Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-xbtfd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-18T16:16:46.172Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Detecting cognitive decline in high-functioning older adults: The relationship between subjective cognitive concerns, frequency of high neuropsychological test scores, and the frontoparietal control network

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2023

Justin E. Karr*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY, USA
Jonathan G. Hakun
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, College of Medicine, The Pennsylvania State University, Hershey, PA, USA Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, State College, PA, USA
Daniel B. Elbich
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, College of Medicine, The Pennsylvania State University, Hershey, PA, USA
Cristina N. Pinheiro
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY, USA
Frederick A. Schmitt
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY, USA Department of Neurology, College of Medicine, University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY, USA Sanders-Brown Center on Aging, College of Medicine, University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY, USA
Suzanne C. Segerstrom
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Kentucky, Lexington, KY, USA
*
Corresponding author: Justin E. Karr; Email: jkarr@uky.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

Neuropsychologists have difficulty detecting cognitive decline in high-functioning older adults because greater neurological change must occur before cognitive performances are low enough to indicate decline or impairment. For high-functioning older adults, early neurological changes may correspond with subjective cognitive concerns and an absence of high scores. This study compared high-functioning older adults with and without subjective cognitive concerns, hypothesizing those with cognitive concerns would have fewer high scores on neuropsychological testing and lower frontoparietal network volume, thickness, and connectivity.

Method:

Participants had high estimated premorbid functioning (e.g., estimated intelligence ≥75th percentile or college-educated) and were divided based on subjective cognitive concerns. Participants with cognitive concerns (n = 35; 74.0 ± 9.6 years old, 62.9% female, 94.3% White) and without cognitive concerns (n = 33; 71.2 ± 7.1 years old, 75.8% female, 100% White) completed a neuropsychological battery of memory and executive function tests and underwent structural and resting-state magnetic resonance imaging, calculating frontoparietal network volume, thickness, and connectivity.

Results:

Participants with and without cognitive concerns had comparable numbers of low test scores (≤16th percentile), p = .103, d = .40. Participants with cognitive concerns had fewer high scores (≥75th percentile), p = .004, d = .71, and lower mean frontoparietal network volumes (left: p = .004, d = .74; right: p = .011, d = .66) and cortical thickness (left: p = .010, d = .66; right: p = .033, d = .54), but did not differ in network connectivity.

Conclusions:

Among high-functioning older adults, subjective cognitive decline may correspond with an absence of high scores on neuropsychological testing and underlying changes in the frontoparietal network that would not be detected by a traditional focus on low cognitive test scores.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © INS. Published by Cambridge University Press 2023

Introduction

Neuropsychologists often have difficulty detecting cognitive decline in older adults with high premorbid cognitive functioning because more neurological change must occur before cognitive test scores meet conventional criteria for defining mild or major cognitive impairment (Albert et al., Reference Albert, DeKosky, Dickson, Dubois, Feldman, Fox, Gamst, Holtzman, Jagust, Petersen, Snyder, Carrillo, Thies and Phelps2011; American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Petersen et al., Reference Petersen, Smith, Waring, Ivnik, Tangalos and Kokmen1999). Current assessment methods and approaches to test interpretation are inherently limited in detecting potential cognitive decline in high-functioning examinees. Low scores have long been the standard for defining cognitive impairment in neuropsychological practice (Dubois et al., Reference Dubois, Feldman, Jacova, DeKosky, Barberger-Gateau, Cummings, Delacourte, Galasko, Gauthier, Jicha, Meguro, OʼBrien, Pasquier, Robert, Rossor, Salloway, Stern, Visser and Scheltens2007; Heaton et al., Reference Heaton, Grant and Matthews1991; Heaton et al., Reference Heaton, Miller, Taylor and Grant2004; Petersen et al., Reference Petersen, Smith, Waring, Ivnik, Tangalos and Kokmen1999; Reitan & Wolfson, Reference Reitan and Wolfson1993), but in high-functioning older adults, decline from a high average or superior premorbid ability level may be present without any low scores on cognitive testing. In a longitudinal cohort of 204 high-functioning older adults, even average scores were predictive of dementia at a follow-up evaluation (Tuokko et al., Reference Tuokko, Garrett, McDowell, Silverberg and Kristjansson2003). In high-functioning individuals undergoing neuropsychological assessments, the absence of high scores, as opposed to the presence of low scores, may indicate cognitive decline and could correspond with an underlying disease process.

Prior research has extensively examined the normal frequency of an examinee obtaining one or more low test scores when administered a battery of neuropsychological tests, both across domains (Binder et al., Reference Binder, Iverson and Brooks2009; Brooks et al., Reference Brooks, Iverson, Holdnack, Holdnack, Drozdick, Weiss and Iverson2013; Brooks et al., Reference Brooks, Iverson and White2009; Mistridis et al., Reference Mistridis, Egli, Iverson, Berres, Willmes, Welsh-Bohmer and Monsch2015) and within specific domains, such as memory (Brooks et al., Reference Brooks, Iverson, Holdnack and Feldman2008; Brooks et al., Reference Brooks, Iverson and White2007) and executive functions (Karr et al., Reference Karr, Garcia-Barrera, Holdnack and Iverson2017, Reference Karr, Garcia-Barrera, Holdnack and Iverson2018). Additional research has examined the other side of the bell curve, demonstrating that high scores are also commonly obtained by healthy examinees completing neuropsychological test batteries (Karr et al., Reference Karr, Garcia-Barrera, Holdnack and Iverson2020; Karr et al., Reference Karr, Rivera Mindt and Iverson2022a; Karr & Iverson, Reference Karr and Iverson2020). For example, roughly half of adults (i.e., 48.9%) in the normative sample for the NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (NIHTB-CB) obtained one or more scores ≥84th percentile (Karr & Iverson, Reference Karr and Iverson2020). Not surprisingly, individuals with higher estimated intelligence tend to obtain fewer low scores and more high scores (Iverson & Karr, Reference Iverson and Karr2021), meaning a very low base rate of high-functioning adults with no high scores on neuropsychological testing. For example, just 4.8% of healthy adults with high average intelligence obtained no test scores ≥75th percentile when interpreting seven scores from three tests on the Delis–Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS) (Karr et al., Reference Karr, Garcia-Barrera, Holdnack and Iverson2020). As such, it would be clinically informative to assess whether fewer high scores on cognitive testing is associated with subjective cognitive concerns and underlying neurological differences.

Participants with subjective cognitive concerns present with subtly lower performances on cognitive testing (Burmester et al., Reference Burmester, Leathem and Merrick2016), and some of the largest cognitive effects of preclinical Alzheimer’s disease occur within the domains of episodic memory and executive functions (Bäckman et al., Reference Bäckman, Jones, Berger, Laukka and Small2005). The frontoparietal control network (FPCN) (Yeo et al., Reference Thomas Yeo, Krienen, Sepulcre, Sabuncu, Lashkari, Hollinshead, Roffman, Smoller, Zöllei, Polimeni, Fischl, Liu and Buckner2011) is involved in aspects of executive functions and memory (Badre & D’Esposito, Reference Badre and D’Esposito2007; Cabeza et al., Reference Cabeza, Ciaramelli, Olson and Moscovitch2008; Spreng et al., Reference Spreng, Stevens, Chamberlain, Gilmore and Schacter2010; Vincent et al., Reference Vincent, Kahn, Snyder, Raichle and Buckner2008), and structural changes in frontal and parietal regions have been consistently observed in subjective cognitive decline (Rivas-Fernández et al., Reference Rivas-Fernández, Lindín, Zurrón, Díaz, Lojo-Seoane, Pereiro and Galdo-Álvarez2023). FPCN connectivity has predicted longitudinal changes in global cognition among healthy older adults (Buckley et al., Reference Buckley, Schultz, Hedden, Papp, Hanseeuw, Marshall, Sepulcre, Smith, Rentz, Johnson, Sperling and Chhatwal2017) and FPCN volume mediates the relationship between age and executive functions in healthy adults (Yao et al., Reference Yao, Yang, Hwang and Hsieh2020). Participants with and without subjective cognitive concerns may present with differences in FPCN volume and connectivity, potentially indicating underlying neurological changes that could lead to mild cognitive impairment or dementia.

For high-functioning older adults, subjective cognitive concerns may indicate decline that is not detected by the presence of low scores but may be related to the absence of high scores and the presence of latent neurological changes. The current study examined whether subjective cognitive concerns were associated with (a) the number of low and high scores on neuropsychological tests of memory and executive functions, and (b) the regional volume, cortical thickness, and connectivity of the FPCN. We hypothesized that high-functioning older adults with subjective cognitive concerns would have fewer high scores than those without subjective cognitive concerns, but a comparable number of low scores. These findings would indicate that objective decline has occurred but would not be detected by a traditional focus on low scores. We also hypothesized that subjective cognitive concerns would be associated with lower FPCN volume, thickness, and connectivity, indicating latent neurological change underlying subjective cognitive concerns.

Method

Participants

Participants were derived from an imaging sub-study of a longitudinal cohort study on self-regulation, brain, and cognitive health in older adults (D. R. Evans & Segerstrom, Reference Evans and Segerstrom2015; Geiger et al., Reference Geiger, Reed, Combs, Boggero and Segerstrom2019; Scott et al., Reference Scott, Reed, Garcia-Willingham, Lawrence and Segerstrom2019; Segerstrom et al., Reference Segerstrom, Reed and Karr2022). To be eligible, participants had to be 60 years or older and nonsmokers. They were excluded if they had autoimmune diseases; were taking opiates, corticosteroids, cytotoxic drugs, TNF blockers, or medications for dementia; received chemotherapy or radiation in the past 5 years or general anesthesia in the past 3 months; or were taking more than two of the following medication: α or β blockers or ACE inhibitors, hormone replacement, thyroid supplement, and antidepressant, anxiolytic, or hypnotic drugs. Overall, participants included in the study were healthy older adults. Apolipoprotein E genotype was not available for individual participants. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board at the University of Kentucky and completed in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration.

