they enjoy consuming meat but do not like the thought of harming animals or the thought that their meat consumption is contributing to global warming(Reference Joy3,Reference Loughnan, Haslam and Bastian13) . Therefore, consumers rationalise their meat consumption by contending that meat is natural – part of human nature and biology; necessary – one cannot live without meat, and nutrients found only in meat are essential for a healthy, balanced diet; normal – how we behave based on customs or traditions; or nice – the taste of meat and associated satisfaction(Reference Joy3,Reference Piazza, Ruby and Loughnan14) . These rationalisations are referred to as the ‘4Ns’, with research in adults showing that these four concepts cover the majority of rationalisations when it comes to meat consumption(Reference Piazza, Ruby and Loughnan14). The 4Ns enable omnivores to continue to eat meat with low levels of guilt and motivation to change meat-eating behaviours(Reference Piazza, Ruby and Loughnan14). Therefore, to overcome a commitment to meat consumption, these rationalisations need to be addressed.

Adolescence presents an opportunity to modify meat consumption at a stage in life that sees an increase in autonomy surrounding food choices(Reference Sturdevant and Spear15), as well as the development and increasing awareness of personal values. Furthermore, dietary habits developed during this time may track into adulthood(Reference Mikkilä, Räsänen and Raitakari16), meaning that interventions or public health messages targeting the adolescent diet may create long-term improvements in population health and planetary outcomes. Previous research suggests adolescents tend to be ‘present-oriented’ and are less concerned about long-term health issues, when compared to older adults(Reference Share and Stewart-Knox17,Reference MacIntyre, Reilly, Miller and Murcott18) . Therefore, if behaviour change strategies are to be effective, an understanding of adolescent motivations for food choice and rationalisation of meat consumption is crucial for health promotion, informing policy and food manufacturers, as well as others who hold an interest in promoting a population shift to a more plant-based diet(Reference Share and Stewart-Knox17). Using the 4Ns questionnaire, the present study aimed to describe meat consumption rationalisation and relationships with meat consumption patterns and food choice motivations in a sample of New Zealand adolescents.

Methods

Study design

The present study is the secondary analysis of the Survey of Nutrition, Dietary Assessment and Lifestyles (SuNDiAL) project, which encompasses two New Zealand-wide cross-sectional surveys, one of female adolescents conducted between February and October 2019 and the other of male adolescents conducted between February and April 2020 (all data for this analysis were collected prior to COVID-19 lockdown). The project was registered with the Australian New Zealand Clinical Trials Registry: ACTRN12619000290190 (2019) and ACTRN12620000185965 (2020).

Participants and recruitment

Recruitment of the girls has been described in detail previously(Reference Peddie, Ranasinghe and Scott19), with similar procedures followed for recruitment of the boys. Briefly, secondary schools were recruited by email in locations where data collectors were based. A total of nineteen secondary schools from eight regions in New Zealand were recruited across the two studies – thirteen schools for the female cohort in 2019 and six different schools for the male cohort in 2020. The number of schools for the male cohort was considerably lower than for the female cohort because recruitment was cut short in 2020 due to COVID-19 lockdown. Data collectors then gave presentations to each of the recruited schools as part of whole-school assemblies, or to year groups or individual classes, to initiate adolescent recruitment. Interested and eligible participants were given a study information sheet and invited to sign up via email or via the study website. In addition to recruitment through high schools, there was purposive sampling of vegetarians in the 2019 female cohort to meet the primary objective of that phase of the SuNDiAL project, which was to compare the dietary intakes of vegetarian and non-vegetarian female adolescents(Reference Peddie, Ranasinghe and Scott19). This included targeted recruitment advertisements through Facebook in Dunedin and Christchurch of vegetarians not from recruited schools. Participants were eligible to participate if they self-identified as either male (in 2020) or female (not pregnant in 2019), were between 15 and 18 years of age, and spoke and understood English.

