Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-dh8gc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-09T03:31:52.091Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Heart rate variability during a cognitive reappraisal task in female patients with borderline personality disorder: the role of comorbid posttraumatic stress disorder and dissociation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 September 2018

Annegret Krause-Utz*
Affiliation:
Institute of Clinical Psychology, Leiden University; Leiden Institute for Brain and Cognition; Leiden, The Netherlands Institute of Psychiatric and Psychosomatic Psychotherapy, Central Institute of Mental Health, Mannheim; Medical Faculty, University of Heidelberg, Mannheim, Germany
Julia-Caroline Walther
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatric and Psychosomatic Psychotherapy, Central Institute of Mental Health, Mannheim; Medical Faculty, University of Heidelberg, Mannheim, Germany
Stefanie Lis
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatric and Psychosomatic Psychotherapy, Central Institute of Mental Health, Mannheim; Medical Faculty, University of Heidelberg, Mannheim, Germany
Christian Schmahl
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, Central Institute of Mental Health, Mannheim; Medical Faculty, University of Heidelberg, Mannheim, Germany Department of Psychiatry, Schulich School of Medicine and Dentistry, Western University, London, Ontario, Canada
Martin Bohus
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatric and Psychosomatic Psychotherapy, Central Institute of Mental Health, Mannheim; Medical Faculty, University of Heidelberg, Mannheim, Germany
*
Author for correspondence: Annegret Krause-Utz, E-mail: a.d.krause@fsw.leidenuniv.nl
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Emotion dysregulation is a core feature of borderline personality disorder (BPD), which often co-occurs with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Difficulties in emotion regulation (ER) have been linked to lower high-frequency heart rate variability (HF-HRV), a measure of autonomous nervous system functioning. However, previous research on vagally-mediated heart rate in BPD revealed heterogeneous findings and the effects of comorbid PTSD and dissociation on HF-HRV are not yet completely understood. This study aim to investigate HF-HRV during resting-state and an ER task in female BPD patients with comorbid PTSD (BPD + PTSD), patients without this comorbidity (BPD), and healthy controls (HC).

Methods

57 BPD patients (BPD: n = 37, BPD + PTSD: n = 20) and 27 HC performed an ER task with neutral, positive, and negative images. Participants were instructed to either attend these pictures or to down-regulate their upcoming emotions using cognitive reappraisal. Subjective arousal and wellbeing, self-reported dissociation, and electrocardiogram data were assessed.

Results

Independent of ER instruction and picture valence, both patient groups (BPD and BPD + PTSD) reported higher subjective arousal and lower wellbeing; patients with BPD + PTSD further exhibited significantly lower HF-HRV compared with the other groups. Higher self-reported state dissociation predicted higher HF-HRV during down-regulating v. attending negative pictures in BPD + PTSD.

Conclusions

Findings suggest increased emotional reactivity to negative, positive, and neutral pictures, but do not provide evidence for deficits in instructed ER in BPD. Reduced HF-HRV appears to be particularly linked to comorbid PTSD, while dissociation may underlie attempts to increase ER and HF-HRV in BPD patients with this comorbidity.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018

Introduction

Borderline personality disorder (BPD) is a severe mental disorder, characterized by a marked instability in affect, self-image, and interpersonal relationships (APA, 2013). A complex interplay of genetic, neurobiological predispositions and stressful life events is assumed to underlie its development (Lieb et al., Reference Lieb, Zanarini, Schmahl, Linehan and Bohus2004; Crowell et al., Reference Crowell, Beauchaine and Linehan2009; Schmahl et al., Reference Schmahl, Herpertz, Bertsch, Ende, Flor, Kirsch, Lis, Meyer-Lindenberg, Rietschel, Schneider, Spanagel, Treede and Bohus2014). Traumatic stress, including severe childhood maltreatment, is highly prevalent in BPD (Battle et al., Reference Battle, Shea, Johnson, Yen, Zlotnick, Zanarini, Sanislow, Skodol, Gunderson, Grilo, McGlashan and Morey2004; Ball and Links, Reference Ball and Links2009), 30–80% of patients suffer from comorbid posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (Zanarini et al., Reference Zanarini, Frankenburg, Dubo, Sickel, Trikha, Levin and Reynolds1998; Zimmerman and Mattia, Reference Zimmerman and Mattia1999; Grant et al., Reference Grant, Chou, Goldstein, Huang, Stinson, Saha, Smith, Dawson, Pulay, Pickering and Ruan2008; Pagura et al., Reference Pagura, Stein, Bolton, Cox, Grant and Sareen2010; Westphal et al., Reference Westphal, Olfson, Bravova, Gameroff, Gross, Wickramaratne, Pilowsky, Neugebauer, Shea, Lantigua, Weissman and Neria2013; Frías and Palma, Reference Frías and Palma2015). Although traumatic experiences are neither necessary nor sufficient for the development of BPD, (post)traumatic stress seems to aggravate symptoms, such as emotion dysregulation and dissociation (Scheiderer et al., Reference Scheiderer, Wood and Trull2015; Cackowski et al., Reference Cackowski, Neubauer and Kleindienst2016).

Emotion dysregulation is a core feature of BPD, involving increased sensitivity and reactivity to emotional stimuli, chronic affective instability, and maladaptive stress coping, e.g. non-suicidal self-harm (Crowell et al., Reference Crowell, Beauchaine and Linehan2009; Carpenter and Trull, Reference Carpenter and Trull2013; Santangelo et al., Reference Santangelo, Reinhard, Koudela-Hamila, Bohus, Holtmann, Eid and Ebner-Priemer2017). There is ample evidence for self-reported difficulties in emotion regulation (ER) (Rosenthal et al., Reference Rosenthal, Gratz, Kosson, Cheavens, Lejuez and Lynch2008; Glenn and Klonsky, Reference Glenn and Klonsky2009; Carpenter and Trull, Reference Carpenter and Trull2013) and altered reactivity in fronto-limbic brain regions implicated in emotional processing and regulation (Krause-Utz et al., Reference Krause-Utz, Winter, Niedtfeld and Schmahl2014; Schmahl et al., Reference Schmahl, Herpertz, Bertsch, Ende, Flor, Kirsch, Lis, Meyer-Lindenberg, Rietschel, Schneider, Spanagel, Treede and Bohus2014; van Zutphen et al., Reference van Zutphen, Siep, Jacob, Goebel and Arntz2015; Schulze et al., Reference Schulze, Schmahl and Niedtfeld2016) in BPD patients compared with healthy controls (HC). However, studies using psychophysiological markers, such as heart rate variability (HRV), revealed mixed findings (Cavazzi and Becerra, Reference Cavazzi and Becerra2014).

HRV refers to the variation in time intervals between heartbeats (beat-to-beat intervals). Vagally-mediated HR or vagal tone, i.e. parasympathetic activity exerted by the vagal nerve, is involved in the synchronization of respiratory and cardiovascular processes and assumed to play a crucial role in the flexible adaptation to stressful environmental demands, including ER (Grossman and Taylor, Reference Grossman and Taylor2007). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) and high-frequency HRV (HF-HRV, in the 0.15–0.40 Hz range) are established measures used to quantify vagal tone and to evaluate activity of the parasympathetic nervous system relative to the sympathetic nervous system (Appelhans et al., Reference Appelhans, Luecken and Candland2006; Grossman and Taylor, Reference Grossman and Taylor2007; Thayer et al., Reference Thayer, Åhs, Fredrikson, Sollers and Wager2012; Williams et al., Reference Williams, Cash, Rankin, Bernardi, Koenig and Thayer2015; Verkuil et al., Reference Verkuil, Brosschot, Tollenaar, Lane and Thayer2016).

Reduced HRV has been linked to detrimental long-term effects on physical and mental health, including increased risk for immune dysfunction, inflammation, cardiovascular diseases (Liao et al., Reference Liao, Carnethon, Evans, Cascio and Heiss2002; Kemp and Quintana, Reference Kemp and Quintana2013), major depression, anxiety (Kemp et al., Reference Kemp, Quintana, Felmingham, Matthews and Jelinek2012; Chalmers et al., Reference Chalmers, Quintana, Abbott and Kemp2014; Koenig et al., Reference Koenig, Kemp, Beauchaine, Thayer and Kaess2016a), and PTSD (Pechtel and Pizzagalli, Reference Pechtel and Pizzagalli2011; Sammito et al., Reference Sammito, Thielmann, Zimmermann and Böckelmann2015; Meyer et al., Reference Meyer, Müller, Zastrow, Schmidinger, Bohus, Herpertz and Bertsch2016).