Among 80 participants who underwent magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), participants were selected if they were estimated to have high premorbid functioning, operationally defined as either (a) scoring ≥75th percentile on the North American Adult Reading Test (NAART) (Blair & Spreen, Reference Blair and Spreen1989; Uttl, Reference Uttl2002) estimated full scale intelligence quotient (FSIQ) (n = 60) or (b) having completed a postsecondary degree (n = 62). There was a strong correspondence between these variables. Most participants scoring ≥75th percentile on the NAART had a college degree (n = 54, 90.0%) and most participants with a college degree scored ≥75th percentile on the NAART (n = 54, 87.1%). This resulted in a sample of 68 participants, who were further subdivided based on the presence or absence of subjective cognitive concerns, defined per self-report on the Medical Outcomes Study Cognitive Functioning Scale (MOS-Cog, with exact methodology described below). The demographic characteristics of the total sample and participants with subjective cognitive concerns (n = 35) and without subjective cognitive concerns (n = 33) are presented in Table 1. There were no significant differences between groups in terms of age, sex, race, education, household income, or NAART estimated FSIQ. The participants, by design, differed significantly on subjective cognitive concerns.

Table 1. Participant demographics

Note. CI = Confidence Interval; MOS-Cog = Medical Outcomes Study Cognitive Functioning Scale; NAART FSIQ = North American Adult Reading Test estimated full scale intelligence quotient.

Measures

Subjective cognitive concerns

Participants completed the MOS-Cog (Stewart et al., Reference Stewart, Ware, Sherbourne, Wells, Stewart and Ware1992), a six-item questionnaire asking about past-month difficulty with general cognitive functions in everyday life (e.g., concentration, memory, problem solving). Participants responded to each item on a six-point scale, ranging from all of the time (1) to none of the time (6). The items were converted to 0–100 scale (i.e., 1 = 0, 2 = 20, 3 = 40, 4 = 60, 5 = 80, and 6 = 100) and averaged to arrive at a total score for the MOS-Cog (range: 0–100), with a higher score indicating fewer cognitive concerns. Participants were categorized as having or not having subjective cognitive concerns based on responses to MOS-Cog items. If participants endorsed one or more MOS-Cog items as some of the time (4), a good bit of the time (3), most of the time (2), or all of the time (1), they were categorized as having subjective cognitive concerns; and those who responded a little of the time (5) or none of the time (6) to all items were categorized as not having subjective cognitive concerns.

Neuropsychological tests

Participants completed the NAART (Blair & Spreen, Reference Blair and Spreen1989; Uttl, Reference Uttl2002), which is a word reading test used to estimate FSIQ; the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) (Strauss et al., Reference Strauss, Sherman and Spreen2006), with total learning for Trials 1–5 and delayed recall included as scores in the base rates analysis; the Trail Making Test (TMT) Parts A and B (Bowie & Harvey, Reference Bowie and Harvey2006; Reitan, Reference Reitan1958), with time-to-completion for each part included as scores in the base rates analysis; the Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT) (Benton et al., Reference Benton, Hamsher and Sivan1994), with the number of words produced across three trials included as scores in the base rates analysis; and the Digit Span (DS) and Letter-Number Sequencing (LNS) subtests from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Fourth edition (WAIS-IV) (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2008), with the total scores for these subtests included in the base rates analysis. Age-adjusted scaled scores were derived using Mayo’s Older Americans Normative Studies (MOANS) norms for the RAVLT (Steinberg et al., Reference Steinberg, Bieliauskas, Smith, Ivnik and Malec2005), TMT Parts A and B (Steinberg et al., Reference Steinberg, Bieliauskas, Smith and Ivnik2005), and COWAT (Steinberg et al., Reference Steinberg, Bieliauskas, Smith and Ivnik2005) and using WAIS-IV norms for the DS and LNS (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2008). For all test scores, a score at or above the 75th percentile was considered a high score based on uniform labeling standards for performance test scores (Guilmette et al., Reference Guilmette, Sweet, Hebben, Koltai, Mahone, Spiegler, Stucky and Westerveld2020); and a score at or below the 16th percentile was considered a low score, based on recommended criteria for cognitive impairment (American Psychiatric Association, 2013).

Physical and mental health

Participants had heart rate, blood pressure, and Body Mass Index (BMI) measured. Participants completed the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS), which is a 30-item questionnaire on past-week depression symptomatology (Yesavage et al., Reference Yesavage, Brink, Rose, Lum, Huang, Adey and Leirer1982). A higher scores indicates more severe depression and scores >10 indicate depression in older adults (Brink et al., Reference Brink, Yesavage, Lum, Heersema, Adey and Rose1982). Participants also completed a 10-item version of the Perceived Stress Scale (S. Cohen et al., Reference Cohen, Kamarck and Mermelstein1983), which measures degrees of life stress in the past month. A higher score indicates a greater degree of life stress.

Imaging data acquisition

Participants were scanned using a 3-T Siemens TIM Trio scanner using an 8-channel array head coil between July 2015 and September 2017. A 3-T Siemens PRISMA scanner with a 20-channel array head coil was used between July 2018 and May 2019, with 38 of 64 participants scanned with the upgraded scanner. Structural and functional images were collected, on average, about two months after neuropsychological tests were completed (M = 60.0 days, SD = 45.8; Mdn = 46, range: 4–244). High-resolution T1-weighted images were collected using a magnetization-prepared rapid gradient-echo (MPRAGE) sequence (TR = 2530 ms; TE = 2.26 ms; FA = 7 degrees; resolution = 1 mm isotropic). Functional images were collected using a T2*-weighted gradient-echo planar sequence (34 interleaved slices, TR = 2000 ms, TE = 27 ms, FA = 70, FOV = 224 mm2, matrix = 64×64, isotropic resolution = 3.5 mm).

Anatomical data preprocessing

The T1-weighted (T1w) image was corrected for intensity nonuniformity (INU) with N4BiasFieldCorrection (Tustison et al., Reference Tustison, Avants, Cook, Zheng, Egan, Yushkevich and Gee2010), distributed with Advanced Normalization Tools (ANTs) 2.2.0 (RRID:SCR_004757) (Avants et al., Reference Avants, Tustison, Song, Cook, Klein and Gee2011; Tustison et al., Reference Tustison, Cook, Holbrook, Johnson, Muschelli, Devenyi, Duda, Das, Cullen, Gillen, Yassa, Stone, Gee and Avants2021) and used as T1w reference. The T1w reference was skull-stripped with a Nipype implementation of the antsBrainExtraction.sh workflow from ANTs, using OASIS30ANTs as target template. Brain tissue segmentation of cerebrospinal fluid, white matter, and gray matter was performed on the brain-extracted T1w using fast (FSL 5.0.9, RRID:SCR_002823) (Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Brady and Smith2001). Brain surfaces were reconstructed using recon-all (FreeSurfer 6.0.1, RRID:SCR_001847) (Dale et al., Reference Dale, Fischl and Sereno1999), and the brain mask estimated previously was refined with a custom variation of the method to reconcile ANTs-derived and FreeSurfer-derived segmentations of the cortical gray matter of Mindboggle (RRID:SCR_002438) (Klein et al., Reference Klein, Ghosh, Bao, Giard, Häme, Stavsky, Lee, Rossa, Reuter, Chaibub Neto, Keshavan and Schneidman2017). Volume-based spatial normalization to one standard space (MNI152NLin6Asym) was performed through nonlinear registration with antsRegistration (ANTs 2.2.0), using brain-extracted versions of both T1w reference and the T1w template. The following template was selected for spatial normalization: FSL\u2019s MNI ICBM 152 nonlinear 6th Generation Asymmetric Average Brain Stereotaxic Registration Model (RRID:SCR_002823; TemplateFlow ID: MNI152NLin6Asym) (A. C. Evans et al., Reference Evans, Janke, Collins and Baillet2012).

Resting-state preprocessing

Functional data were preprocessed using fMRIPrep 1.5.3 (Esteban, Markiewicz, et al., (2018); Esteban, Blair, et al., (2018); RRID:SCR_016216) (Esteban et al., Reference Esteban, Markiewicz, Blair, Moodie, Isik, Erramuzpe, Kent, Goncalves, DuPre, Snyder, Oya, Ghosh, Wright, Durnez, Poldrack and Gorgolewski2019, Reference Esteban, Ciric, Finc, Blair, Markiewicz, Moodie, Kent, Goncalves, DuPre, Gomez, Ye, Salo, Valabregue, Amlien, Liem, Jacoby, Stojić, Cieslak, Urchs and Gorgolewski2020) based on Nipype 1.3.1 (RRID:SCR_002502) (Gorgolewski et al., Reference Gorgolewski, Burns, Madison, Clark, Halchenko, Waskom and Ghosh2011). A reference volume and its skull-stripped version were generated using a custom fMRIPrep methodology. Susceptibility distortion correction (SDC) was omitted. The BOLD reference was then co-registered to the T1w reference using bbregister (FreeSurfer) which implements boundary-based registration (Greve & Fischl, Reference Greve and Fischl2009). Co-registration was configured with six degrees of freedom. Head motion parameters with respect to the BOLD reference (transformation matrices, and six corresponding rotation and translation parameters) are estimated before any spatiotemporal filtering using mcflirt (FSL 5.0.9) (Jenkinson et al., Reference Jenkinson, Bannister, Brady and Smith2002). The BOLD time series were resampled to surfaces in FreeSurfer (fsaverage5) space. The BOLD time series (including slice-timing correction when applied) were resampled onto their native space by applying the transforms to correct for head motion. These resampled BOLD time series are referred to as preprocessed BOLD.