Data collection

Once online consent was obtained, participants were given a series of online questionnaires to complete in their own time. These included questions about demographics, health, dietary habits, and motivations and attitudes towards food and were administered online through REDCap (Research Electronic Data Capture)(Reference Harris, Taylor and Thielke20). The data collectors visited the schools to collect anthropometric measurements (height and weight), assign accelerometers to participants for the measurement of physical activity and conduct the first of two 24-h diet recalls (the second being conducted during a follow-up phone or video call within the following 2 weeks). The analysis presented here only utilises data from the online questionnaires and the anthropometric assessments.

The 4Ns questionnaire

The 4Ns questionnaire consists of sixteen statements about different types of rationalisations when it comes to meat consumption(Reference Piazza, Ruby and Loughnan14). There are four subscales (four items per subscale), scored on a seven-point scale: participants were asked to mark how strongly they agreed or disagreed with the given statements. The subscales are Natural (‘it is only natural to eat meat’; ‘it is unnatural to eat an all plant-based diet’; ‘our human ancestors ate meat all the time’; ‘human beings naturally crave meat’), Normal (‘not eating meat is socially unacceptable’; ‘it is abnormal for humans not to eat meat’; ‘it is normal to eat meat’; ‘most people I know eat meat’), Necessary (‘it is necessary to eat meat in order to be healthy’; ‘you cannot get all the protein, vitamins and minerals you need on an all plant-based diet’; ‘human beings need to eat meat’; ‘a healthy diet requires at least some meat’) and Nice (‘meat adds so much flavour to a meal it does not make sense to leave it out’; ‘the best tasting food is normally a meat based dish (e.g. steak, chicken breast, grilled fish)’; ‘meals without meat would just be bland and boring’; ‘meat is delicious’). Scores for each subscale were calculated as the mean of all items, giving a score between 1 (strongly disagree) and 7 (strongly agree). Agreement with the rationalisations (subscales) was categorised based on a score of greater than 4.

Household deprivation

Participants provided their home address which was used to determine their New Zealand Deprivation Index 2018 decile(Reference Atkinson, Salmond and Crampton21). This was then collapsed into three categories: low deprivation (deciles 1 to 3); medium deprivation (deciles 4 to 7) and high deprivation (deciles 8 to 10).

BMI

Height and weight were measured using standard protocols(Reference Peddie, Ranasinghe and Scott19). BMI was calculated and Z-scores determined using the WHO growth charts(Reference de Onis22). Overweight was classified as those with a Z-score greater than 1 and less than or equal to 2; and obese was classified as those with a Z-score greater than 2.

Meat consumption habits

Participants were asked at the beginning of the online survey ‘Are you vegetarian or vegan?’ with yes/no options. Meat consumption habits were assessed using the Dietary Habits Questionnaire(23). Participants were asked how often they consume processed meats, other red meats, pork, poultry, fish or other seafoods, with answer options including ‘more than 3 times a day; 2–3 times a day; once a day; 5–6 times a week; 2–4 times a week; once a week; 2–3 times a month; monthly; rarely or I do not eat these’. These meat variables were combined to give an overall ‘number of times a week’ meat was consumed. This was then collapsed into a binary variable: ‘Low meat consumers’ (no more than five times a week) and ‘Moderate/high meat consumers’ (more than five times a week).

Food choice motivations

Motivations for food choice were measured using a combination of the Food Choice Questionnaire(Reference Steptoe, Pollard and Wardle24) and the Ethical Food Choice Motives Questionnaire(Reference Lindeman and Väänänen25). These questionnaires have been well validated in several populations, including in a sample of Irish adolescents(Reference Share and Stewart-Knox17,Reference Steptoe, Pollard and Wardle24Reference Markovina, Stewart-Knox and Rankin26) , and were specifically developed to assess both health-related and non-health-related factors that affect food choice, as well as factors relevant to vegetarians (e.g. animal welfare, environmental protection and religion). Participants were asked how important the food they eat on a typical day resonated with the given statement. Food choice motivations were measured on a scale from 1 to 4 (1 = not at all important; 4 = very important). Eight of the nine subscales from the Food Choice Questionniare were used, with the ‘Ethical concern’ subscale replaced by the ‘Environmental concerns’, ‘Animal welfare’ and ‘Religion’ subscales from the Ethical Food Choice Motives questionnaire(Reference Lindeman and Väänänen25). In total, the eleven subscales were Health (6 items), Mood (6 items), Convenience (4 items), Sensory appeal (4 items), Natural content (3 items), Price (3 items), Weight control (3 items), Familiarity (3 items), Animal welfare (2 items), Environmental concerns (3 items) and Religion (2 items). Subscale scores were calculated as the mean of all items.