In BPD, previous research on HRV revealed inconsistencies, which may partly relate to differences in study design (resting-state v. experimental studies) and sample characteristics (e.g. medication status, gender, trauma history, and comorbidities). As to resting-state HRV, a recent meta-analysis points to lower vagally-mediated heart rate in BPD patients compared with HC (Koenig et al., Reference Koenig, Kemp, Feeling, Thayer and Kaess2016b). The majority of studies included in this analysis involved medicated samples (Kuo and Linehan, Reference Kuo and Linehan2009; Dixon-Gordon et al., Reference Dixon-Gordon, Chapman, Lovasz and Walters2011; Gratz et al., Reference Gratz, Tull, Matusiewicz, Breetz, Lejuez and Lejuez2013) or patients with unclear medication status (Weinberg et al., Reference Weinberg, Klonsky and Hajcak2009), which may affect HRV (Ebner-Priemer et al., Reference Ebner-Priemer, Welch, Grossman, Reisch, Linehan and Bohus2007), studies including un-medicated BPD samples found no alterations in baseline HRV (Austin et al., Reference Austin, Riniolo and Porges2007; Reitz et al., Reference Reitz, Kluetsch, Niedtfeld, Knorz, Lis, Paret, Kirsch, Meyer-Lindenberg, Treede, Baumgärtner, Bohus and Schmahl2015; Meyer et al., Reference Meyer, Müller, Zastrow, Schmidinger, Bohus, Herpertz and Bertsch2016). In the study by Meyer and colleagues (Reference Meyer, Müller, Zastrow, Schmidinger, Bohus, Herpertz and Bertsch2016), during a 5-min resting-state electrocardiogram recording, a measure of HRV (RMSSD) was the lowest in un-medicated patients with PTSD, followed by un-medicated BPD patients in remission, patients with current BPD, and HC, while the latter groups did not differ significantly. Across groups, higher childhood trauma severity predicted lower HRV, suggesting a complex interplay of traumatic experiences and autonomic nervous system functioning.

Studies investigating emotional reactivity revealed similar inconsistencies: Austin et al. (Reference Austin, Riniolo and Porges2007) found no baseline differences but decreases in RSA (vagally-mediated HR), during an experimental condition with emotional film clips in BPD patients compared with HC. In contrast, Kuo and Linehan (Reference Kuo and Linehan2009) observed reduced baseline RSA and increased RSA during sad film clips in BPD.

During a stressful task, individuals with BPD features showed decreased baseline RSA and increased sympathetic activity (Weinberg et al., Reference Weinberg, Klonsky and Hajcak2009). In other studies using experimental stressors, reduced HRV was only observed in BPD patients with comorbid avoidant personality (Gratz et al., Reference Gratz, Tull, Matusiewicz, Breetz, Lejuez and Lejuez2013) and during a painful stimulation (incision into the forearm) (Reitz et al., Reference Reitz, Kluetsch, Niedtfeld, Knorz, Lis, Paret, Kirsch, Meyer-Lindenberg, Treede, Baumgärtner, Bohus and Schmahl2015). In the study by Gratz and colleagues (Reference Gratz, Tull, Matusiewicz, Breetz, Lejuez and Lejuez2013), those with BPD and comorbid avoidant personality disorder showed greater decreases in HF-HRV in response to a stressor, suggesting poor ER.

In a comprehensive assessment of emotional processing in BPD, Kuo and colleagues (Reference Kuo, Fitzpatrick, Metcalfe and Mcmain2016) investigated HRV during baseline, emotional reactivity (passive viewing of negative v. neutral images), and ER (mindfulness-based awareness v. distraction-based strategies). Patients with BPD showed reduced baseline RSA but did not differ from HC during the ER task, indicating a similar adaptation to distressing conditions. Similarly, other data suggest that individuals with BPD do not significantly differ in implementing ER skills (Metcalfe et al., Reference Metcalfe, Fitzpatrick and Kuo2015; Fitzpatrick and Kuo, Reference Fitzpatrick and Kuo2016). Moreover, and unlike HC, they showed increased HRV when instructed to accept (v. suppress) emotional experiences during social rejection (Dixon-Gordon et al., Reference Dixon-Gordon, Turner, Zachary Rosenthal and Chapman2017). Svaldi and colleagues (Reference Svaldi, Dorn, Matthies and Philipsen2012) found increases in HF-HRV from baseline to instructed ER, independent of strategy (accept/suppress emotions during a sadness-inducing film clip). All patients in this study had a lifetime diagnosis of PTSD, which might influence HF-HRV.

The above-mentioned studies mainly used negative (v. neutral) stimuli, while a growing body of literature in BPD suggests that positive stimuli are perceived as similarly disturbing (Rüsch et al., Reference Rüsch, Weber, Il'yasov, Lieb, Ebert, Hennig and van Elst2007; Sieswerda et al., Reference Sieswerda, Arntz, Mertens and Vertommen2007; Hagenhoff et al., Reference Hagenhoff, Franzen, Gerstner, Koppe, Sammer, Netter, Gallhofer and Lis2013; Thome et al., Reference Thome, Liebke, Bungert, Schmahl, Domes, Bohus and Lis2015). Moreover, potential effects of dissociation on HF-HRV in BPD remain unclear. Stress-related dissociative states, such as depersonalization, derealization, and numbing are a core symptom of BPD, occurring in 75–80% of patients (APA, 2013). Research in the dissociative subtype of PTSD and depersonalization suggests that dissociation may be a form of emotion over-modulation, promoting a dampening of stressful (trauma-related) emotions, probably at the cost of other mental resources crucial to goal-directed behavior, such as learning and memory (Sierra and Berrios, Reference Sierra and Berrios1998; Lanius et al., Reference Lanius, Vermetten, Loewenstein, Brand, Schmahl, Bremner and Spiegel2010). Dissociation was found to dampen (reduce/attenuate) skin conductance responses (SCR) (Ebner-Priemer et al., Reference Ebner-Priemer, Badeck, Beckmann, Wagner, Feige, Weiss, Lieb and Bohus2005, Reference Ebner-Priemer, Mauchnik, Kleindienst, Schmahl, Peper, Rosenthal, Flor and Bohus2009; Barnow et al., Reference Barnow, Limberg, Stopsack, Spitzer, Grabe, Freyberger and Hamm2012; Fitzpatrick and Kuo, Reference Fitzpatrick and Kuo2015) and limbic reactivity (Ebner-Priemer et al., Reference Ebner-Priemer, Badeck, Beckmann, Wagner, Feige, Weiss, Lieb and Bohus2005; Barnow et al., Reference Barnow, Limberg, Stopsack, Spitzer, Grabe, Freyberger and Hamm2012; Fitzpatrick and Kuo, Reference Fitzpatrick and Kuo2015), while impairing learning and memory (Ebner-Priemer et al., Reference Ebner-Priemer, Mauchnik, Kleindienst, Schmahl, Peper, Rosenthal, Flor and Bohus2009; Krause-Utz et al., Reference Krause-Utz, Winter, Schriner, Chiu, Lis, Spinhoven, Bohus, Schmahl and Elzinga2018) in BPD. However, studies investigating effects of dissociation on HF-HRV during emotional challenge are still scarce. To our knowledge, only one study in BPD so far reported effects of dissociation on RSA during emotional recovery (Fitzpatrick and Kuo, Reference Fitzpatrick and Kuo2015). In this study, after the induction of anger (but not sadness or fear) a decrease in RSA was found, which was not specific for BPD but also observed in generalized social anxiety disorder patients. Interestingly, higher dissociation predicted reduced sympathetic reactivity, suggesting better emotional adaptation, after induction of anger and sadness. Since this study focused on emotional recovery, HRV studies using ER tasks in BPD are still needed.

This study aim to investigate HF-HRV during baseline and its adaptation during an ER (cognitive reappraisal) task with negative, positive, and neutral stimuli in BPD patients with v. without comorbid PTSD and HC, examining the influence of dissociation, while controlling for medication. We expected increased subjective emotional reactivity (higher arousal, lower wellbeing) and decreased ER (lower decrease/increase of arousal/wellbeing when instructed to down-regulate v. attend emotional pictures) in both BPD groups compared with HC. Based on previous findings (Meyer et al., Reference Meyer, Müller, Zastrow, Schmidinger, Bohus, Herpertz and Bertsch2016), HF-HRV were expected to be the lowest in patients with comorbid PTSD, followed by BPD patients without PTSD, and HC.

Methods

Participants

Initially, 68 women with BPD according to Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM)-IV (American Psychiatric Association, APA, 2000), including 37 patients without PTSD and 31 patients with comorbid PTSD (BPD + PTSD), and 28 female HC participated. Inclusion criteria were ability to understand/give informed consent, aged 18–55, and female gender. Since the majority of BPD patients are female (APA, 2013) and gender may have a confounding effect on HRV (Verkuil et al., Reference Verkuil, Brosschot, Marques, Kampschroer, Sternberg and Thayer2015), we included only women. Exclusion criteria for patients were lifetime history of bipolar disorder, psychotic disorder, acute life-threatening suicidal crisis, mental deficiency, and severe organic disorder. Exclusion criterion for HC was lifetime history of psychiatric disorders and severe organic disorders. Psychotropic medication was no exclusion criterion but controlled for in the analysis and matched between the two patient groups (see Table 1).