The BOLD time series were resampled into standard space, generating a preprocessed BOLD run in MNI-152 space. A reference volume and its skull-stripped version were generated using a custom fMRIPrep methodology. Automatic removal of motion artifacts using ICA-AROMA (Pruim et al., Reference Pruim, Mennes, van Rooij, Llera, Buitelaar and Beckmann2015) was performed on the preprocessed BOLD on MNI space time series after removal of non-steady state volumes and spatial smoothing with an isotropic, Gaussian kernel of 6 mm FWHM. Corresponding non-aggressively denoised runs were produced after such smoothing. Additionally, the aggressive noise regressors were collected and placed in the corresponding confounds file. Several confounding time series were calculated based on the preprocessed BOLD: FD, DVARS, and three region-wise global signals. FD and DVARS are calculated for each functional run, both using their implementations in Nipype (Power et al., Reference Power, Mitra, Laumann, Snyder, Schlaggar and Petersen2014). The three global signals are extracted within the cerebrospinal fluid, white matter, and whole-brain masks. Gridded resamplings were performed using ANTs, configured with Lanczos interpolation to minimize the smoothing effects of other kernels (Lanczos, Reference Lanczos1964). Non-gridded resamplings were performed using mri_vol2surf (FreeSurfer).

Resting-state time-series parcellation and analysis

The resulting images from ICA-AROMA run via fMRIPrep were further corrected by regressing out global signals cerebrospinal fluid and white matter, as well as a linear trend using fsl_regfilt. Subsequently, the data was also bandpass filtered between 0.01 and 0.1 Hz (Ciric et al., Reference Ciric, Wolf, Power, Roalf, Baum, Ruparel, Shinohara, Elliott, Eickhoff, Davatzikos, Gur, Gur, Bassett and Satterthwaite2017). To construct resting-state connectivity matrices, the 7 Network 400 parcel variant was used (Schaefer et al., Reference Schaefer, Kong, Gordon, Laumann, Zuo, Holmes, Eickhoff and Yeo2018), derived from a well-known atlas of resting networks (Yeo et al., Reference Thomas Yeo, Krienen, Sepulcre, Sabuncu, Lashkari, Hollinshead, Roffman, Smoller, Zöllei, Polimeni, Fischl, Liu and Buckner2011). The MNI registered atlas was used as a mask from which to extract time series from each of the 400 individual regions. All time series were correlated with one another to construct the final matrix. Time-series correlations for regions falling within the FPCN (presented in Figure 1) were averaged to generate a single value representing the connectivity between all regions in the network.

Figure 1. Frontoparietal control network parcellation used in the current study.

Anatomical parcellation & analysis

The anatomical scans for all participants were preprocessed using FreeSurfer. Using these transformations and the Schaefer atlas registrations in FreeSurfer space, the atlas was back projected onto the MPRAGE. Cortical thickness and volume were extracted using the FPCN as a single region of interest.

Statistical analyses

There was a minimal missing cognitive test data (i.e., 1.5–4.4% missingness per variable), with a nonsignificant Little’s test (Little, Reference Little1988), χ 2 (31) = 22.07, p = .881. Missing neuropsychological test data were imputed using an estimation–maximization method (Enders, Reference Enders2010), because complete testing data was required in order to calculate the counts of low and high test scores for the full battery. Seven norm-referenced scaled scores (M = 10, SD = 3) were derived from the RAVLT, TMT Parts A and B, COWAT, and WAIS-IV DS and LNS subtests. The individual neuropsychological test scores, number of low scores (i.e., ≤16th percentile), the number of high scores (i.e., ≥75th percentile), and the three neuroimaging parameters (i.e., bilateral FPCN volume adjusted for intracranial volume, cortical thickness, and connectivity) were compared between participants with and without subjective cognitive concerns using t tests, with Cohen’s d reported as a corresponding effect size (Cohen, Reference Cohen1988). The continuous MOS-Cog was also correlated with the count of low and high scores and the neuroimaging parameters in the full sample. Sensitivity analysis (Erdfelder et al., Reference Erdfelder, Faul, Buchner and Lang2009) indicated that the sample had sufficient power (1 − β ≤ .80) to detect a roughly large group difference (d ≥ .70) and a medium correlation (r ≥ .33).

Post hoc analyses included (a) an examination of differences in regional volume, thickness, and connectivity of the default mode network (DMN) to determine the specificity of group differences in the FPCN; (b) an evaluation of covariates (i.e., age, sex, physical and mental health variables) with low and high score counts and neuroimaging variables and analyses of covariance controlling for variables related to dependent variables of interest; (c) a comparison of FPCN volume, thickness, and connectivity by scanner upgrade (i.e., 8-channel versus 20-channel); and (d) a comparison of groups on alternative metrics for aggregating neuropsychological test performances, including an intraindividual standard deviation (ISD) as an estimate of intraindividual variability in test performances and the frequency of below average test performances (i.e., <50th percentile).

Results

Cognitive test performances

Participants with and without subjective cognitive concerns were compared on individual neuropsychological test performances (Table 2). Participants with subjective cognitive concerns performed significantly lower on only the COWAT and TMT Part A. A small portion of participants obtained one or more low neuropsychological test scores (i.e., 17.6%) and very few obtained two or more low scores (i.e., 4.4%). Participants without subjective cognitive concerns obtained a similar number of low scores as participants with subjective cognitive concerns (Table 3). Nearly all participants obtained one or more high neuropsychological test scores (i.e., 94.1%), with more than half obtaining four or more high scores (i.e., 58.8%). Participants without subjective cognitive concerns obtained a significantly greater number of high scores compared to participants with subjective cognitive concerns (Table 3). When using the MOS-Cog as a continuous variable, there were no significant associations between subjective cognitive concerns and the number of low scores, r = −.04, p = .720, or high scores, r = .19, p = .123.

Table 2. Mean performances on individual neuropsychological tests

Note. All scores reflect scaled scores (M = 10, SD = 3). CI = Confidence Interval; COWAT = Controlled Oral Word Association Test; LNS = Letter-Number Sequencing; RAVLT = Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test; WAIS-IV = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Fourth edition; TMT = Trail Making Test.

Table 3. Comparison of high-functioning participants with and without subjective cognitive concerns on number of low and high scores on neuropsychological testing

Note. CI = Confidence Interval.

Neuroimaging

Participants were compared on FPCN regional volume, cortical thickness, and connectivity (Table 4). Participants with subjective cognitive concerns had bilateral lower mean volume and cortical thickness of the FPCN, but did not differ from those without subjective cognitive concerns in network connectivity. None of the neuroimaging variables significantly correlated with the number of high or low scores. Larger FPCN volume significantly correlated with fewer cognitive concerns for the full sample (left: r = .32, p = .009; right: r = .35, p = .005). Higher thickness corresponded with fewer cognitive concerns, although the correlations were not statistically significant (left: r = .24, p = .054; right: r = .21, p = .092). Connectivity did not correlate with cognitive concerns (r = −.07, p = .583).

Table 4. Comparison of high-functioning participants with and without subjective cognitive concerns on volume, thickness, and connectivity of the frontoparietal control and default mode networks

Note. CI = Confidence Interval; n = 32 per group due to three participants with subjective cognitive concerns missing. For regional volumes, units were mm3, standardized by intracranial volume; for cortical thickness, units were mm; and for stationary connectivity, units were mean r value.

Post hoc analyses

DMN comparisons

To examine the specificity of FPCN differences, participants with and without subjective cognitive concerns were compared on cortical thickness, regional volume, and connectivity of the DMN (Table 4). There were no significant group differences on any neuroimaging variables, albeit right regional volume and bilateral cortical thickness were associated with approximately medium effect sizes (d range: .47–.49) that approached significance (p range: .055–.060).

Examination of covariates

Age was unrelated to the number of low scores, r = −.08, p = .540, and the number of high scores, r = .01, p = .964; and sex was unrelated to the number of low scores, t = .21, p = .832, d = .06 [95% Confidence Interval: −.46, .57], and the number of high scores, t = .97, p = .334, d = .26 [−.26, .77]. Age was related to bilateral FPCN cortical thickness (left: r = −.30, p = .015; right: r = −.30, p = .016) and volume (left: r = −.38, p = .002; right: r = −.41, p < .001), but not connectivity (r = −.03, p = .835). Sex was related to only left FPCN volume, t = 2.24, p = .029, d = .61 [.06, 1.16], with female participants having higher volume than male participants; but there were no group differences in cortical thickness (left: t = 1.09, p = .281, d = .30 [−.24, .84]; right: t = .63, p = .532, d = .17 [−.37, .71]), right volume (t = 1.54, p = .128, d = .42 [−.12, .96]), or connectivity (t = 1.58, p = .119, d = .43 [−.11, .97]).

Participants with and without subjective cognitive concerns were compared on physical and mental health variables (Table 5). Participants with and without subjective cognitive concerns did not differ in terms of BMI, heart rate, systolic or diastolic blood pressure, or depression. Based on GDS cutoff (i.e., >10), 25% of participants without subjective cognitive concerns and 37.5% of participant with subjective cognitive concerns reported at least mild depression. The groups differed on the PSS, associated with a medium-to-large effect size (d = .71). As a continuous variable, the MOS-Cog was significantly correlated with the PSS (r = −.30, p = .015), but not the GDS (r = −.10, p = .435). These variables were also examined as correlates of low and high score frequencies and FPCN neuroimaging variables. Higher depression scores correlated with fewer high neuropsychological test scores, r = −.34, p = .007, and more low scores, r = .27, p = .029, but no other physical or mental health variables significantly correlated with high or low score counts. Perceived stress and depression were not correlated with any FPCN neuroimaging variables. Greater BMI correlated with increased resting-state FPCN connectivity, r = .29, p = .021, and reduced FPCN volume in both hemispheres (i.e., left: r = −.30, p = .016; right: r = −.29, p = .019). The only other significant correlation indicated higher diastolic blood pressure was associated with greater FPCN cortical thickness in the right hemisphere, r = .31, p = .014.

Table 5. Comparison of high-functioning participants with and without subjective cognitive concerns on physical and mental health variables

Note. CI = Confidence Interval; n = 32 per group due to three participants with subjective cognitive concerns missing; For the Perceived Stress Scale and Geriatric Depression Scale, Levene’s test of equality of variance was significant (p < .05) and equal variance was not assumed.