Statistical methods

All statistical analyses were undertaken in Stata 16.1 (StataCorp.). To determine demographic predictors of the meat rationalisation scores in non-vegetarians, mixed effects regression models were used, with the subscale score as the dependent variable and the demographic variable (sex, age, deprivation, BMI Z-score or weight status) as the independent variable. School was included as a random effect. Mean differences, 95 % CI and P-values were calculated.

To illustrate the differences in the distributions of the meat rationalisation subscale scores between non-vegetarians and vegetarians, box plots were generated. The relationships between meat consumption patterns (low consumers v. moderate/high consumers) and meat consumption rationalisation scores were assessed using mixed effects regression models as before, but with further adjustment for age, sex and deprivation. To assess correlations between meat consumption rationalisation scores and food choice motivations, correlation coefficients were calculated for non-vegetarians. Residuals of all regression models were plotted and visually assessed for homogeneity of variance and normality.

Results

School recruitment and participant response

Thirteen secondary schools consented to participate and were recruited in 2019, across which 257 females consented to participate. Fifteen vegetarian females were additionally recruited in 2019 through targeted advertising. Complete data for this assessment were collected from a total of 259 female participants. Eight secondary schools consented to participate and were recruited in 2020; however, due to the restrictions in place with COVID-19 during 2020, only six of the schools were able to participate. One hundred and forty-six males consented to participate, with data collected from a final sample of 126 male participants. Overall, 385 participants enrolled, consented and provided data for this secondary analysis of the SuNDiAL project.

Demographic characteristics of participants

Forty-eight (12·5 %) participants self-identified as vegetarian, forty-four (91·7 %) of whom were female. Vegetarians tended to be slightly older, live in an area of lower deprivation and be of a healthy weight; however, the proportions of participants with low, medium and high levels of deprivation and who were categorised as healthy, overweight or obese were similar in non-vegetarian males v. females (Table 1).

Table 1 Demographic and anthropometric characteristics of all participants (n 385)

* Fifteen female vegetarians were recruited through targeted methods; 44 (91.7 %) of the vegetarians were female. Vegetarians were self-identified.

Household deprivation measured using deciles of the NZDep2018 index, with Low: deciles 1–3; Medium: deciles 4–7; High: deciles 8–10. Two participants in the non-vegetarian sample were missing these data.

BMI Z-score calculated using the WHO growth charts. Overweight: BMI Z-score ≥1 & <2; Obese: BMI Z-score ≥2. 52 participants did not have anthropometric measures undertaken (n 45 non-vegetarians and n 7 vegetarians).

Predictors of meat consumption rationalisation in non-vegetarains

Table 2 reports mean differences (95 % CI) in the 4Ns subscale scores between different demographic groups (i.e. sex, household deprivation and weight status), as well as for age (for each year older) and BMI Z-score (for each Z-score higher). Across subscales, ‘nice’ was the rationalisation with the highest level of agreement, while ‘necessary’ had the lowest (Table 2). Males agreed more strongly across all four subscales, when compared to females, with the greatest mean difference seen in the ‘nice’ subscale (−0·6; (95 % CI −0·9, −0·2)). There was no evidence of a relationship between age and meat consumption rationalisations. Those who lived in more deprived households were more likely to rationalise meat consumption as ‘nice’ compared to those of low deprivation. There was no evidence that BMI Z-score or weight status was related to the rationalisations that consuming meat is ‘natural’, ‘nice’ or ‘normal’, but a small tendency for those of higher BMI Z-score or weight status to agree that eating meat is not necessary.