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of patients with BPD + PTSD, patients without comorbid PTSD (BPD), and HC

Values are presented in means ± s.d. or frequencies (n) and percentages (%).

BDI-II, Beck Depression Inventory-II; BSL-23, Borderline Symptom List 23; DERS, Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (*higher scores indicate more difficulties in ER); Nonacceptance, nonacceptance of emotions, Goals, difficulties in goal directed behavior, Impulse, impulse control difficulties, Awareness, lack of emotional awareness, Strategies, limited access to strategies, Clarity, lack of emotional clarity; DES, Dissociative Experience Scale; DSS-4, Dissociation Stress Scale 4; ERQ, Emotion regulation questionnaire; MWT-B, Mehrfachwortschatztest; STAI, State Anxiety Inventory, **CTQ scores were available in n = 19 BPD and n = 14 BPD + PTSD. Groups with different superscripts (a, b, c) differ significantly (p < 0.05).

Patients were recruited through existing databases within the Clinical Research Unit 256 (Schmahl et al., Reference Schmahl, Herpertz, Bertsch, Ende, Flor, Kirsch, Lis, Meyer-Lindenberg, Rietschel, Schneider, Spanagel, Treede and Bohus2014) and advertisements in newspapers or internet platforms within a larger longitudinal training study (reported elsewhere). HC were recruited from a pool of healthy subjects that previously agreed to participate in future studies. All participants underwent standardized diagnostic interviews by trained clinical psychologists, including the Structured Interview for DSM-IV (SCID-I) (First et al., Reference First, Spitzer, Gibbon and Williams1997) and International Personality Disorder Examination (IPDE) (Loranger, Reference Loranger1999) to assess BPD or to assure that diagnostic criteria for BPD were still met in patients recruited from a database. Further assessments included a test of intelligence (MWT) (Lehrl, Reference Lehrl1989), questionnaires on BPD symptom severity (Borderline Symptom List 23, BSL-23) (Bohus et al., Reference Bohus, Kleindienst, Limberger, Stieglitz, Domsalla, Chapman, Steil, Philipsen and Wolf2009), depression (Beck Depression Inventory, BDI) (Hautzinger et al., Reference Hautzinger, Keller and Kühner2009), trait dissociation (Dissociation Experience Scale, DES) (Bernstein and Putnam, Reference Bernstein and Putnam1986), and anxiety (State Anxiety Questionnaire, STAI) (Laux et al., Reference Laux, Glanzmann, Schaffner and Spielberger1981). Subjective ER was assessed by the Difficulties in ER Scale (DERS) (Gratz and Roemer, Reference Gratz and Roemer2004) and a modified version of the ER Questionnaire (ERQ) (Gross and John, Reference Gross and John2003) (online Supplementary Material).

From 68 patients, six patients did not complete the study due to increased distress. Five BPD patients and one HC whose HRV data were contaminated by artifacts, caused by technical problems, were excluded from the final analysis. The final sample comprised 27 HC, 37 patients without PTSD (BPD), and 20 patients with comorbid PTSD (BPD + PTSD). Overall, 23 patients received psychotropic medication, approximately 40.5% per group (BPD: n = 15; BPD + PTSD: n = 8; Table 1 and online Supplementary Table S1).

There were no group differences in age, years of education, and intelligence (Table 1). As expected, both patient groups scored significantly higher on clinical measures than HC. The two patient groups did not differ significantly in BPD symptom severity, depressive symptoms, difficulties in ER, use of cognitive reappraisal or suppression (Table 1), and comorbidities other than PTSD (Table 2). Patients in the BPD + PTSD group reported higher trait dissociation (DES) and more severe childhood trauma (CTQ) (Table 1).

Table 2. List of medications and comorbidities in patients with BPD + PTSD and BPD patients without comorbid PTSD (BPD)

Values are presented in means ± s.d. or frequencies (n) and percentages (%).

GAD, generalized anxiety disorder; OCD, obsessive compulsive disorder; SARI, serotonin antagonist and reuptake inhibitor; SSRI, selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors; SNRI, serotonin and norepinephrine reuptake inhibitor.

Material

The ER task was an adapted cognitive reappraisal paradigm (Ochsner et al., Reference Ochsner, Bunge, Gross and Gabrieli2002; Koenigsberg et al., Reference Koenigsberg, Fan, Ochsner, Liu, Guise, Pizzarello, Dorantes, Guerreri, Tecuta, Goodman, New and Siever2009; Schulze et al., Reference Schulze, Domes, Krüger, Berger, Fleischer, Prehn, Schmahl, Grossmann, Hauenstein and Herpertz2011). Participants were instructed to either pay attention to the pictures without actively attempting to regulate upcoming emotions (attend), or to down-regulate upcoming emotions, e.g. reinterpret the content of pictures. Following a standardized procedure, two practice trials with examples and feedback were provided to ensure participants understood task instructions correctly. The task involved 105 pictures of three stimulus categories (42 negative, 42 positive, and 21 neutral pictures) from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS) (Lang et al., Reference Lang, Bradley and Cuthbert2008). Based on existing norms, ‘negative’ stimuli were selected based on high arousal (mean = 6.41, s.d. = 2.19), and low valence ratings (1.79 ± 1.18), ‘positive’ pictures had high arousal (4.99 ± 2.38) and high valence ratings (8.02 ± 1.26), and ‘neutral’ pictures had low arousal (3.19 ± 1.93) and moderate valence ratings (5.23 ± 1.23) (see online Supplementary Material). Using a randomized picture block design, 15 picture blocks of seven pictures were presented, starting with an instruction (attend or downregulate) (4s), followed by seven pictures (6s). The five task conditions (neutral_attend, positive_attend, negative_attend, positive_downregulate, and negative_downregulate) were presented threefold each (online Supplementary Fig. S1). Order of picture blocks and instructions was randomized and balanced per condition resulting in 12 pseudorandom sequences. Before and after each picture block, participants rated their subjective arousal (between 1 = ‘not at all aroused’ and 10 = ‘extremely aroused’) and emotional well-being (−10 = ‘not at all well’, +10 = ‘extremely well’) on a visual analog scale (Self-Assessment Manikin, SAM) (Bradley and Lang, Reference Bradley and Lang1994). At baseline, before and after the task, participants completed the Dissociation Stress Scale 4 (DSS-4) (Stiglmayr et al., Reference Stiglmayr, Schmahl, Bremner, Bohus and Ebner-Priemer2009), i.e. four items on acute dissociative states and one item on aversive inner tension (between ‘0 = not at all’ and ‘10 = extremely’). Presentation software (Neurobehavioral Systems, Inc., Berkeley, USA) was used to present stimuli via a 17.3″-monitor located on a table in the laboratory.

Procedure

The study was approved by the local ethics committee of Heidelberg University. All participants were informed about the study. After giving written informed consent, they underwent clinical assessments. The experiment was conducted in a laboratory at the Institute for Psychiatric and Psychosomatic Psychotherapy of the Central Institute of Mental Health (CIMH) in Mannheim. Electrocardiogram (EKG) data were continuously obtained during the experiment, according to guidelines of the Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology (Malik, Reference Malik1996) using devices of the Biosemi Active Two system with a sampling rate of 1024 Hz (Biosemi, Amsterdam, Netherlands, ActiView software; http://www.biosemi.com). Two Ag/AgCl electrodes were placed under the right clavicle and on the left side under the lowest rib. Participants were instructed to sit still, while a 5-min baseline assessment of HRV was conducted. To create a relaxing atmosphere, a soundless 5-min nature film was presented in the background. Then, the experimenter left the room and participants performed the ER task (13 min). Participants further performed a working memory task (reported elsewhere). At the end, subjects were debriefed, thanked, and paid for their participation (12€/h).

Statistical analysis

Using software IBM SPSS Statistics Version 23, group and task effects on the dependent variables (DVs) were evaluated using separate analyses of variance (ANOVA), described below. Assumptions of normality, linearity, homoscedasticity, and sphericity (Kolmogorov–Smirnov tests, visual inspection of graphic plots, Mauchly's test with Greenhouse–Geisser-corrections) and equality of variances (Levene's test) were checked. Significant ANOVA effects were followed up using Tukey's HSD tests for multiple comparisons, independent-sample t tests, and paired t tests, respectively. Significance level for all analyses was p = 0.05, two-tailed. For significant effects, partial η 2Footnote Footnote 1 or Cohen's d Footnote 2 are reported (Cohen, Reference Cohen1988). To illustrate changes in DVs for ‘down-regulatev.attend’, contrasts between these ER conditions were calculated for positive and negative pictures (regulate_minus_attend_negative and regulate_minus_attend_positive, respectively).