Per these analyses, a series of analyses of covariance were conducted controlling for covariates that were related to either subjective cognitive concerns or the dependent variable of interest. Controlling for PSS and GDS, participants with and without subjective cognitive concerns significantly differed in their number of high scores, F = 6.75, p = .012, η p 2 = .10, but not number of low scores, F = 2.26, p = .138, η p 2 = .04. Controlling for age, sex, and BMI, the groups differed in FPCN volume in the left hemisphere, F = 4.70, p = .034, η p 2 = .07, but not the right hemisphere, F = 3.37, p = .072, η p 2 = .05. Controlling for age and systolic and diastolic blood pressure, participants differed in FPCN cortical thickness in the left hemisphere, F = 4.41, p = .040, η p 2 = .07, but not the right hemisphere, F = 3.57, p = .064, η p 2 = .06. Controlling for BMI, groups did not differ in FPCN connectivity, F = .52, p = .474, η p 2 = .01.

Scanner upgrade

The scanner array head coil was upgraded from 8-channel to 20-channel during data collection. Post hoc analyses examined differences based on scanner. The groups were compared on FPCN variables, including volume (left: t = .30, p = .769; right: t = 1.26, p = .211), thickness (left: t = .35, p = .732; right: t = .06, p = .952), and connectivity (t = 1.50, p = .140), collectively indicating no group differences related to the scanner upgrade.

Alternative methods of neuropsychological test interpretation

To examine whether differences in high and low scores were attributable to differences in performance variability across neuropsychological tests, participants were compared on their ISD. The mean ISD for participants with subjective cognitive concerns were essentially identical (M = 2.4, SD = 0.7 for both groups) and did not significantly differ, t = .11, p = .913, d = .03 [−.45, .50]. Participants were also compared on number of scores<50th percentile, with results reported in Table 3. Participants with subjective cognitive concerns obtained more scores<50th percentile than participants without subjective cognitive concerns.

Discussion

This study compared high-functioning older adults (i.e., estimated FSIQ ≥75th percentile or college-educated) with and without subjective cognitive concerns on the number of low scores and high scores obtained on a seven-test neuropsychological battery and FPCN regional volume, cortical thickness, and connectivity. Whereas no difference was observed between groups in the number of low scores, participants with subjective cognitive concerns had fewer high scores (M = 3.1, SD = 2.0; Mdn = 3, range: 0–7) than those without subjective cognitive concerns (M = 4.4, SD = 1.5; Mdn = 5, range: 2–7), with a large effect size (d = .71 [95% CI: .22, 1.20]). Post hoc analyses indicated a large group difference in counts of scores<50th percentile as well (d = .84 [.34, 1.32]), with participants with subjective cognitive concerns (M = 1.8, SD = 1.5; Mdn = 2, range: 0–6) again having more scores below this cutoff than participants without subjective cognitive concerns (M = 0.8, SD = 0.8; Mdn = 1, range: 0–3). These differences in test performances were not attributable to intraindividual variability, which appears related to cognitive aging (Hultsch et al., Reference Hultsch, Strauss, Hunter, MacDonald, Craik and Salthouse2008), but did not differ between groups in the current sample. Participants with subjective cognitive concerns also had lower bilateral FPCN volume and cortical thickness, with medium-to-large effect sizes (d range: .54–.74). Collectively, these findings indicate that high-functioning older adults who report subjective cognitive concerns (a) may be experiencing underlying neurological changes that do not correspond with obtaining low scores on neuropsychological testing, and (b) may be experiencing cognitive decline indicated by a reduction in high scores from a prior higher ability level.

Among high-functioning older adults, frontoparietal regions and network activity have been examined in the context of research on cognitive reserve, which is often estimated based on higher education, occupational attainment, and/or premorbid intelligence. The current sample would be considered to have high cognitive reserve per most research definitions (Stern et al., Reference Stern, Arenaza‐Urquijo, Bartrés‐Faz, Belleville, Cantilon, Chetelat, Ewers, Franzmeier, Kempermann, Kremen, Okonkwo, Scarmeas, Soldan, Udeh‐Momoh, Valenzuela, Vemuri and Vuoksimaa2020). Higher premorbid IQ has been associated with less frontal activity in healthy older adults (Solé-Padullés et al., Reference Solé-Padullés, Bartrés-Faz, Junqué, Vendrell, Rami, Clemente, Bosch, Villar, Bargalló, Jurado, Barrios and Molinuevo2009; Steffener et al., Reference Steffener, Reuben, Rakitin and Stern2011), whereas a higher cognitive reserve composite (e.g., based on premorbid IQ, education, and occupation) has been associated with greater gray matter volume in frontoparietal regions (Bartrés-Faz et al., Reference Bartrés-Faz, Solé-Padullés, Junqué, Rami, Bosch, Bargalló, Falcón, Sánchez-Valle and Molinuevo2009). Per these prior findings, increased functional activity and lower frontoparietal volume may correspond with an underlying decline in high-functioning older adults. There were no functional differences at resting-state observed in the current study, but lower bilateral volume and thickness in frontoparietal regions was observed. Participants with subjective cognitive concerns may be noticing changes that correspond with greater perceived cognitive difficulties in everyday life and a reduction in high scores on testing. These changes may be explained by underlying changes in frontoparietal or other regions.

Post hoc analyses were conducted that (a) controlled for relevant demographic and physical and health variables as covariates, and (b) examined the DMN to determine whether group differences were specific to the FPCN. The adjusted analyses indicated that group differences in frontoparietal volume and thickness were specific to the left hemisphere after controlling for demographic and physical health variables, which were associated with slightly larger effect sizes in unadjusted group comparisons. This finding indicates that volume differences between high-functioning older adults with and without subjective cognitive concerns are more pronounced in the left hemisphere, which aligns with research indicating that the left frontal cortex may underlie reserve capacity in both normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease (Franzmeier et al., Reference Franzmeier, Hartmann, Taylor, Araque-Caballero, Simon-Vermot, Kambeitz-Ilankovic, Bürger, Catak, Janowitz, Müller, Ertl-Wagner, Stahl, Dichgans, Duering and Ewers2018). Group differences were not observed for the DMN, which has shown associations with cognitive aging (Hafkemeijer et al., Reference Hafkemeijer, van der Grond and Rombouts2012). Many effect sizes for the DMN were small-to-medium, and neared significance for some volume and thickness variables, meaning the sample was underpowered to detect more subtle effects than observed for the FPCN.

The neuroimaging findings add to a growing body of research on brain differences between older adults with and without subjective cognitive concerns (Parker et al., Reference Parker, Ohlhauser, Scarapicchia, Smart, Szoeke and Gawryluk2022). Researchers have compared older adults with subjective cognitive decline to healthy control participants, finding frontal differences (Archer et al., Reference Archer, Kennedy, Barnes, Pepple, Boyes, Randlesome, Clegg, Leung, Ourselin, Frost, Rossor and Fox2010; Hong et al., Reference Hong, Yoon, Shim, Ahn, Yang and Lee2015; Kuhn et al., Reference Kuhn, Moulinet, Perrotin, La Joie, Landeau, Tomadesso, Bejanin, Sherif, De La Sayette, Desgranges, Vivien, Poisnel and Chételat2019; Toledo et al., Reference Toledo, Bjerke, Chen, Rozycki, Jack, Weiner, Arnold, Reiman, Davatzikos, Shaw and Trojanowski2015) and parietal differences (Archer et al., Reference Archer, Kennedy, Barnes, Pepple, Boyes, Randlesome, Clegg, Leung, Ourselin, Frost, Rossor and Fox2010; Hong et al., Reference Hong, Yoon, Shim, Ahn, Yang and Lee2015) in regional volume and thickness. Researchers have also found differences on structural MRI in other brain regions not explored in the current study, including regions typically impacted in Alzheimer’s disease, such as the entorhinal cortex (Fan et al., Reference Fan, Lai, Chen, Hsu, Chen, Huang, Cheng, Tseng, Hua, Chen and Chiu2018; Meiberth et al., Reference Meiberth, Scheef, Wolfsgruber, Boecker, Block, Träber, Erk, Heneka, Jacobi, Spottke, Walter, Wagner, Hu and Jessen2015; Ryu et al., Reference Ryu, Lim, Na, Shim, Cho, Yoon, Hong and Yang2017) and hippocampus (Archer et al., Reference Archer, Kennedy, Barnes, Pepple, Boyes, Randlesome, Clegg, Leung, Ourselin, Frost, Rossor and Fox2010; Hafkemeijer et al., Reference Hafkemeijer, Altmann-Schneider, Oleksik, van de Wiel, Middelkoop, van Buchem, van der Grond and Rombouts2013; Perrotin et al., Reference Perrotin, de Flores, Lamberton, Poisnel, La Joie, de la Sayette, Mézenge, Tomadesso, Landeau, Desgranges, Chételat, Tales, Jessen, Butler, Wilcock, Phillips and Bayer2015; Striepens et al., Reference Striepens, Scheef, Wind, Popp, Spottke, Cooper-Mahkorn, Suliman, Wagner, Schild and Jessen2010). Although group differences in functional connectivity were not observed for the current sample, functional MRI studies have found decreased activation in frontal regions in subjective cognitive decline (Yasuno et al., Reference Yasuno, Kazui, Yamamoto, Morita, Kajimoto, Ihara, Taguchi, Matsuoka, Kosaka, Tanaka, Kudo, Takeda, Nagatsuka, Iida and Kishimoto2015). In aggregate, neurological differences appear associated with subjective cognitive decline in older adults, with the current findings indicating volumetric and cortical thickness differences specific to the FPCN.