Table 2 Demographic and anthropometric predictors of meat consumption rationalisation in non-vegetarians (n 337)

* Each subscale (natural, nice, necessary and normal) is scored using a seven-point scale from strongly disagree to strongly agree.

Household deprivation measured using deciles of the NZDep2018 index, with Low: deciles 1–3; Medium: deciles 4–7; and High: deciles 8–10. Two participants in the non-vegetarian sample were missing these data.

BMI Z-score calculated using the WHO growth charts. Overweight:BMI Z-score ≥1 & <2; Obese: BMI Z-score ≥2. 52 participants did not have anthropometric measures undertaken (n 45 non-vegetarians and n 7 vegetarians).

Meat consumption rationalisation between vegetarians and non-vegetarians

Vegetarians disagreed with nearly all of the meat consumption rationalisations (Fig. 1). In non-vegetarians, 60·2 % agreed that eating meat was ‘natural’ (compared to 4·2 % of vegetarians), 64·7 % agreed that meat was ‘nice’ (compared to 0 % of vegetarians), 51·3 % agreed that meat was necessary (compared to 0 % of vegetarians) and 56·4 % agreed that eating meat was normal (compared to 14·6 % of vegetarians).

Fig. 1 Box plots of meat rationalisation scores by self-identified vegetarian status (n 337 non-vegetarians and n 48 vegetarians). Each subscale is scored using a seven-point scale from strongly disagree to strongly agree about eating meat so that a score of 4 corresponded to ‘neither agree nor disagree’ (indicated by the dashed line)

Meat consumption rationalisation and meat consumption patterns

Table 3 reports mean differences (95 % CI) in the 4Ns subscale scores for low meat consumers and self-identified vegetarians compared to moderate/high meat consumers. Moderate/high meat consumers endorsed all 4N subscales more than low meat consumers and self-identifed vegetarians (Table 3). Self-identified vegetarians agreed to an even lesser extent that meat was natural, nice, necessary or normal, compared to the moderate/high meat consumers, with the greatest difference seen again in the ‘nice’ subscale (mean (sd) 4·8 (1·3) moderate/high v. 1·6 (0·8) self-identified vegetarian).

Table 3 Differences in meat consumption rationalisation by meat consumption patterns* (n 377)

* Meat consumption patterns determined using the Dietary Habits Questionnaire, which assessed frequency of meat consumption (processed meat, red meat, pork and poultry). Those who indicated that they consumed meat no more than five times a week were classified as ‘low meat consumers’ (n 39); those who indicated that they consumed meat more than five times a week were classified as ‘moderate/high meat consumers’ (n 290). Eight participants did not complete the Dietary Habits Questionnaire and were excluded from this analysis.

Adjusted mean differences (95% CI) and P-values were adjusted for age, sex, and deprivation and accounted for school clusters.

Meat consumption rationalisation and food choice motivations

On average, sensory appeal was the most important food choice motivation in non-vegetarians, closely followed by price, while religion was the least important in this sample of New Zealand adolescents (Table 4). Adolescent non-vegetarians whose food choice was motivated by convenience, sensory appeal, price and familiarity tended to agree more with all 4N subscales, indicated by the positive correlation coefficients, although correlations were weak (all less than 0·23). Whereas adolescents whose food choice was motivated by animal welfare and environmental concerns were less likely to agree with all 4N subscales, indicated by the negative correlation coefficients (again correlations were small, all less than 0·25).

Table 4 Correlations* between food choice motivations and rationalisation of meat consumption in non-vegetarians (n 337)

* Correlation coefficients are reported.

Food choice motivations were measured using four-point scales from not at all important (1) to very important (4).

Discussion

This is the first study in New Zealand to investigate the correlates of meat consumption rationalisation in male and female adolescents. Despite current recommendations and health messages surrounding the negative effects that high meat consumption can have(Reference Bendinelli, Palli and Masala4Reference Wada, Oba and Tsuji6,Reference Thompson9) , over 50 % of non-vegetarian adolescents agreed that meat is ‘natural’, ‘nice’, ‘necessary’ and ‘normal’, while the low meat consumers and vegetarians were more likely to disagree with these rationalisations. Therefore, to be able to shift those with higher levels of meat consumption towards becoming low consumers, addressing these rationalisations will be important.