Ratings

Ratings of arousal and emotional well-being were analyzed using separate repeated measures ANOVAs (rm-ANOVAs). To evaluate emotional reactivity, a 3 × 3 repeated rm-ANOVA with group as between-subject factor and valence of pictures in the passive viewing condition (negative_attend, positive_attend, and neutral_attend) as within-factor was performed. To analyze ER, a 3 × 2 × 2 rm-ANOVA with instruction (attend, downregulate) and valence (negative, positive) as within-factors was conducted. No 3 × 2 × 3 rm-ANOVA could be performed, as there was only one instruction (attend) for neutral pictures.

HF-HRV

EKG data were preprocessed using software Kubios HRV [Finland, http://kubios.uef.fi/ (Tarvainen et al., Reference Tarvainen, Niskanen, Lipponen, Ranta-aho and Karjalainen2014)]. Recorded data were visually inspected for false or undetected R-waves, data with technical or physiological artifacts were excluded. Spectral analysis was used to calculate high frequency variations in beat-to-beat intervals (0.15–0.40 Hz). As recommended by current guidelines (Malik, Reference Malik1996) and to improve robustness of data, HF-HRV measures of the 15 ER task blocks were averaged for each condition, resulting in five DVs (neutral_attend, positive_attend, negative_attend, positive_downregulate, and negative_downregulate). Data were analyzed using an autoregressive (AR) model, which is independent of signal length (Parati et al., Reference Parati, Saul, Di Rienzo and Mancia1995; Di Simplicio et al., Reference Di Simplicio, Costoloni, Western, Hanson, Taggart and Harmer2012). HF-HRV measures are reported in normative units, evaluating the relative percentage (%) of parasympathic to sympathic activity, which is thought to reduce noise stemming from artifacts (Malik, Reference Malik1996). Analyses were also performed for absolute values. Group differences in baseline HF-HRV were tested using a univariate ANOVA. HF-HRV during the ER task was evaluated equivalent to the above-mentioned analysis of ratings (emotional reactivity: 3 × 3 rm-ANOVA, ER: 3 × 2 × 2 rm-ANOVA).

Post-hoc analyses: Since 21 patients had current major depression, which may affect HF-HRV (Kemp et al., Reference Kemp, Quintana, Felmingham, Matthews and Jelinek2012; Koenig et al., Reference Koenig, Kemp, Beauchaine, Thayer and Kaess2016a), additional HF-HRV analyses excluding patients with this comorbidity were performed.

Medication: In separate rm-ANCOVAs, medication status (no medication, antidepressants, and antipsychotics) was included as statistical covariate to test for effects on HF-HRV results. Additionally, all HF-HRV analyses were repeated excluding medicated patients.

Dissociation: Changes in dissociation over the course of the experiment were evaluated using a 2 × 3 rm-ANOVA with group (BPD + PTSD, BPD) as between-subject factor and time (baseline, before task, and after task) as within-subject factor.

To test whether dissociation predicted emotional reactivity/regulation, multiple regression analyses were performed, with mean DSS-4 scores at baseline, before and after task as predictors and HF-HRV values for neutral_attend, positive_attend, negative_attend (emotional reactivity), regulate_minus_attend_negative, and regulate_minus_attend_positive (emotional regulation), respectively as DVs. It was checked whether results changed after adding arousal, DERS, ERQ, BSL, BDI, and STAI scores as covariates. Regression analyses were performed for BPD and BPD + PTSD separately, adjusting significance levels for the number of groups (p ⩽ 0.025, two-tailed).

Results

Subjective ratings

Results of the rm-ANOVAs and post-hoc tests are reported in Table 3.

Table 3. Results of the ANOVA and post-hoc group tests for subjective ratings

BPD + PTSD, borderline personality disorder patients with comorbid posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD); BPD, borderline personality disorder patients without comorbid posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD); HC, healthy controls.

Emotional reactivity

Arousal: Arousal was higher in both BPD and BPD + PTSD compared with HC (Fig. 1a). This group difference was not influenced by picture valence. Valence had a significant main effect: arousal ratings were higher for negative than for neutral and positive pictures. There was no significant interaction effect.

Fig. 1. Means and standard errors of the mean of subjective ratings for arousal (a) and emotional well-being (b) in patients with BPD + PTSD, BPD patients without comorbid PTSD (BPD), and HC during the ER task. The left graphs depict arousal ratings for neutral, positive, and negative pictures in the passive viewing conditions (negative_attend, positive_attend, neutral_attend, and e motional reactivity). The right graphs illustrate changes in ratings for the ‘down-regulate’ minus ‘attend’ condition (regulate_minus_attend_negative, regulate_minus_attend_positive, and ER).

Emotional well-being: A significant group effect, with lower well-being in BPD than in HC was found (Fig. 1b). Picture valence had a significant main effect: attending negative stimuli resulting in lower well-being compared with neutral and positive pictures. Attending positive stimuli resulted in higher well-being compared with neutral pictures. The interaction effect was not significant.

ER

Arousal: Ratings were higher in both BPD and BPD + PTSD compared with HC. This group difference was not influenced by ER instruction or picture valence. Valence influenced arousal ratings, with higher arousal for negative v. positive pictures, and significantly interacted with instruction: arousal ratings for negative pictures were higher in the ‘attend’ v. ‘down-regulate’ condition, while the opposite was found for positive pictures.

Well-being: Groups differed in emotional well-being, with lower ratings in BPD than HC. Picture valence influenced the ratings, with lower well-being for negative v. positive pictures. Instruction had a significant main effect (well-being was lower for ‘attend’ v. ‘down-regulate’) and significantly interacted with valence: instructions to down-regulate increased well-being for negative pictures but decreased well-being for positive pictures.

HF-HRV

Results of the ANOVAs and post-hoc tests for HF-HRV (n.u.) are summarized in Table 4 (for absolute HF-HRV see online Supplementary Table S2 and Fig. S2).

Table 4. Results of the ANOVA and post-hoc group tests for HF-HRV for the whole group (HC: n = 27, BPD + PTSD: n = 20, BPD: n = 37)

BPD + PTSD, borderline personality disorder patients with comorbid posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD); BPD, borderline personality disorder patients without comorbid posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD); HC, healthy controls.

Baseline: There was a trend for a group effect, with lower HF-HRV in BPD + PTSD than in HC and BPD.

Emotional reactivity: The group effect was significant: BPD + PTSD showed lower HF-HRV than HC and (as a trend) BPD, independent of picture valence and ER instruction. The other effects were not significant.

ER: The group effect was again significant, with significantly lower HF-HRV in BPD + PTSD compared with BPD and HC, independent of picture valence (positive, negative), and instruction (attend, down-regulate) (Fig. 2). No other effects reached statistical significance.

Fig. 2. Means and standard errors of the mean of HF-HRV (normalized units) in patients with BPD + PTSD, BPD patients without comorbid PTSD (BPD), and HC during baseline conditions and the ER task. The left graph shows HF-HRV during baseline and the passive viewing conditions of the ER task (baseline, negative_attend, positive_attend, neutral_attend, and e motional reactivity). The right graph depicts changes in HF-HRV for the ‘down-regulate’ minus ‘attend’ condition (regulate_minus_attend_negative, regulate_minus_attend_positive, and ER).

Post-hoc analysis: Results remained stable when excluding patients with major depression (online Supplementary Table S3).

Medication: Medication status was not a significant covariate in any analysis and did not change results (online Supplementary Table S4a). When repeating the analyses excluding medicated patients, baseline differences became significant, with lower HF-HRV in BPD + PTSD than in the other groups, while results for the ER task did not change (online Supplementary Table S4b).

Dissociation during the ER task: A main effect with significant increases in dissociation was found (F (2,54) = 12.11, p < 0.0001, η 2 = 0.31). There was no significant group effect (F (1,55) = 2.70, p = 0.106, η 2 = 0.05) but a significant interaction effect (F (2,54) = 3.50, p = 0.037, η 2 = 0.12) with stronger increases in dissociation during task compared with baseline in BPD + PTSD (0.63 ± 0.14) than in the BPD group (1.58 ± 0.22) (t (55) = 1.71, p = 0.057).

Results of the multiple regression analyses are reported in online Supplementary Table S5. In BPD, no effect reached statistical significance (all p > 0.025). In BPD + PTSD, dissociation positively predicted HF-HRV during regulating minus attending negative pictures (F (3,19) = 4.08, p = 0.025, R 2 = 0.433, R 2(adjusted) = 0.327), with DSS-4 scores at baseline being a significant unique predictor, when controlling for dissociation at other time points (B = 0.812, s.e. = 0.233, β = 1.10, t = 3.49, p = 0.003, online Supplementary Fig. S3). Results remained significant, when controlling for arousal, STAI, ERQ, DERS, BDI, and BSL scores as covariateFootnote 3 (online Supplementary Table S5, for correlations see online Supplementary Table S6).