A key issue in detecting a degenerative process in high-functioning older adults is that much more cognitive and neurological change must occur before the individual would present with traditionally low cognitive test scores (e.g., ≤16th percentile) or the cognitive change would begin to interfere with activities of daily living. That said, the underlying change is still occurring, and early detection may allow for earlier intervention. In the absence of low test scores or functional impairment, subjective cognitive concerns have been associated with global amyloid burden in community-dwelling older adults (Buckley et al., Reference Buckley, Sikkes, Villemagne, Mormino, Rabin, Burnham, Papp, Doré, Masters, Properzi, Schultz, Johnson, Rentz, Sperling and Amariglio2019), increased risk for mild cognitive impairment and dementia (Jessen et al., Reference Jessen, Amariglio, Buckley, van der Flier, Han, Molinuevo, Rabin, Rentz, Rodriguez-Gomez, Saykin, Sikkes, Smart, Wolfsgruber and Wagner2020), and, in the current study, lower FPCN volume and thickness in high-functioning older adults. As opposed to awaiting low scores to present on testing, self-reported perceptions of cognitive change and an absence of high scores on neuropsychological assessment may align with underlying pathology that is not detected through a traditional focus on low scores.

A relatively sparse literature has examined neuropsychological methods for detecting potential cognitive decline in high-functioning older adults. Existing evidence suggests that the consideration of premorbid intelligence in normative comparisons captures cases of cognitive decline that may otherwise be overlooked. An early study examined the use of NAART FSIQ to adjust normative comparisons among 58 high-functioning older adults, finding that FSIQ-adjusted norms led to the detection of a possible Alzheimer’s process that may have been missed when using age-adjusted norms alone (Rentz et al., Reference Rentz, Calvo, Scinto, Sperling, Budson and Daffner2000). In one sample of 42 highly intelligent older individuals, no participants had any cognitive impairments using age-based norms; but, when using IQ-adjusted norms, 47.6% were detected as having either executive or memory impairments, which predicted further decline at 3.5 years follow-up (Rentz et al., Reference Rentz, Huh, Faust, Budson, Scinto, Sperling and Daffner2004). Traditional approaches to neuropsychological assessment may fail to detect cognitive decline in high-functioning older adults, leading researchers to develop IQ-based normative data for highly intelligent people, albeit with a limited sample size of 75 participants (Rentz et al., Reference Rentz, Sardinha, Huh, Searl, Daffner and Sperling2006). Researchers have even called for norms specific to high-level professions (e.g., physician-based normative data) (Gaudet & Del Bene, Reference Gaudet and Del Bene2022).

As a way to control for baseline ability level, multiple approaches exist to estimate premorbid intelligence (Kirton et al., Reference Kirton, Soble, Marceaux, Messerly, Bain, Webber, Fullen, Alverson and McCoy2020), but there are rarely stratifications of normative data by IQ or score adjustments for IQ. More often, education is considered as a proxy for premorbid ability, either through demographic-adjusted scores or normative stratifications. In the current study, some participants with college degrees did not have a NAART FSIQ ≥75th percentile (n = 8; 11.7%), and some participants with NAART FSIQ ≥75th percentile did not obtain a college degree (n = 6; 8.8%). The use of education stratifications or adjustments may lead to a normative comparison sample that does not match the intelligence of a high-functioning examinee, rationalizing greater use of alternative approaches to detecting potential cognitive decline. These could include IQ-stratified norms or multivariate base rates of high scores to determine whether the number of high scores obtained on testing is fewer than expected.

Few studies have examined the normal frequency of high scores on neuropsychological test batteries, with multivariate base rates of high scores developed for only the D-KEFS (Karr et al., Reference Karr, Garcia-Barrera, Holdnack and Iverson2020) and the NIHTB-CB (Iverson & Karr, Reference Iverson and Karr2021; Karr & Iverson, Reference Karr and Iverson2020; Karr et al., Reference Karr, Rivera Mindt and Iverson2022b). These studies have provided base rates of obtaining no high scores among healthy adults, with stratifications by estimated intelligence. When interpreting the seven D-KEFS test scores, just 4.8% of participants with a FSIQ ≥ 110 obtained no scores ≥75th percentile, making it uncommon to obtain no high scores among high-functioning adults. Per the current findings, subjective cognitive concerns are associated with both obtaining fewer high scores and underlying MRI-derived neurological changes among high-functioning older adults.

Multivariate base rates of high scores, with stratifications by intelligence, provide base rates of obtaining few or no high scores, and can allow for an indirect translation of the current findings into clinical practice. Take for example, a 68-year-old woman of high intelligence with a doctoral degree who works as a provost at an American university. Upon renewal of her appointment, multiple subordinate employees describe work performance changes, noting disorganization, forgetfulness, and inattentiveness. Her husband reports greater irritability. She reports more difficulty at work, noting greater difficulty with management responsibilities in the last year. She completes the Neuro-QoL Cognitive Function questionnaire (Cella et al., Reference Cella, Lai, Nowinski, Victorson, Peterman, Miller, Bethoux, Heinemann, Rubin, Cavazos, Reder, Sufit, Simuni, Holmes, Siderowf, Wojna, Bode, McKinney, Podrabsky and Moy2012; Gershon et al., Reference Gershon, Lai, Bode, Choi, Moy, Bleck, Miller, Peterman and Cella2012), reporting significant subjective cognitive concerns (T = 32). On neuropsychological assessment, she completes the WASI (FSIQ = 120); Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS-IV) subtests (Logical Memory – Immediate: Scaled Score [SS] = 12; Delayed: SS = 9) and Visual Reproduction (Immediate: SS = 12; Delayed: SS = 10); and D-KEFS tests, including Trail Making (Number-Letter Switching – Time-to-Completion: SS = 12), Verbal Fluency (Letter Fluency – Total Correct: SS = 11; Category Fluency – Total Correct: SS = 10; Category Switching – Total Correct: SS = 10; Category Switching – Total Switching Accuracy: SS = 9), and Color-Word Interference (Inhibition – Time-to-Completion: SS = 11; Inhibition/Switching – Time-to-Completion: SS = 11).

This examinee obtains two scores ≥75th percentile on WMS-IV Logical Memory and Visual Reproduction Immediate trials, but has average Delayed trial performances; and on the D-KEFS, all performances fall within the average range apart from Trail Making, which was 75th percentile. The base rates of high scores on the WMS-IV have not been published, but these findings indicate a reduction in performances at the delayed trial. For the D-KEFS, obtaining one score ≥75th percentile occurs among just 16.9% of healthy adults in the normative sample with WASI FSIQ ≥ 110 (Karr et al., Reference Karr, Garcia-Barrera, Holdnack and Iverson2020). This performance approximates ∼ 1 SD below the mean in comparison to an IQ-matched normative comparison group. Considering self- and informant-reported subjective cognitive concerns and fewer high scores than expected, this examinee may be experiencing cognitive decline, despite obtaining no low test scores (i.e., ≤16th percentile).

A focus on methods for detecting potential cognitive decline in high-functioning older adults is warranted, as even average performances in cognition may correspond with reduced work capacity in high-level positions and cognitive decline in this population has been associated with broader health concerns, including increased risk of hospitalization (Chodosh et al., Reference Chodosh, Seeman, Keeler, Sewall, Hirsch, Guralnik and Reuben2004) and reduced gait speed (Rosso et al., Reference Rosso, Metti, Faulkner, Redfern, Yaffe, Launer, Elizabeth Shaaban, Nadkarni, Rosano, Montero-Odasso and Perry2019), which may correspond with increased fall risk (Menant et al., Reference Menant, Schoene, Sarofim and Lord2014). Further, early interventions may be beneficial before underlying degeneration advances enough to produce low scores on cognitive testing. Dietary changes may be beneficial, with research demonstrating that antioxidants and beta-carotene may be protective against cognitive decline among high-functioning older adults (Hu et al., Reference Hu, Bretsky, Crimmins, Guralnik, Reuben and Seeman2006). Many clinical trials have examined nonpharmacological interventions among older adults with subjective cognitive decline (e.g., exercise, cognitive training), finding evidence for a benefit on cognitive functioning (Smart et al., Reference Smart, Karr, Areshenkoff, Rabin, Hudon, Gates, Ali, Arenaza-Urquijo, Buckley, Chetelat, Hampel, Jessen, Marchant, Sikkes, Tales, van der Flier and Wesselman2017).

This study provides insight into the correspondence between subjective cognitive concerns, the number of high scores obtained on neuropsychological testing, and underlying neurological differences, indicating that high-functioning older adults with subjective cognitive concerns tend to obtain fewer high scores and have lower FPCN volume and thickness. Although novel, this study has limitations that affect the generalizability of the findings. Subjective cognitive concerns were measured for the past month, as opposed to a longer onset; meaning such concerns may be transient rather than indicating long-term perceptions of decline. These concerns were correlated with perceived stress in the past month, but not mood in the past week. The sample size was relatively small and homogenous, consisting of primarily women (i.e., 69.1%), nearly all of whom were White (i.e., 97.1%), recruited from a single urban area in a midwestern state. Complete data was required to produce high and low score counts for individual participants, which led to imputation of some data. These findings may vary from results that would be obtained from a sample without missingness. By design, the sample was highly educated (i.e., 91.2% with a college degree), but involved limited representation of highly intelligent older adults without college degrees. Although there was substantial correspondence between NAART estimated FSIQ and college education, these variables are both proxies of premorbid functioning, and other variables, such as occupational complexity, were not considered when selecting this high-functioning sample. Per annual household income, there was a broad range of socioeconomic status of participants (i.e., range: $12,000–$500,000). The test battery was also brief, with just seven-test scores interpreted for analyses, which is much lower than the number of test scores typically obtained during a neuropsychological assessment. These findings offer preliminary evidence that subjective cognitive concerns and a lower number of high scores may indicate cognitive decline in high-functioning older adults, but future research is needed to examine more diverse samples using test batteries more consistent with neuropsychological practice. Such studies would both replicate the current findings and expand their generalizability and translation into practice.

Acknowledgments

This research was funded by the National Institute on Aging (NIA) of the National Institutes of Health (NIH) (#R01-AG026307). This work was also supported, in part, by a Building Interdisciplinary Research Careers in Women’s Health (BIRCWH) grant (#K12-DA035150) from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA) of the NIH and the University of Kentucky Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center funded by the National Institute on Aging (#P30AG072946). The authors have no competing interests or conflicts of interest to report.