Moderate/high consumers of meat endorsed that meat was ‘nice’ the most (mean 4·8 (1·3)); a finding similar to that of Piazza et al., (2015), where ‘nice’ was also endorsed the most in male and female adults (mean 5·0 and 4·5, respectively). Further to this, ‘sensory appeal’ was the most important food choice motivation in non-vegetarian adolescents. Adolescents have previously acknowledged that taste was of primary importance when they choose what to eat, while also revealing a general feeling that plant-based foods would not be as tasty because ‘meat has more flavour in it’ as well as it ‘looks like it’ll have more flavour’(Reference Ensaff, Coan and Sahota27). The taste and enjoyment of eating meat is one of the strongest barriers to transitioning to a plant-based diet for many, especially males(Reference Lea and Worsley28,Reference Pohjolainen, Vinnari and Jokinen29) with 23 % of adolescents in a South Australian study(Reference Worsley and Skrzypiec30) reporting they liked the taste of meat too much and had missed it on previous attempts at vegetarianism. The development and promotion of plant-based/meat-free meals that impart some of the same sensory qualities as meat may be needed to address this barrier. This trend is becoming evident in supermarkets and fast-food restaurants, where plant-based ‘meat’ options are increasing in availability(Reference Kalocsay, King and Lichtenstein31).

In contrast, the idea that meat consumption is ‘necessary’ was endorsed the least in non-vegetarian adolescents (mean 4·2 (1·4)), although still prevalent with 51 % agreeing to some extent. As ‘health’ was within the top four food choice motivations and was positively correlated to the ‘necessary’ subscale, this suggests that there may be some concern about potential health effects of eliminating meat from the diet. Concern over nutritional inadequacy has previously been raised in adults as a barrier to implementing a more plant-based diet(Reference Lea, Crawford and Worsley8,Reference Lea and Worsley28,Reference Lea and Worsley32,Reference Reipurth, Hørby and Gregersen33) , specifically in regard to protein and Fe; therefore, it may be that this belief is held by adolescents also. Potential risks to health need to be well understood, especially for a population experiencing growth and puberty (as in adolescence)(Reference Story, Neumark-Sztainer and French34,35) , so that any dietary shift can be promoted safely and with confidence. At present, research investigating the health effects of low meat consumption in adolescents is limited(Reference Gibson and Ashwell36). Subsequent to this, reassurance that meat is not ‘necessary’ may differ dependent on sex, for example, males may want to consume meat for muscle development, whereas females may want to avoid Fe deficiency.

That meat consumption is ‘natural’ and ‘normal’ had similar results (4·5 (1·1) and 4·4 (0·9), respectively) in the moderate/high meat consumers; however, a greater mean difference between moderate/high and low consumers was observed for the ‘natural’ subscale (mean difference 0·7 ‘natural’ v. 0·4 ‘normal’). Meat eaters maintain that it is only natural to eat meat, with the perception that deriving nutrients from meat is part of being a human being(Reference Piazza, Ruby and Loughnan14), a view Joy (2010) believes has come through socialisation, with customs and traditions shaping the habits of many. Furthermore, many consumers maintain meat-eating is an accepted and possibly expected social norm, part of human nature and biology and therefore normal(Reference Joy3,Reference Piazza, Ruby and Loughnan14) . Given this, although low meat eaters were less inclined to agree that eating meat is ‘natural’, their neutral stance on whether it is ‘normal’ or not somewhat agrees with Joy (2010) and Piazza et al. (2015). Yet this may also be due to the increased prevalence of plant-based or vegetarian diets in recent years(Reference Rosenfeld and Burrow37), which are also becoming part of the expected social norm.