Discussion

This study investigated HF-HRV during baseline and an ER paradigm with positive, negative, and neutral pictures in 20 BPD patients with comorbid PTSD (BPD + PTSD), 37 patients without this comorbidity (BPD), and 27 HC. BPD patients, both with and without co-occurring PTSD, reported increased arousal and lower wellbeing, independent of ER instruction and picture valence. BPD + PTSD showed significantly lower HF-HRV than HC and BPD. State dissociation at baseline positively predicted HF-HRV for down-regulating (minus attending) negative pictures in BPD + PTSD.

Subjective ratings

Altered emotional processing is a core feature of BPD. Ample studies revealed subjectively increased emotional sensitivity, reactivity, instability (Crowell et al., Reference Crowell, Beauchaine and Linehan2009; Carpenter and Trull, Reference Carpenter and Trull2013; Santangelo et al., Reference Santangelo, Reinhard, Koudela-Hamila, Bohus, Holtmann, Eid and Ebner-Priemer2017), and maladaptive ER strategies (e.g. suppression) in BPD (Beblo et al., Reference Beblo, Pastuszak, Griepenstroh, Fernando, Driessen, Schütz, Rentzsch and Schlosser2010; Salsman and Linehan, Reference Salsman and Linehan2012). Consistent with this, patients in the present study reported more difficulties in ER (DERS), more suppression, and less cognitive reappraisal (ERQ) than HC. Partly in line with our hypothesis, BPD patients in both groups reported increased emotional reactivity in terms of higher arousal and lower well-being during the ER task. High aversive arousal is a basic characteristic of BPD (Stiglmayr et al., Reference Stiglmayr, Ebner-Priemer, Bretz, Behm, Mohse, Lammers, Anghelescu, Schmahl, Schlotz, Kleindienst and Bohus2008) and those affected tend to perceive neutral (ambiguous) pictures as more arousing (Donegan et al., Reference Donegan, Sanislow, Blumberg, Fulbright, Lacadie, Skudlarski, Gore, Olson, McGlashan and Wexler2003; Domes et al., Reference Domes, Schulze and Herpertz2009; Dyck et al., Reference Dyck, Habel, Slodczyk, Schlummer, Backes, Schneider and Reske2009; Unoka et al., Reference Unoka, Fogd, Füzy and Csukly2011; Daros et al., Reference Daros, Zakzanis and Ruocco2013). Moreover, there is growing evidence that altered emotion processing in BPD does not only apply to negative material but also to positive stimuli (Rüsch et al., Reference Rüsch, Weber, Il'yasov, Lieb, Ebert, Hennig and van Elst2007; Sieswerda et al., Reference Sieswerda, Arntz, Mertens and Vertommen2007; Hagenhoff et al., Reference Hagenhoff, Franzen, Gerstner, Koppe, Sammer, Netter, Gallhofer and Lis2013; Thome et al., Reference Thome, Liebke, Bungert, Schmahl, Domes, Bohus and Lis2015). Extending these findings, our study revealed altered reactivity not only to negative stimuli but also to positive and neutral stimuli in BPD.

In contrast to our hypothesis, we did not observe group differences in ER: all groups perceived similar decreases in arousal and increases in well-being, when instructed to down-regulate emotional responses to negative pictures. Similarly, previous studies found no differences in the subjective implementation of ER strategies, such as cognitive reappraisal, distancing (Koenigsberg et al., Reference Koenigsberg, Fan, Ochsner, Liu, Guise, Pizzarello, Dorantes, Guerreri, Tecuta, Goodman, New and Siever2009; Schulze et al., Reference Schulze, Domes, Krüger, Berger, Fleischer, Prehn, Schmahl, Grossmann, Hauenstein and Herpertz2011), and distraction (Kuo et al., Reference Kuo, Fitzpatrick, Metcalfe and Mcmain2016), although BPD patients showed altered neural activity in circuits implicated in ER (Koenigsberg et al., Reference Koenigsberg, Fan, Ochsner, Liu, Guise, Pizzarello, Dorantes, Guerreri, Tecuta, Goodman, New and Siever2009; Schulze et al., Reference Schulze, Domes, Krüger, Berger, Fleischer, Prehn, Schmahl, Grossmann, Hauenstein and Herpertz2011; Paret et al., Reference Paret, Kluetsch, Zaehringer, Ruf, Demirakca, Bohus, Ende and Schmahl2016; Schmitt et al., Reference Schmitt, Winter, Niedtfeld, Herpertz and Schmahl2016; Zilverstand et al., Reference Zilverstand, Parvaz and Goldstein2017). In part, methodological aspects may explain these discrepancies, as subjective ratings generally involve the risk of conscious and/or unconscious response biases.

HF-HRV

Despite similar self-appraisals in both BPD groups, only patients with comorbid PTSD showed significant HF-HRV alterations during the ER task. Again, group differences were independent of instruction and picture valence: HF-HRV levels were significantly lower in BPD + PTSD than in HC and (as a trend) in BPD across all conditions. When excluding medicated patients, this pattern remained significant. Extending previous research (Meyer et al., Reference Meyer, Müller, Zastrow, Schmidinger, Bohus, Herpertz and Bertsch2016), our findings indicate a significant impact of comorbid PTSD on HF-HRV, not only under baseline conditions but also during an ER task in BPD, pointing to complex interactions between (post)traumatic stress and autonomic nervous system functioning (Pechtel and Pizzagalli, Reference Pechtel and Pizzagalli2011; Sammito et al., Reference Sammito, Thielmann, Zimmermann and Böckelmann2015). Other variables that may affect HF-HRV, such as depressive symptoms, BPD symptom severity, and comorbidities other than PTSD did not differ between the patient groups, rendering it unlikely that they accounted for the observed group differences in HF-HRV.

In line with previous studies applying ER tasks, we did not find evidence for significant deficits in ER, i.e. HF-HRV differences dependent on the ER instruction, in BPD. Likewise, patients with BPD previously showed no differences in HRV when implementing and strengthening ER skills (Svaldi et al., Reference Svaldi, Dorn, Matthies and Philipsen2012; Metcalfe et al., Reference Metcalfe, Fitzpatrick and Kuo2015; Fitzpatrick and Kuo, Reference Fitzpatrick and Kuo2016; Kuo et al., Reference Kuo, Fitzpatrick, Metcalfe and Mcmain2016; Dixon-Gordon et al., Reference Dixon-Gordon, Turner, Zachary Rosenthal and Chapman2017).

Interestingly, state dissociation positively predicted HF-HRV during ER in BPD + PTSD. More specifically, patients with BPD + PTSD, who experienced higher dissociation, particularly at baseline, showed higher HF-HRV when instructed to down-regulate (minus passively attend) negative pictures. In line with this, BPD patients with higher dissociation previously showed reduced sympathetic reactivity, suggesting better emotional adaptation, after induction of anger and sadness (Fitzpatrick and Kuo, Reference Fitzpatrick and Kuo2015). Moreover, dissociation was associated with dampened startle responses, SCR (Ebner-Priemer et al., Reference Ebner-Priemer, Badeck, Beckmann, Wagner, Feige, Weiss, Lieb and Bohus2005; Ebner-Priemer et al., Reference Ebner-Priemer, Mauchnik, Kleindienst, Schmahl, Peper, Rosenthal, Flor and Bohus2009; Barnow et al., Reference Barnow, Limberg, Stopsack, Spitzer, Grabe, Freyberger and Hamm2012) and limbic reactivity (Krause-Utz et al., Reference Krause-Utz, Frost, Winter and Elzinga2017) to stressful material in BPD. At the same time, dissociation was found to interfere with learning and memory (Ebner-Priemer et al., Reference Ebner-Priemer, Mauchnik, Kleindienst, Schmahl, Peper, Rosenthal, Flor and Bohus2009; Krause-Utz et al., Reference Krause-Utz, Frost, Winter and Elzinga2017) and predicted poor treatment outcome (Arntz et al., Reference Arntz, Stupar-Rutenfrans, Bloo, van Dyck and Spinhoven2015; Kleindienst et al., Reference Kleindienst, Priebe, Görg, Dyer, Steil, Lyssenko, Winter, Schmahl and Bohus2016). In the context of these earlier findings, our results suggest that dissociation may underlie attempts of increasing ER, possibly at the cost of other mental processes that are crucial to goal-directed behavior (Lanius et al., Reference Lanius, Vermetten, Loewenstein, Brand, Schmahl, Bremner and Spiegel2010). More specifically, our present findings provide preliminary evidence for an impact of dissociation on HF-HRV during ER in BPD patients with comorbid PTSD. Much more research is needed to shed more light on this relationship and how it may relate to treatment outcome. Future studies should investigate effects of experimentally induced dissociation on HF-HRV during an ER task to gain more insight into possible causal relationships.