References

Albert, M. S., DeKosky, S. T., Dickson, D., Dubois, B., Feldman, H. H., Fox, N. C., Gamst, A., Holtzman, D. M., Jagust, W. J., Petersen, R. C., Snyder, P. J., Carrillo, M. C., Thies, B., & Phelps, C. H. (2011). The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging‐Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s and Dementia, 7(3), 270279. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Archer, H. A., Kennedy, J., Barnes, J., Pepple, T., Boyes, R., Randlesome, K., Clegg, S., Leung, K. K., Ourselin, S., Frost, C., Rossor, M. N., & Fox, N. C. (2010). Memory complaints and increased rates of brain atrophy: Risk factors for mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 25(11), 11191126. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.2440 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Avants, B. B., Tustison, N. J., Song, G., Cook, P. A., Klein, A., & Gee, J. C. (2011). A reproducible evaluation of ANTs similarity metric performance in brain image registration. NeuroImage, 54(3), 20332044. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.025 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bäckman, L., Jones, S., Berger, A. K., Laukka, E. J., & Small, B. J. (2005). Cognitive impairment in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease: A meta-analysis. Neuropsychology, 19(4), 520531. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.19.4.520 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Badre, D., & D’Esposito, M. (2007). Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for a hierarchical organization of the prefrontal cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(12), 20822099. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.12.2082 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartrés-Faz, D., Solé-Padullés, C., Junqué, C., Rami, L., Bosch, B., Bargalló, N. A.;ria, Falcón, C., Sánchez-Valle, R., & Molinuevo, J. L. (2009). Interactions of cognitive reserve with regional brain anatomy and brain function during a working memory task in healthy elders. Biological Psychology, 80(2), 256259. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.10.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benton, A. L., Hamsher, K. S., & Sivan, A. B. (1994). Multilingual aphasia examination. In Encyclopedia of clinical neuropsychology (3rd ed.). Psychological Corporation, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-79948-3_900 Google Scholar
Binder, L. M., Iverson, G. L., & Brooks, B. L. (2009). To err is human: “Abnormal” neuropsychological scores and variability are common in healthy adults. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 24(1), 3146. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acn001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blair, J. R., & Spreen, O. (1989). Predicting premorbid IQ: A revision of the National Adult Reading Test. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 3(2), 129136. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854048908403285 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowie, C. R., & Harvey, P. D. (2006). Administration and interpretation of the Trail Making Test. Nature Protocols, 1(5), 22772281. https://doi.org/10.1038/nprot.2006.390 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brink, T. L., Yesavage, J. A., Lum, O., Heersema, P. H., Adey, M., & Rose, T. L. (1982). Screening tests for geriatric depression. Clinical Gerontologist, 1(1), 3743. https://doi.org/10.1300/J018v01n01_06 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooks, B. L., Iverson, G. L., & Holdnack, J. A. (2013). Understanding and using multivariate base rates with the WAIS-IV/WMS-IV. In Holdnack, J. A., Drozdick, L. W., Weiss, L. G., & Iverson, G. L. (Eds.), Advanced clinical interpretation (pp. 75102). Elsevier Science. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-386934-0.00002-X Google Scholar
Brooks, B. L., Iverson, G. L., Holdnack, J. A., & Feldman, H. H. (2008). Potential for misclassification of mild cognitive impairment: A study of memory scores on the Wechsler Memory Scale-III in healthy older adults. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14(3), 463478. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617708080521 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brooks, B. L., Iverson, G. L., & White, T. (2007). Substantial risk of “Accidental MCI” in healthy older adults: Base rates of low memory scores in neuropsychological assessment. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13(3), 490500. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617707070531 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brooks, B. L., Iverson, G. L., & White, T. (2009). Advanced interpretation of the Neuropsychological Assessment Battery with older adults: Base rate analyses, discrepancy scores, and interpreting change. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 24(7), 647657. http://ovidsp.ovid.com/ovidweb.cgi?T=JS&PAGE=reference&D=emed9&NEWS=N&AN=2010003436 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buckley, R. F., Schultz, A. P., Hedden, T., Papp, K. V., Hanseeuw, B. J., Marshall, G., Sepulcre, J., Smith, E. E., Rentz, D. M., Johnson, K. A., Sperling, R. A., & Chhatwal, J. P. (2017). Functional network integrity presages cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer disease. Neurology, 89(1), 2937. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000004059 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buckley, R. F., Sikkes, S., Villemagne, V. L., Mormino, E. C., Rabin, J. S., Burnham, S., Papp, K. V., Doré, V., Masters, C. L., Properzi, M. J., Schultz, A. P., Johnson, K. A., Rentz, D. M., Sperling, R. A., & Amariglio, R. E. (2019). Using subjective cognitive decline to identify high global amyloid in community-based samples: A cross-cohort study. Alzheimer’s and Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment and Disease Monitoring, 11(1), 670678. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dadm.2019.08.004 Google ScholarPubMed
Burmester, B., Leathem, J., & Merrick, P. (2016). Subjective cognitive complaints and objective cognitive function in aging: A systematic review and meta-analysis of recent cross-sectional findings. Neuropsychology Review, 26(4), 376393. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-016-9332-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cabeza, R., Ciaramelli, E., Olson, I. R., & Moscovitch, M. (2008). The parietal cortex and episodic memory: An attentional account. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(8), 613625. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2459 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cella, D., Lai, J.-S., Nowinski, C. J., Victorson, D., Peterman, A., Miller, D., Bethoux, F., Heinemann, A., Rubin, S., Cavazos, J. E., Reder, A. T., Sufit, R., Simuni, T., Holmes, G. L., Siderowf, A., Wojna, V., Bode, R., McKinney, N., Podrabsky, T., …Moy, C. (2012). Neuro-QOL: Brief measures of health-related quality of life for clinical research in neurology. Neurology, 78(23), 18601867. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0b013e318258f744 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chodosh, J., Seeman, T. E., Keeler, E., Sewall, A., Hirsch, S. H., Guralnik, J. M., & Reuben, D. B. (2004). Cognitive decline in high-functioning older persons is associated with an increased risk of hospitalization. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 52(9), 14561462. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-5415.2004.52407.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ciric, R., Wolf, D. H., Power, J. D., Roalf, D. R., Baum, G. L., Ruparel, K., Shinohara, R. T., Elliott, M. A., Eickhoff, S. B., Davatzikos, C., Gur, R. C., Gur, R. E., Bassett, D. S., & Satterthwaite, T. D. (2017). Benchmarking of participant-level confound regression strategies for the control of motion artifact in studies of functional connectivity. NeuroImage, 154, 174187. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.020 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. In Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed.). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203771587 Google Scholar
Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24(4), 385396. https://doi.org/10.2307/2136404 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dale, A. M., Fischl, B., & Sereno, M. I. (1999). Cortical surface-based analysis: I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. NeuroImage, 9(2), 179194. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.1998.0395 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dubois, B., Feldman, H. H., Jacova, C., DeKosky, S. T., Barberger-Gateau, P., Cummings, J., Delacourte, A., Galasko, D., Gauthier, S., Jicha, G., Meguro, K., OʼBrien, J., Pasquier, F., Robert, P., Rossor, M., Salloway, S., Stern, Y., Visser, P. J., & Scheltens, P. (2007). Research criteria for the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: Revising the NINCDS-ADRDA criteria. The Lancet Neurology, 6(8), 734746. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(07)70178-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enders, C. K. (2010). Applied missing data analysis. The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Erdfelder, E., Faul, F., Buchner, A., & Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 11491160. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149 Google Scholar
Esteban, O., Ciric, R., Finc, K., Blair, R. W., Markiewicz, C. J., Moodie, C. A., Kent, J. D., Goncalves, M., DuPre, E., Gomez, D. E. P., Ye, Z., Salo, T., Valabregue, R., Amlien, I. K., Liem, F., Jacoby, N., Stojić, H., Cieslak, M., Urchs, S., …Gorgolewski, K. J. (2020). Analysis of task-based functional MRI data preprocessed with fMRIPrep. Nature Protocols, 15(7), 21862202. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41596-020-0327-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Esteban, O., Markiewicz, C. J., Blair, R. W., Moodie, C. A., Isik, A. I., Erramuzpe, A., Kent, J. D., Goncalves, M., DuPre, E., Snyder, M., Oya, H., Ghosh, S. S., Wright, J., Durnez, J., Poldrack, R. A., & Gorgolewski, K. J. (2019). fMRIPrep: A robust preprocessing pipeline for functional MRI. Nature Methods, 16(1), 111116. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41592-018-0235-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, A. C., Janke, A. L., Collins, D. L., & Baillet, S. (2012). Brain templates and atlases. NeuroImage, 62(2), 911922. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.024 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, D. R., & Segerstrom, S. C. (2015). Physical activity and depressive symptoms interact to predict executive functioning among community-dwelling older adults. Experimental Aging Research, 41(5), 534545. https://doi.org/10.1080/0361073X.2015.1085741 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fan, L.‐Y., Lai, Y.‐M., Chen, T.‐F., Hsu, Y.‐C., Chen, P.‐Y., Huang, K.‐Z., Cheng, T.‐W., Tseng, W.‐Y. I., Hua, M.‐S., Chen, Y.‐F., & Chiu, M.‐J. (2018). Diminution of context association memory structure in subjects with subjective cognitive decline. Human Brain Mapping, 39(6), 25492562. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24022 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franzmeier, N., Hartmann, J., Taylor, A. N. W., Araque-Caballero, , Simon-Vermot, L., Kambeitz-Ilankovic, L., Bürger, K., Catak, C., Janowitz, D., Müller, C., Ertl-Wagner, B., Stahl, R., Dichgans, M., Duering, M., & Ewers, M. (2018). The left frontal cortex supports reserve in aging by enhancing functional network efficiency Rik Ossenkoppele. Alzheimer’s Research and Therapy, 10(28), 112. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-018-0358-y Google Scholar