Additional food choice motivations such as convenience, price and mood were also rated strongly. Convenience is a factor previously identified as a barrier to implementing a more plant-based diet in adults(Reference Pohjolainen, Vinnari and Jokinen29), with many stating they did not know how to prepare plant-based meals(Reference Lea, Crawford and Worsley8). Others believe it takes too long to prepare plant-based meals(Reference Lea, Crawford and Worsley8) or that meat fits well with their usual dietary intake(Reference de Boer, Schösler and Aiking38). What foods are available in the home as well as who is preparing the meals will likely influence convenience as some adolescents have little say in this(Reference Bassett, Chapman and Beagan39). Plant-based or vegetarian foods have also been perceived as more expensive when compared to meat-based dishes(Reference Lea, Crawford and Worsley8), and therefore cost is a barrier for some consumers. Thus, if plant-based food choices can be marketed as an affordable and convenient option, an increase in purchases may occur.

‘Familiarity’ tended to be positively associated with meat consumption rationalisations. Food neophobia has previously emerged as a barrier to plant-based food choice in adolescents(Reference Ensaff, Coan and Sahota27) as well as in adults(Reference Pohjolainen, Vinnari and Jokinen29), with many expressing a reluctance to try new foods and preference for consuming familiar foods. The possibility of a growing willingness to try new foods could be motivated by adolescents’ increasing autonomy during this life stage or the influence peers can have on adolescent choices(Reference Sturdevant and Spear15,Reference Worsley and Skrzypiec30,Reference Story, Neumark-Sztainer and French34,Reference Whitney, Rolfes and Crowe40) .

Adolescents are known for their unhealthy food choices(Reference Story, Neumark-Sztainer and French34) and although they have a basic understanding of the healthfulness of certain foods, they tend not to worry about the long-term health consequences of their food choices(Reference Share and Stewart-Knox17,Reference MacIntyre, Reilly, Miller and Murcott18,Reference Bissonnette and Contento41) . With current evidence suggesting family factors may positively influence the eating behaviour of adolescents, it is vital that education and interventions promoting the consumption of a more plant-based diet be extended to the families of adolescents(Reference Pearson, Atkin and Biddle42,Reference Rasmussen, Krølner and Klepp43) . In-school education exposing students and their families to a range of foods that are not meat-based alongside education of the nutritional and environmental benefits of a plant-based diet could have a widespread effect. With a focus on how plant-based meals can be prepared and how tasty and accessible plant-based options are, this could be a strategy to encourage positive health and planetary outcomes for generations to come.

While this study collected data from a range of adolescents, the results are limited by the non-representative sample. It is possible that subgroups of the population (e.g. different ethnic groups) might rationalise meat consumption in different ways and this should be considered in the interpretation of this data. Meat consumption was assessed by the Dietary Habits Questionnaire, which assessed frequency of consumption but did not provide quantitative estimates of consumption; however, this level of detail was not necessary for the objectives of this analysis. Despite these limitations, this study provides a set of results that can be used to inform future research and strategies to introduce a plant-based diet.

Conclusion

Most adolescents rationalise their meat consumption because meat tastes good – a large proportion also think that eating meat is necessary, which may reflect concerns about nutritional inadequacies. It therefore seems likely that to facilitate adolescents to transition to a more plant-based eating pattern, we need to find/promote meat alternatives that taste good, are easy to prepare, are cost effective and provide nutritional adequacy.

Acknowledgements

Acknowledgements: The authors would like to thank the University of Otago Master of Dietetics students in 2019 and 2020 who worked as the data collectors on this study. The authors also acknowledge the integral role that Professor Caroline Horwath provided in the study design phase, particularly in regard to measuring meat consumption rationalisations. Financial support: This work was supported by the New Zealand Lottery Health Grant [R-LHR-2019-102520], who had no role in the design, analysis or writing of this article. Conflict of interest: The authors declare no conflicts of interest. Authorship: M.C.P. and J.J.H. designed the study and wrote the protocol. T.S. coordinated the study and oversaw all data collection. K.R.L. conducted literature review and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. J.J.H. undertook all statistical analyses. All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript. Ethics of human subject participation: This study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki, and all procedures involving research study participants were approved by the University of Otago Human Ethics Committee (Health) in February 2019: H19/004 and again in February 2020: H20/004. All participants provided informed electronic consent, and electronic consent was obtained from parents/guardians for those under 16 years of age.