Limitations

To our knowledge, this is the first study in BPD examining HF-HRV during an ER task with positive and negative stimuli, taking comorbid PTSD and dissociation into account. Findings for the ER task remained stable after excluding medicated patients, rendering it unlikely that results are explained by medication status. However, exclusion of medicated patients reduced our sample sizes, restricting the statistical power to detect differential effects. As we only included women, findings cannot be generalized to male populations. Since we did not include a control group of PTSD patients without BPD, we cannot conclude whether findings are specific to PTSD or the co-occurrence of BPD + PTSD, which may worsen psychopathology. While patient groups did not differ concerning comorbidities other than PTSD, comorbidities in these groups may have affected results. Findings did not change after excluding patients with comorbid major depression. However, studies with larger subsamples of patients with and without comorbid depression are needed to replicate our findings. Moreover, the ecological validity of our ER paradigm may be limited. Future study should link HF-HRV to ER in every-day life, especially in interpersonal situations, which may be particularly challenging for individuals with BPD (Schmahl et al., Reference Schmahl, Herpertz, Bertsch, Ende, Flor, Kirsch, Lis, Meyer-Lindenberg, Rietschel, Schneider, Spanagel, Treede and Bohus2014).

In conclusion, reduced HF-HRV during baseline and an ER task may be particularly linked to comorbid PTSD. Dissociation may underlie attempts to increase ER and HF-HRV in BPD patients with this comorbidity, which should be considered in future research and treatment.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291718002489

Acknowledgements

This study has been funded by the German Research Foundation (DFG), KFO 256 ‘Mechanisms of Disturbed Emotion Processing in Patients with Borderline Personality Disorder’. The authors thank all participants of this study for their crucial contribution and Rachel Frost, Mark Adam, Majbritt Jepsen, and Sophia Scholtes for their support in the pre-processing of the HF-HRV data.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Footnotes

The notes appear after the main text.

1 η p2 ⩽ 0.039 representing small effects, η p2 = 0.04–0.139 mediate effects, and η p2 ⩾ 0.14 large effects.

2 d = 0.20 representing small effects, d = 0.50 mediate effects, and d = 0.80 large effects.

3 When controlling for arousal (B = 0.795, s.e. = 0.251, β = 1.08, t = 3.17, p = 0.006), STAI (B = 0.984, s.e. = 0.305, β = 1.47, t = 3.23, p = 0.006), ERQ reappraisal (B = 0.727, s.e. = 0.238, β = 0.984, t = 3.06, p = 0.008), ERQ suppression (B = 0.809, s.e. = 0.239, β = 1.10, t = 3.38, p = 0.004), DERS (B = 0.919, s.e. = 0.247, β = 1.25, t = 3.73, p = 0.002), BDI (B = 0.887, s.e. = 0.260, β = 1.20, t = 3.41, p = 0.004), and BSL (B = 1.01, s.e. = 0.219, β = 1.37, t = 4.60, p < 0.001).