Gaudet, C. E., & Del Bene, V. A. (2022). Neuropsychological assessment of the aging physician: A review & commentary. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 35(3), 271279. https://doi.org/10.1177/08919887211016063 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geiger, P. J., Reed, R. G., Combs, H. L., Boggero, I. A., & Segerstrom, S. C. (2019). Longitudinal associations among older adults’ neurocognitive performance, psychological distress, and self-reported cognitive function. Psychology and Neuroscience, 12(2), 224235. https://doi.org/10.1037/pne0000155 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gershon, R. C., Lai, J. S., Bode, R., Choi, S., Moy, C., Bleck, T., Miller, D., Peterman, A., & Cella, D. (2012). Neuro-QOL: Quality of life item banks for adults with neurological disorders: Item development and calibrations based upon clinical and general population testing. Quality of Life Research, 21(3), 475486. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11136-011-9958-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gorgolewski, K., Burns, C. D., Madison, C., Clark, D., Halchenko, Y. O., Waskom, M. L., & Ghosh, S. S. (2011). Nipype: A flexible, lightweight and extensible neuroimaging data processing framework in Python. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics, 5. https://doi.org/10.3389/fninf.2011.00013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greve, D. N., & Fischl, B. (2009). Accurate and robust brain image alignment using boundary-based registration. NeuroImage, 48(1), 6372. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.060 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guilmette, T. J., Sweet, J. J., Hebben, N., Koltai, D., Mahone, E. M., Spiegler, B. J., Stucky, K., Westerveld, M., & Conference Participants (2020). American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology consensus conference statement on uniform labeling of performance test scores. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 34(3), 437453. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2020.1722244 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hafkemeijer, A., Altmann-Schneider, I., Oleksik, A. M., van de Wiel, L., Middelkoop, H. A. M., van Buchem, M. A., van der Grond, J., & Rombouts, S. A. R. B. (2013). Increased functional connectivity and brain atrophy in elderly with subjective memory complaints. Brain Connectivity, 3(4), 353362. https://doi.org/10.1089/brain.2013.0144 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hafkemeijer, A., van der Grond, J., & Rombouts, S. A. R. B. (2012). Imaging the default mode network in aging and dementia. Biochimica et Biophysica Acta - Molecular Basis of Disease, 1822(3), 431441. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbadis.2011.07.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heaton, R. K., Grant, I., & Matthews, C. G. (1991). Comprehensive norms for an extended Halstead-Reitan Battery: Demographic corrections, research findings, and clinical applications. Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.Google Scholar
Heaton, R. K., Miller, S. W., Taylor, M. J., & Grant, I. (2004). Revised comprehensive norms for an expanded Halstead-Reitan Battery: Demographically adjusted neuropsychological norms for African American and Caucasian adults professional manual. Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.Google Scholar
Hong, Y. J., Yoon, B., Shim, Y. S., Ahn, K. J., Yang, D. W., & Lee, J. H. (2015). Gray and white matter degenerations in subjective memory impairment: Comparisons with normal controls and mild cognitive impairment. Journal of Korean Medical Science, 30(11), 16521658. https://doi.org/10.3346/jkms.2015.30.11.1652 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hu, P., Bretsky, P., Crimmins, E. M., Guralnik, J. M., Reuben, D. B., & Seeman, T. E. (2006). Association between serum beta-carotene levels and decline of cognitive function in high-functioning older persons with or without apolipoprotein E 4 alleles: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Journals of Gerontology - Series A Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 61(6), 616620. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/61.6.616 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hultsch, D. F., Strauss, E., Hunter, M. A., & MacDonald, S. W. S. (2008). Intraindividual variability, cognition, and aging. In Craik, F. I. M., & Salthouse, T. A. (Eds.), The handbook of aging and cognition (pp. 491556). Routledge.Google Scholar
Iverson, G. L., & Karr, J. E. (2021). Improving the methodology for identifying mild cognitive impairment in intellectually high-functioning adults using the NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery. Frontiers in Psychology, 12(724888), 110. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.724888 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jenkinson, M., Bannister, P., Brady, M., & Smith, S. (2002). Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. NeuroImage, 17(2), 825841. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1132 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jessen, F., Amariglio, R. E., Buckley, R. F., van der Flier, W. M., Han, Y., Molinuevo, J. L., Rabin, L., Rentz, D. M., Rodriguez-Gomez, O., Saykin, A. J., Sikkes, S. A. M., Smart, C. M., Wolfsgruber, S., & Wagner, M. (2020). The characterisation of subjective cognitive decline. The Lancet Neurology, 19(3), 271278,CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karr, J. E., Garcia-Barrera, M. A., Holdnack, J. A., & Iverson, G. L. (2017). Using multivariate base rates to interpret low scores on an abbreviated battery of the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 32(3), 297305. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acw105 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karr, J. E., Garcia-Barrera, M. A., Holdnack, J. A., & Iverson, G. L. (2018). Advanced clinical interpretation of the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System: Multivariate base rates of low scores. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 32(1), 4253. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2017.1334828 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karr, J. E., Garcia-Barrera, M. A., Holdnack, J. A., & Iverson, G. L. (2020). The other side of the bell curve: Multivariate base rates of high scores on the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 26(4), 382393. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1355617719001218 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karr, J. E., & Iverson, G. L. (2020). Interpreting high scores on the NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery: Potential utility for detecting cognitive decline in high-functioning individuals. Neuropsychology, 34(7), 764773. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000691 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karr, J. E., Rivera Mindt, M., & Iverson, G. L. (2022a). A multivariate interpretation of the Spanish-language NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery: The normal frequency of low scores. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 37(2), 338351. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acab064 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karr, J. E., Rivera Mindt, M., & Iverson, G. L. (2022b). Assessing cognitive decline in high-functioning Spanish-speaking patients: High score base rates on the Spanish-language NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 37(5), 939951. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acab097 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirton, J. W., Soble, J. R., Marceaux, J. C., Messerly, J., Bain, K. M., Webber, T. A., Fullen, C., Alverson, W. A., & McCoy, K. J. M. (2020). Comparison of models of premorbid IQ estimation using the TOPF, OPIE-3, and Barona equation, with corrections for the Flynn effect. Neuropsychology, 34(1), 4352. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000569 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, A., Ghosh, S. S., Bao, F. S., Giard, J., Häme, , Stavsky, E., Lee, N., Rossa, B., Reuter, M., Chaibub Neto, E., Keshavan, A., & Schneidman, D. (2017). Mindboggling morphometry of human brains. PLoS Computational Biology, 13(2), e1005350. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005350 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhn, E., Moulinet, Iès, Perrotin, A., La Joie, R., Landeau, B., Tomadesso, C., Bejanin, A., Sherif, S., De La Sayette, V., Desgranges, B., Vivien, D., Poisnel, G., & Chételat, G. B. (2019). Cross-sectional and longitudinal characterization of SCD patients recruited from the community versus from a memory clinic: Subjective cognitive decline, psychoaffective factors, cognitive performances, and atrophy progression over time. Alzheimer’s Research and Therapy, 11(1). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-019-0514-z Google ScholarPubMed
Lanczos, C. (1964). Evaluation of noisy data. Journal of the Society for Industrial and Applied Mathematics Series B Numerical Analysis, 1(1), 7685. https://doi.org/10.1137/0701007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Little, R. J. A. (1988). A test of missing completely at random for multivariate data with missing values. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 83(404), 11981202. https://doi.org/10.1080/01621459.1988.10478722 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meiberth, D., Scheef, L., Wolfsgruber, S., Boecker, H., Block, W., Träber, F., Erk, S., Heneka, M. T., Jacobi, H., Spottke, A., Walter, H., Wagner, M., Hu, X., & Jessen, F. (2015). Cortical thinning in individuals with subjective memory impairment. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 45(1), 139146. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-142322 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Menant, J. C., Schoene, D., Sarofim, M., & Lord, S. R. (2014). Single and dual task tests of gait speed are equivalent in the prediction of falls in older people: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Ageing Research Reviews, 16(1), 83104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.arr.2014.06.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mistridis, P., Egli, S. C., Iverson, G. L., Berres, M., Willmes, K., Welsh-Bohmer, K. A., & Monsch, A. U. (2015). Considering the base rates of low performance in cognitively healthy older adults improves the accuracy to identify neurocognitive impairment with the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease-Neuropsychological Assessment Battery. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 265(5), 407417. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-014-0571-z CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parker, A. F., Ohlhauser, L., Scarapicchia, V., Smart, C. M., Szoeke, C., & Gawryluk, J. R. (2022). A systematic review of neuroimaging studies comparing individuals with subjective cognitive decline to healthy controls. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 86(4), 15451567. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-215249 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perrotin, A., de Flores, R., Lamberton, F., Poisnel, G., La Joie, R., de la Sayette, V., Mézenge, F., Tomadesso, C., Landeau, B., Desgranges, B., Chételat, G. B., Tales, A., Jessen, F., Butler, C., Wilcock, G., Phillips, J., & Bayer, T. (2015). Hippocampal subfield volumetry and 3D surface mapping in subjective cognitive decline. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 48(S1), S141S150. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-150087 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, R. C., Smith, G. E., Waring, S. C., Ivnik, R. J., Tangalos, E. G., & Kokmen, E. (1999). Mild cognitive impairment: Clinical characterization and outcome. Archives of Neurology, 56(3), 303308. https://doi.org/10.1001/archneur.56.3.303 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Power, J. D., Mitra, A., Laumann, T. O., Snyder, A. Z., Schlaggar, B. L., & Petersen, S. E. (2014). Methods to detect, characterize, and remove motion artifact in resting state fMRI. NeuroImage, 84, 320341. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.048 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pruim, R. H. R., Mennes, M., van Rooij, D., Llera, A., Buitelaar, J. K., & Beckmann, C. F. (2015). ICA-AROMA: A robust ICA-based strategy for removing motion artifacts from fMRI data. NeuroImage, 112, 267277. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.064 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reitan, R. M. (1958). Validity of the Trail Making Test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 8(3), 271276. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1958.8.3.271 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1993). The Halstead-Reitan neuropsychological test battery: Theory and clinical interpretation (2nd ed.). Neuropsychology Press.Google Scholar