References

Aiking, H (2014) Protein production: planet, profit plus people? Am J Clin Nutr 100, 483S489S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curry, P (2011) Ecological Ethics, 2nd ed. Cambridge: Polity Press.Google Scholar
Joy, M (2010) Why We Love Dogs, Eat Pigs and Wear Cows: An Introduction to Carnism, 1st ed. Berkely, CA: Conari Press, U.S.Google Scholar
Bendinelli, B, Palli, D, Masala, G et al. (2013) Association between dietary meat consumption and incident type 2 diabetes: the EPIC-InterAct study. Diabetologia 56, 4759.Google ScholarPubMed
Bovalino, S, Charleson, G & Szoeke, C (2016) The impact of red and processed meat consumption on cardiovascular disease risk in women. Nutrition 32, 349354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wada, K, Oba, S, Tsuji, M et al. (2017) Meat consumption and colorectal cancer risk in Japan: the Takayama study. Cancer Sci 108, 10651070.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Boer, J & Aiking, H (2011) On the merits of plant-based proteins for global food security: marrying macro and micro perspectives. Ecol Econ 70, 12591265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lea, EJ, Crawford, D & Worsley, A (2006) Public views of the benefits and barriers to the consumption of a plant-based diet. Eur J Clin Nutr 60, 828837.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, R (2015) Red Meat and Bowel Cancer Risk – How Strong Is The Evidence? London: World Cancer Research Fund International; available at https://www.wcrf.org/int/blog/articles/2015/10/red-meat-and-bowel-cancer-risk-how-strong-evidence (accessed November 2020).Google Scholar
Willett, W, Rockström, J, Loken, B et al. (2019) Healthy Diets from Sustainable Food Systems. Food Planet Health. Stockholm: EAT–Lancet Commission.Google Scholar
Grant, JD (2017) Time for change: benefits of a plant-based diet. Can Fam Physician 63, 744746.Google ScholarPubMed
Sallis, JF, Pinski, RB, Grossman, RM et al. (1988) The development of self-efficacy scales for health related diet and exercise behaviors. Health Educ Res 3, 283292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loughnan, S, Haslam, N & Bastian, B (2010) The role of meat consumption in the denial of moral status and mind to meat animals. Appetite 55, 156159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piazza, J, Ruby, MB, Loughnan, S et al. (2015) Rationalizing meat consumption. The 4Ns. Appetite 91, 114128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sturdevant, MS & Spear, BA (2002) Adolescent psychosocial development. J Am Diet Assoc 102, S30S31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mikkilä, V, Räsänen, L, Raitakari, OT et al. (2005) Consistent dietary patterns identified from childhood to adulthood: the cardiovascular risk in young Finns study. Br J Nutr 93, 923931.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Share, M & Stewart-Knox, B (2012) Determinants of food choice in Irish adolescents. Food Qual Prefer 25, 5762.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacIntyre, S, Reilly, J, Miller, D et al. (1998) Food choice, food scares and health: the role of the media. In The Nation’s Diet: The Social Science of Food Choice, pp. 122 [Murcott, A, editor]. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Peddie, M, Ranasinghe, C, Scott, T et al. (2020) Dietary intake nutritional status and lifestyle of adolescent vegetarian and nonvegetarian girls in New Zealand (the SuNDiAL project): protocol for a clustered, cross-sectional survey. JMIR Res Protoc 9, e17310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, PA, Taylor, R, Thielke, R et al. (2009) Research electronic data capture (REDCap) – a metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing translational research informatics support. J Biomed Inform 42, 377381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atkinson, J, Salmond, C & Crampton, P (2019) NZDep2018 Index of Deprivation, Interim Research Report. Wellington: University of Otago.Google Scholar
de Onis, M (2007) Development of a WHO growth reference for school-aged children and adolescents. Bull World Health Organ 85, 660667.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
University of Otago & Ministry of Health (2011) Methodology Report for the 2008/09 New Zealand Adult Nutrition Survey. Wellington: Ministry of Health.Google Scholar