References

American Psychiatric Association (APA) (2000) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (APA) (2013) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Appelhans, BM, Luecken, LJ and Candland, DK (2006) Heart rate variability as an index of regulated emotional responding. Review of General Psychology 10, 229240.Google Scholar
Arntz, A, Stupar-Rutenfrans, S, Bloo, J, van Dyck, R and Spinhoven, P (2015) Prediction of treatment discontinuation and recovery from Borderline Personality Disorder: results from an RCT comparing Schema Therapy and Transference Focused Psychotherapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy 74, 6071.Google Scholar
Austin, MA, Riniolo, TC and Porges, SW (2007) Borderline personality disorder and emotion regulation: insights from the Polyvagal Theory. Brain and Cognition 65, 6976.Google Scholar
Ball, J and Links, P (2009) Borderline personality disorder and childhood trauma: evidence for a causal relationship. Current Psychiatry Reports 11, 6368.Google Scholar
Barnow, S, Limberg, A, Stopsack, M, Spitzer, C, Grabe, HJ, Freyberger, HJ and Hamm, A (2012) Dissociation and emotion regulation in borderline personality disorder. Psychological Medicine 42, 783794.Google Scholar
Battle, CL, Shea, MT, Johnson, DM, Yen, S, Zlotnick, C, Zanarini, MC, Sanislow, CA, Skodol, AE, Gunderson, JG, Grilo, CM, McGlashan, TH and Morey, LC (2004) Childhood maltreatment associated with adult personality disorders: findings from the Collaborative Longitudinal Personality Disorders Study. Journal of Personality Disorders 18, 193211.Google Scholar
Beblo, T, Pastuszak, A, Griepenstroh, J, Fernando, S, Driessen, M, Schütz, A, Rentzsch, K and Schlosser, N (2010) Self-reported emotional dysregulation but no impairment of emotional intelligence in borderline personality disorder: an explorative study. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 198, 385388.Google Scholar
Bernstein, EM and Putnam, FW (1986) Development, reliability, and validity of a dissociation scale. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 174, 727735.Google Scholar
Bohus, M, Kleindienst, N, Limberger, MF, Stieglitz, RD, Domsalla, M, Chapman, AL, Steil, R, Philipsen, A and Wolf, M (2009) The short version of the Borderline Symptom List (BSL-23): development and initial data on psychometric properties. Psychopathology 42, 3239.Google Scholar
Bradley, NN and Lang, PJ (1994) Measuring emotion: the Self-Assessment Manikin and the Semantic Differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 25, 4959.Google Scholar
Cackowski, S, Neubauer, T and Kleindienst, N (2017) The impact of posttraumatic stress disorder on the symptomatology of borderline personality disorder. Borderline Personal Disord Emot Dysregul 4, 6. doi: 10.1186/s40479-017-0057-5.Google Scholar
Carpenter, RW and Trull, TJ (2013) Components of emotion dysregulation in borderline personality disorder: a review. Current Psychiatry Reports 15, 335.Google Scholar
Cavazzi, T and Becerra, R (2014) Psychophysiological research of borderline personality disorder: review and implications for biosocial theory. Europe's Journal of Psychology 10, 185203.Google Scholar
Chalmers, J, Quintana, D, Abbott, M and Kemp, A (2014) Anxiety disorders are associated with reduced heart rate variability: a meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychiatry 5, 111.Google Scholar
Cohen, J (1988) Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Crowell, SE, Beauchaine, TP and Linehan, MM (2009) A biosocial developmental model of borderline personality: elaborating and extending Linehan's theory. Psychological Bulletin 135, 495510.Google Scholar
Daros, AR, Zakzanis, KK and Ruocco, AC (2013) Facial emotion recognition in borderline personality disorder. Psychological Medicine 43, 19531963.Google Scholar
Di Simplicio, M, Costoloni, G, Western, D, Hanson, B, Taggart, P and Harmer, CJ (2012) Decreased heart rate variability during emotion regulation in subjects at risk for psychopathology. Psychological Medicine 42, 17751783.Google Scholar
Dixon-Gordon, KL, Chapman, AL, Lovasz, N and Walters, K (2011) Too upset to think: the interplay of borderline personality features, negative emotions, and social problem solving in the laboratory. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment 2, 243.Google Scholar
Dixon-Gordon, KL, Turner, BJ, Zachary Rosenthal, M and Chapman, AL (2017) Emotion regulation in borderline personality disorder: an experimental investigation of the effects of instructed acceptance and suppression. Behavior Therapy 48, 750764.Google Scholar
Domes, G, Schulze, L and Herpertz, S (2009) Emotion recognition in borderline personality disorder - a review of the literature. Journal of Personality Disorders 23, 619.Google Scholar
Donegan, NH, Sanislow, CA, Blumberg, HP, Fulbright, RK, Lacadie, C, Skudlarski, P, Gore, JC, Olson, IR, McGlashan, TH and Wexler, BE (2003) Amygdala hyperreactivity in borderline personality disorder: implications for emotional dysregulation. Biological Psychiatry 54, 12841293.Google Scholar
Dyck, M, Habel, U, Slodczyk, J, Schlummer, J, Backes, V, Schneider, F and Reske, M (2009) Negative bias in fast emotion discrimination in borderline personality disorder. Psychological Medicine 39, 855864.Google Scholar
Ebner-Priemer, UW, Badeck, S, Beckmann, C, Wagner, A, Feige, B, Weiss, I, Lieb, K and Bohus, M (2005) Affective dysregulation and dissociative experience in female patients with borderline personality disorder: a startle response study. Journal of Psychiatric Research 39, 8592.Google Scholar
Ebner-Priemer, UW, Welch, SS, Grossman, P, Reisch, T, Linehan, MM and Bohus, M (2007) Psychophysiological ambulatory assessment of affective dysregulation in borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Research 150, 265275.Google Scholar
Ebner-Priemer, UW, Mauchnik, J, Kleindienst, N, Schmahl, C, Peper, M, Rosenthal, MZ, Flor, H and Bohus, M (2009) Emotional learning during dissociative states in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience 34, 214222.Google Scholar
First, MB, Spitzer, RL, Gibbon, M and Williams, JBW (1997) Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-V Axis I Disorders – Clinical Version (SCID-CV). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.Google Scholar
Fitzpatrick, S and Kuo, JR (2015) A comprehensive examination of delayed emotional recovery in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 47, 5159.Google Scholar
Fitzpatrick, S and Kuo, JR (2016) The impact of stimulus arousal level on emotion regulation effectiveness in borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Research 241, 242248.Google Scholar
Frías, Á and Palma, C (2015) Comorbidity between post-traumatic stress disorder and borderline personality disorder: a review. Psychopathology 48, 110.Google Scholar
Glenn, CR and Klonsky, ED (2009) Emotion dysregulation as a core feature of borderline personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders 23, 2028.Google Scholar
Grant, BF, Chou, SP, Goldstein, RB, Huang, B, Stinson, FS, Saha, TD, Smith, SM, Dawson, DA, Pulay, AJ, Pickering, RP and Ruan, WJ (2008) Prevalence, correlates, disability, and comorbidity of DSM-IV borderline personality disorder: results from the Wave 2 National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 69, 533545.Google Scholar
Gratz, K and Roemer, L (2004) Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: development, factor structure, and initial validation of the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment 26, 4154.Google Scholar
Gratz, KL, Tull, MT, Matusiewicz, AM, Breetz, AA, Lejuez, CW and Lejuez, CW (2013) Multimodal examination of emotion regulation difficulties as a function of co-occurring avoidant personality disorder among women with borderline personality disorder. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment 4, 304314.Google Scholar
Gross, JJ and John, OP (2003) Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 85, 348362.Google Scholar
Grossman, P and Taylor, EW (2007) Toward understanding respiratory sinus arrhythmia: relations to cardiac vagal tone, evolution and biobehavioral functions. Biological Psychology 74, 263285.Google Scholar
Hagenhoff, M, Franzen, N, Gerstner, L, Koppe, G, Sammer, G, Netter, P, Gallhofer, B and Lis, S (2013) Reduced sensitivity to emotional facial expressions in borderline personality disorder: effects of emotional valence and intensity. Journal of Personality Disorders 27, 1935.Google Scholar
Hautzinger, M, Keller, F and Kühner, C (2009) Beck-Depressions-Inventar (BDI-II), 2nd Edn. Frankfurt: Pearson Assessment.Google Scholar
Kemp, AH and Quintana, DS (2013) The relationship between mental and physical health: insights from the study of heart rate variability. International Journal of Psychophysiology 89, 288296.Google Scholar
Kemp, AH, Quintana, DS, Felmingham, KL, Matthews, S and Jelinek, HF (2012) Depression, comorbid anxiety disorders, and heart rate variability in physically healthy, unmedicated patients: implications for cardiovascular risk. PLoS ONE 7, e30777.Google Scholar
Kleindienst, N, Priebe, K, Görg, N, Dyer, A, Steil, R, Lyssenko, L, Winter, D, Schmahl, C and Bohus, M (2016) State dissociation moderates response to dialectical behavior therapy for posttraumatic stress disorder in women with and without borderline personality disorder. European Journal of Psychotraumatology 7, 30375.Google Scholar
Koenig, J, Kemp, AH, Beauchaine, TP, Thayer, JF and Kaess, M (2016 a). Depression and resting state heart rate variability in children and adolescents - A systematic review and meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review 46, 136150.Google Scholar
Koenig, J, Kemp, AH, Feeling, NR, Thayer, JF and Kaess, M (2016 b). Resting state vagal tone in borderline personality disorder: a meta-analysis. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry 64, 1826.Google Scholar
Koenigsberg, HW, Fan, J, Ochsner, KN, Liu, X, Guise, KG, Pizzarello, S, Dorantes, C, Guerreri, S, Tecuta, L, Goodman, M, New, A and Siever, LJ (2009) Neural correlates of the use of psychological distancing to regulate responses to negative social cues: a study of patients with borderline personality disorder. Biological Psychiatry 66, 854863.Google Scholar
Krause-Utz, A, Winter, D, Niedtfeld, I and Schmahl, C (2014) The latest neuroimaging findings in borderline personality disorder. Current Psychiatry Reports 16, 438.Google Scholar
Krause-Utz, A, Frost, R, Winter, D and Elzinga, BM (2017) Dissociation and alterations in brain function and structure: implications for borderline personality disorder. Current Psychiatry Reports 19, 6.Google Scholar
Krause-Utz, A, Winter, D, Schriner, F, Chiu, CD, Lis, S, Spinhoven, P, Bohus, M, Schmahl, C and Elzinga, BM (2018) Reduced amygdala reactivity and impaired working memory during dissociation in borderline personality disorder. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 268, 401415.Google Scholar
Kuo, JR and Linehan, MM (2009) Disentangling emotion processes in borderline personality disorder: physiological and self-reported assessment of biological vulnerability, baseline intensity, and reactivity to emotionally evocative stimuli. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 118, 531544.Google Scholar
Kuo, JR, Fitzpatrick, S, Metcalfe, RK and Mcmain, S (2016) A multi-method laboratory investigation of emotional reactivity and emotion regulation abilities in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 50, 5260.Google Scholar
Lang, PJ, Bradley, MM and Cuthbert, BN (2008) International affective picture system (IAPS). In Affective Ratings of Pictures and Instruction Manual. Technical Report A-8. Gainesville, FL: University of Florida.Google Scholar