Rentz, D. M., Calvo, V. L., Scinto, L. F. M., Sperling, R. A., Budson, A. E., & Daffner, K. R. (2000). Detecting early cognitive decline in high-functioning elders. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 33(1), 2749.Google Scholar
Rentz, D. M., Huh, T. J., Faust, R. R., Budson, A. E., Scinto, L. F. M., Sperling, R. A., & Daffner, K. R. (2004). Use of IQ-adjusted norms to predict progressive cognitive decline in highly intelligent older individuals. Neuropsychology, 18(1), 3849. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.18.1.38 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rentz, D. M., Sardinha, L. M., Huh, T. J., Searl, M. M., Daffner, K. R., & Sperling, R. A. (2006). IQ-based norms for highly intelligent adults. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 20(4), 637648. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854040500477498 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rivas-Fernández, M.Á., Lindín, M., Zurrón, M., Díaz, F., Lojo-Seoane, C., Pereiro, A. X., & Galdo-Álvarez, S. (2023). Neuroanatomical and neurocognitive changes associated with subjective cognitive decline. Frontiers in Medicine, 10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fmed.2023.1094799 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosso, A. L., Metti, A. L., Faulkner, K., Redfern, M., Yaffe, K., Launer, L., Elizabeth Shaaban, C., Nadkarni, N. K., Rosano, C., Montero-Odasso, M., & Perry, G. (2019). Complex walking tasks and risk for cognitive decline in high functioning older adults. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 71(s1), S65S73. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-181140 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ryu, S. Y., Lim, E. Y., Na, S., Shim, Y. S., Cho, J. H., Yoon, B., Hong, Y. J., & Yang, D. W. (2017). Hippocampal and entorhinal structures in subjective memory impairment: A combined MRI volumetric and DTI study. International Psychogeriatrics, 29(5), 785792. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610216002349 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaefer, A., Kong, R., Gordon, E. M., Laumann, T. O., Zuo, X.-N., Holmes, A. J., Eickhoff, S. B., & Yeo, B. T. T. (2018). Local-global parcellation of the human cerebral cortex from intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. Cerebral Cortex, 28(9), 30953114. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhx179 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, A. B., Reed, R. G., Garcia-Willingham, N. E., Lawrence, K. A., & Segerstrom, S. C. (2019). Lifespan socioeconomic context: Associations with cognitive functioning in later life. Journals of Gerontology - Series B Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 74(1), 113125. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gby071 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Segerstrom, S. C., Reed, R. G., & Karr, J. E. (2022). Cytomegalovirus and Toxoplasma Gondii Serostatus prospectively correlated with problems in self-regulation but not executive function among older adults. Psychosomatic Medicine, 84(5), 603611. https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0000000000001086 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smart, C. M., Karr, J. E., Areshenkoff, C. N., Rabin, L. A., Hudon, C., Gates, N., Ali, J. I., Arenaza-Urquijo, E. M., Buckley, R. F., Chetelat, G., Hampel, H., Jessen, F., Marchant, N. L., Sikkes, S. A. M., Tales, A., van der Flier, W. M., & Wesselman, L. (2017). Non-pharmacologic interventions for older adults with subjective cognitive decline: Systematic review, meta-analysis, and preliminary recommendations. Neuropsychology Review, 27(3), 245257. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-017-9342-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solé-Padullés, C., Bartrés-Faz, D., Junqué, C., Vendrell, P., Rami, L., Clemente, I. C., Bosch, B., Villar, A., Bargalló, N. A., Jurado, M. A., Barrios, M., & Molinuevo, J. L. (2009). Brain structure and function related to cognitive reserve variables in normal aging, mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiology of Aging, 30(7), 11141124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.10.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spreng, R. N., Stevens, W. D., Chamberlain, J. P., Gilmore, A. W., & Schacter, D. L. (2010). Default network activity, coupled with the frontoparietal control network, supports goal-directed cognition. NeuroImage, 53(1), 303317. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steffener, J., Reuben, A., Rakitin, B. C., & Stern, Y. (2011). Supporting performance in the face of age-related neural changes: Testing mechanistic roles of cognitive reserve. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 5(3), 212221. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-011-9125-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steinberg, B. A., Bieliauskas, L. A., Smith, G. E., & Ivnik, R. J. (2005). Mayo’s older Americans normative studies: Age- and IQ-adjusted norms for the Trail-Making Test, the Stroop test, and MAE Controlled Oral Word Association Test. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 19(3-4), 329377. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854040590945210 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steinberg, B. A., Bieliauskas, L. A., Smith, G. E., Ivnik, R. J., & Malec, J. F. (2005). Mayo’s older Americans normative studies: Age- and IQ-adjusted norms for the Auditory Verbal Learning Test and the Visual Spatial Learning Test. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 19(3-4), 464523. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854040590945193 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, Y., Arenaza‐Urquijo, E. M., Bartrés‐Faz, D., Belleville, S., Cantilon, M., Chetelat, G., Ewers, M., Franzmeier, N., Kempermann, G., Kremen, W. S., Okonkwo, O., Scarmeas, N., Soldan, A., Udeh‐Momoh, C., Valenzuela, M., Vemuri, P., Vuoksimaa, E., & Reserve, Resilience and Protective Factors PIA Empirical Definitions and Conceptual Frameworks Workgroup (2020). Whitepaper: Defining and investigating cognitive reserve, brain reserve, and brain maintenance. Alzheimer’s and Dementia, 16(9), 13051311. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2018.07.219 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stewart, A. L., Ware, J. E., Sherbourne, C. D., & Wells, K. B. (1992). Psychological distress/well-being and cognitive functioning measures. In Stewart, A. L., & Ware, J. E. (Eds.), Measuring functioning and well-being: The Medical Outcomes Study approach (pp. 102142). Duke University Press.Google Scholar
Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S., & Spreen, O. (2006). A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary (3rd ed.). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Striepens, N., Scheef, L., Wind, A., Popp, J., Spottke, A., Cooper-Mahkorn, D., Suliman, H., Wagner, M., Schild, H. H., & Jessen, F. (2010). Volume loss of the medial temporal lobe structures in subjective memory impairment. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 29(1), 7581. https://doi.org/10.1159/000264630 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toledo, J. B., Bjerke, M., Chen, K., Rozycki, M., Jack, C. R., Weiner, M. W., Arnold, S. E., Reiman, E. M., Davatzikos, C., Shaw, L. M., & Trojanowski, J. Q. (2015). Memory, executive, and multidomain subtle cognitive impairment: Clinical and biomarker findings. Neurology, 85(2), 144153. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000001738 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tuokko, H., Garrett, D. D., McDowell, I., Silverberg, N., & Kristjansson, B. (2003). Cognitive decline in high-functioning older adults: Reserve or ascertainment bias? Aging and Mental Health, 7(4), 259270. https://doi.org/10.1080/1360786031000120750 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tustison, N. J., Avants, B. B., Cook, P. A., Zheng, Y., Egan, A., Yushkevich, P. A., & Gee, J. C. (2010). N4ITK: Improved N3 bias correction. IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging, 29(6), 13101320. https://doi.org/10.1109/TMI.2010.2046908 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tustison, N. J., Cook, P. A., Holbrook, A. J., Johnson, H. J., Muschelli, J., Devenyi, G. A., Duda, J. T., Das, S. R., Cullen, N. C., Gillen, D. L., Yassa, M. A., Stone, J. R., Gee, J. C., & Avants, B. B. (2021). The ANTsX ecosystem for quantitative biological and medical imaging. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 9068. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-87564-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uttl, B. (2002). North American Adult Reading Test: Age norms, reliability, and validity. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24(8), 11231137. https://doi.org/10.1076/jcen.24.8.1123.8375 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vincent, J. L., Kahn, I., Snyder, A. Z., Raichle, M. E., & Buckner, R. L. (2008). Evidence for a frontoparietal control system revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology, 100(6), 33283342. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.90355.2008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (4th ed.). Pearson, Inc.Google Scholar
Yao, Z. F., Yang, M. H., Hwang, K., & Hsieh, S. (2020). Frontoparietal structural properties mediate adult life span differences in executive function. Scientific Reports, 10(9066), 114. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-66083-w CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yasuno, F., Kazui, H., Yamamoto, A., Morita, N., Kajimoto, K., Ihara, M., Taguchi, A., Matsuoka, K., Kosaka, J., Tanaka, T., Kudo, T., Takeda, M., Nagatsuka, K., Iida, H., & Kishimoto, T. (2015). Resting-state synchrony between the retrosplenial cortex and anterior medial cortical structures relates to memory complaints in subjective cognitive impairment. Neurobiology of Aging, 36(6), 21452152. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2015.03.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas Yeo, B. T., Krienen, F. M., Sepulcre, J., Sabuncu, M. R., Lashkari, D., Hollinshead, M., Roffman, J. L., Smoller, J. W., Zöllei, L., Polimeni, J. R., Fischl, B., Liu, H., & Buckner, R. L. (2011). The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology, 106(3), 11251165. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00338.2011 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yesavage, J. A., Brink, T. L., Rose, T. L., Lum, O., Huang, V., Adey, M., & Leirer, V. O. (1982). Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: A preliminary report. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 17(1), 3749. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(82)90033-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, Y., Brady, M., & Smith, S. (2001). Segmentation of brain MR images through a hidden Markov random field model and the expectation-maximization algorithm. IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging, 20(1), 4557. https://doi.org/10.1109/42.906424 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Participant demographics

Figure 1

Figure 1. Frontoparietal control network parcellation used in the current study.

Figure 2

Table 2. Mean performances on individual neuropsychological tests

Figure 3

Table 3. Comparison of high-functioning participants with and without subjective cognitive concerns on number of low and high scores on neuropsychological testing

Figure 4

Table 4. Comparison of high-functioning participants with and without subjective cognitive concerns on volume, thickness, and connectivity of the frontoparietal control and default mode networks

Figure 5

Table 5. Comparison of high-functioning participants with and without subjective cognitive concerns on physical and mental health variables