Steptoe, A, Pollard, TM & Wardle, J (1995) Development of a measure of the motives underlying the selection of food: the food choice questionnaire. Appetite 25, 267284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindeman, M & Väänänen, M (2000) Measurement of ethical food choice motives. Appetite 34, 5559.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markovina, J, Stewart-Knox, BJ, Rankin, A et al. (2015) Food4Me study: validity and reliability of Food Choice Questionnaire in 9 European countries. Food Qual Prefer 45, 2632.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ensaff, H, Coan, S, Sahota, P et al. (2015) Adolescents’ food choice and the place of plant-based foods. Nutrients 7, 46194637.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lea, E & Worsley, A (2003) Benefits and barriers to the consumption of a vegetarian diet in Australia. Public Health Nutr 6, 505511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pohjolainen, P, Vinnari, M & Jokinen, P (2015) Consumers’ perceived barriers to following a plant-based diet. Br Food J 117, 11501167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Worsley, A & Skrzypiec, G (1998) Teenage vegetarianism: prevalence, social and cognitive contexts. Appetite 30, 151170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kalocsay, K, King, T, Lichtenstein, T et al. (2020) Plant-Based Meat: A Healthier Choice? Melbourne: Food Frontier.Google Scholar
Lea, E & Worsley, A (2001) Influences on meat consumption in Australia. Appetite 36, 127136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reipurth, MFS, Hørby, L, Gregersen, CG et al. (2019) Barriers and facilitators towards adopting a more plant-based diet in a sample of Danish consumers. Food Qual Prefer 73, 288292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Story, M, Neumark-Sztainer, D & French, S (2002) Individual and environmental influences on adolescent eating behaviors. J Am Diet Assoc 102, S40S51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organisation (2020) Adolescent Health and Development. https://www.who.int/news-room/q-a-detail/adolescent-health-and-development (accessed November 2020).Google Scholar
Gibson, S & Ashwell, M (2004) Implications of low red meat consumption for iron status of young people in Britain. Nutr Food Sci 34, 253259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenfeld, DL & Burrow, AL (2017) Vegetarian on purpose: understanding the motivations of plant-based dieters. Appetite 116, 456463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Boer, J, Schösler, H & Aiking, H (2017) Towards a reduced meat diet: mindset and motivation of young vegetarians, low, medium and high meat-eaters. Appetite 113, 387397.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bassett, R, Chapman, GE & Beagan, BL (2008) Autonomy and control: the co-construction of adolescent food choice. Appetite 50, 325332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitney, E, Rolfes, S, Crowe, T et al. (2013) Understanding Nutrition, 2nd ed. Australia: Cengage Learning.Google Scholar
Bissonnette, MM & Contento, IR (2001) Adolescents’ perspectives and food choice behaviors in terms of the environmental impacts of food production practices: application of a psychosocial model. J Nutr Educ Behav 33, 72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pearson, N, Atkin, AJ, Biddle, SJH et al. (2009) Parenting styles, family structure and adolescent dietary behaviour. Public Health Nutr 13, 12451253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rasmussen, M, Krølner, R, Klepp, K-I et al. (2006) Determinants of fruit and vegetable consumption among children and adolescents: a review of the literature. Part I: quantitative studies. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 3, 22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Demographic and anthropometric characteristics of all participants (n 385)

Figure 1

Table 2 Demographic and anthropometric predictors of meat consumption rationalisation in non-vegetarians (n 337)

Figure 2

Fig. 1 Box plots of meat rationalisation scores by self-identified vegetarian status (n 337 non-vegetarians and n 48 vegetarians). Each subscale is scored using a seven-point scale from strongly disagree to strongly agree about eating meat so that a score of 4 corresponded to ‘neither agree nor disagree’ (indicated by the dashed line)

Figure 3

Table 3 Differences in meat consumption rationalisation by meat consumption patterns* (n 377)

Figure 4

Table 4 Correlations* between food choice motivations† and rationalisation of meat consumption in non-vegetarians (n 337)