Lanius, RA, Vermetten, E, Loewenstein, RJ, Brand, B, Schmahl, C, Bremner, JD and Spiegel, D (2010) Emotion modulation in PTSD: clinical and neurobiological evidence for a dissociative subtype. American Journal of Psychiatry 167, 640647.Google Scholar
Laux, L, Glanzmann, P, Schaffner, P and Spielberger, CD (1981) State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Spielberger, C.D., Gorsuch, R.L., Lushene, R.E., 1970) - German version. Beltz: Weinheim.Google Scholar
Lehrl, S (1989) Mehrfachwahl-Wortschatz-Intelligenztest: MWT-B (2., überarb. Aufl. ed.). Perimed-Fachbuch-Verl.-Ges.: Erlangen.Google Scholar
Liao, D, Carnethon, M, Evans, GW, Cascio, WE and Heiss, G (2002) Lower heart rate variability is associated with the development of coronary heart disease in individuals with diabetes: the atherosclerosis risk in communities (ARIC) study. Diabetes 51, 35243531.Google Scholar
Lieb, K, Zanarini, MC, Schmahl, C, Linehan, MM and Bohus, M (2004) Borderline personality disorder. The Lancet 364, 453461.Google Scholar
Loranger, AW (1999) International Personality Disorder Examination (IPDE): DSM-IV and ICD-10 Modules. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Malik, M (1996) Heart rate variability. Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use. Task force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology. Annals of Noninvasive Electrocardiology 1, 151181.Google Scholar
Metcalfe, RK, Fitzpatrick, S and Kuo, JR (2015) A laboratory examination of emotion regulation skill strengthening in borderline personality disorder. Personality Disorders 8, 237246.Google Scholar
Meyer, PW, Müller, LE, Zastrow, A, Schmidinger, I, Bohus, M, Herpertz, SC and Bertsch, K (2016) Heart rate variability in patients with post-traumatic stress disorder or borderline personality disorder: relationship to early life maltreatment. Journal of Neural Transmission 23, 11071118.Google Scholar
Ochsner, KN, Bunge, SA, Gross, JJ and Gabrieli, JD (2002) Rethinking feelings: an FMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 14, 12151229.Google Scholar
Pagura, J, Stein, MB, Bolton, JM, Cox, BJ, Grant, B and Sareen, J (2010) Comorbidity of borderline personality disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder in the U.S. population. Journal of Psychiatric Research 44, 11901198.Google Scholar
Parati, G, Saul, JP, Di Rienzo, M and Mancia, G (1995) Spectral analysis of blood pressure and heart rate variability in evaluating cardiovascular regulation. A critical appraisal. Hypertension 25, 12761286.Google Scholar
Paret, C, Kluetsch, R, Zaehringer, J, Ruf, M, Demirakca, T, Bohus, M, Ende, G and Schmahl, C (2016) Alterations of amygdala-prefrontal connectivity with real-time fMRI neurofeedback in BPD patients. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 11, 952960.Google Scholar
Pechtel, P and Pizzagalli, D (2011) Effects of early life stress on cognitive and affective function: an integrated review of human literature. Psychopharmacology 214, 5570.Google Scholar
Reitz, S, Kluetsch, R, Niedtfeld, I, Knorz, T, Lis, S, Paret, C, Kirsch, P, Meyer-Lindenberg, A, Treede, RD, Baumgärtner, U, Bohus, M and Schmahl, C (2015) Incision and stress regulation in borderline personality disorder: neurobiological mechanisms of self-injurious behaviour. The British Journal of Psychiatry 207, 165172.Google Scholar
Rosenthal, MZ, Gratz, KL, Kosson, DS, Cheavens, JS, Lejuez, CW and Lynch, TR (2008) Borderline personality disorder and emotional responding: a review of the research literature. Clinical Psychology Reviews 28, 7591.Google Scholar
Rüsch, N, Weber, M, Il'yasov, KA, Lieb, K, Ebert, D, Hennig, J and van Elst, LT (2007) Inferior frontal white matter microstructure and patterns of psychopathology in women with borderline personality disorder and comorbid attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. NeuroImage 35, 738747.Google Scholar
Salsman, NL and Linehan, MM (2012) An investigation of the relationships among negative affect, difficulties in emotion regulation, and features of borderline personality disorder. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment 34, 260267.Google Scholar
Sammito, S, Thielmann, B, Zimmermann, P and Böckelmann, I (2015) Influence of post-traumatic stress disorder on heart rate variability as marker of the autonomic nervous system—a systematic review. Fortschritte der Neurologie Psychiatrie 83, 3037. [Article in German].Google Scholar
Santangelo, PS, Reinhard, I, Koudela-Hamila, S, Bohus, M, Holtmann, J, Eid, M and Ebner-Priemer, UW (2017) The temporal interplay of self-esteem instability and affective instability in Borderline Personality Disorder patients’ everyday life. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 126, 10571065.Google Scholar
Scheiderer, EM, Wood, PK and Trull, TJ (2015) The comorbidity of borderline personality disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder: revisiting the prevalence and associations in a general population sample. Borderline Personality Disorder and Emotion Dysregulation 2, 11.Google Scholar
Schmahl, C, Herpertz, SC, Bertsch, K, Ende, G, Flor, H, Kirsch, P, Lis, S, Meyer-Lindenberg, A, Rietschel, M, Schneider, M, Spanagel, R, Treede, RD and Bohus, M (2014) Mechanisms of disturbed emotion processing and social interaction in borderline personality disorder: state of the art and research agenda of the German Clinical Research Unit. Borderline Personality Disorder and Emotion Dysregulation 1, 12.Google Scholar
Schmitt, R, Winter, D, Niedtfeld, I, Herpertz, SC and Schmahl, C (2016) Effects of psychotherapy on neuronal correlates of reappraisal in female patients with borderline personality disorder. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging 1, 548557.Google Scholar
Schulze, L, Domes, G, Krüger, A, Berger, C, Fleischer, M, Prehn, K, Schmahl, C, Grossmann, A, Hauenstein, K and Herpertz, SC (2011) Neuronal correlates of cognitive reappraisal in borderline patients with affective instability. Biological Psychiatry 69, 564573.Google Scholar
Schulze, L, Schmahl, C and Niedtfeld, I (2016) Neural correlates of disturbed emotion processing in borderline personality disorder: a multimodal meta-analysis. Biological Psychiatry 79, 97106.Google Scholar
Sierra, M and Berrios, GE (1998) Depersonalization: neurobiological perspectives. Biological Psychiatry 44, 898908.Google Scholar
Sieswerda, SH, Arntz, AR, Mertens, I and Vertommen, S (2007) Hypervigilance in patients with borderline personality disorder: specificity, automaticity, and predictors. Behaviour Research and Therapy 45, 10111024.Google Scholar
Stiglmayr, CE, Ebner-Priemer, UW, Bretz, J, Behm, R, Mohse, M, Lammers, CH, Anghelescu, IG, Schmahl, C, Schlotz, W, Kleindienst, N and Bohus, M (2008) Dissociative symptoms are positively related to stress in borderline personality disorder. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 117, 139147.Google Scholar
Stiglmayr, C, Schmahl, C, Bremner, JD, Bohus, M and Ebner-Priemer, U (2009) Development and psychometric characteristics of the DSS-4 as a short instrument to assess dissociative experience during neuropsychological experiments. Psychopathology 42, 370374.Google Scholar
Svaldi, J, Dorn, C, Matthies, S and Philipsen, A (2012) Effects of suppression and acceptance of sadness on the urge for non-suicidal self-injury and self-punishment. Psychiatry Research 200, 404416.Google Scholar
Tarvainen, MP, Niskanen, JP, Lipponen, JA, Ranta-aho, PO and Karjalainen, PA (2014) Kubios HRV – Heart rate variability analysis software. Computer Methods and Programs in Biomedicine 113, 210220.Google Scholar
Thayer, JF, Åhs, F, Fredrikson, M, Sollers, JJ and Wager, TD (2012) A meta-analysis of heart rate variability and neuroimaging studies: implications for heart rate variability as a marker of stress and health. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 36, 747756.Google Scholar
Thome, J, Liebke, L, Bungert, M, Schmahl, C, Domes, G, Bohus, M and Lis, S (2015) Confidence in facial emotion recognition in borderline personality disorder. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment 7, 159168.Google Scholar
Unoka, Z, Fogd, D, Füzy, M and Csukly, G (2011) Misreading the facial signs: specific impairments and error patterns in recognition of facial emotions with negative valence in borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Research 189, 419425.Google Scholar
van Zutphen, L, Siep, N, Jacob, GA, Goebel, R and Arntz, A (2015) Emotional sensitivity, emotion regulation and impulsivity in borderline personality disorder: a critical review of fMRI studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews 51, 6476.Google Scholar
Verkuil, B, Brosschot, JF, Marques, AH, Kampschroer, K, Sternberg, EM and Thayer, JF (2015) Gender differences in the impact of daily sadness on 24-h heart rate variability. Psychophysiology 52, 16821688.Google Scholar
Verkuil, B, Brosschot, J, Tollenaar, M, Lane, R and Thayer, J (2016) Prolonged non-metabolic heart rate variability reduction as a physiological marker of psychological stress in daily life. Annals of Behavioural Medicine 50, 704714.Google Scholar
Weinberg, A, Klonsky, ED and Hajcak, G (2009) Autonomic impairment in borderline personality disorder: a laboratory investigation. Brain and Cognition 71, 279286.Google Scholar
Westphal, M, Olfson, M, Bravova, M, Gameroff, MJ, Gross, R, Wickramaratne, P, Pilowsky, DJ, Neugebauer, R, Shea, S, Lantigua, R, Weissman, M and Neria, Y (2013) Borderline personality disorder, exposure to interpersonal trauma, and psychiatric comorbidity in urban primary care patients. Psychiatry 76, 365380.Google Scholar
Williams, DP, Cash, C, Rankin, C, Bernardi, A, Koenig, J and Thayer, JF (2015) Resting heart rate variability predicts self reported difficulties in emotion regulation: a focus on different facets of emotion regulation. Frontiers in Psychology 6, 18.Google Scholar
Zanarini, MC, Frankenburg, FR, Dubo, ED, Sickel, AE, Trikha, A, Levin, A and Reynolds, V (1998) Axis II comorbidity of borderline personality disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry 39, 296302.Google Scholar
Zilverstand, A, Parvaz, MA and Goldstein, RZ (2017) Neuroimaging cognitive reappraisal in clinical populations to define neural targets for enhancing emotion regulation. A systematic review. NeuroImage 151, 105116.Google Scholar
Zimmerman, M and Mattia, JI (1999) Axis I diagnostic comorbidity and borderline personality disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry 40, 245252.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of patients with BPD + PTSD, patients without comorbid PTSD (BPD), and HC

Figure 1

Table 2. List of medications and comorbidities in patients with BPD + PTSD and BPD patients without comorbid PTSD (BPD)

Figure 2

Table 3. Results of the ANOVA and post-hoc group tests for subjective ratings

Figure 3

Fig. 1. Means and standard errors of the mean of subjective ratings for arousal (a) and emotional well-being (b) in patients with BPD + PTSD, BPD patients without comorbid PTSD (BPD), and HC during the ER task. The left graphs depict arousal ratings for neutral, positive, and negative pictures in the passive viewing conditions (negative_attend, positive_attend, neutral_attend, and emotional reactivity). The right graphs illustrate changes in ratings for the ‘down-regulate’ minus ‘attend’ condition (regulate_minus_attend_negative, regulate_minus_attend_positive, and ER).

Figure 4

Table 4. Results of the ANOVA and post-hoc group tests for HF-HRV for the whole group (HC: n = 27, BPD + PTSD: n = 20, BPD: n = 37)

Figure 5

Fig. 2. Means and standard errors of the mean of HF-HRV (normalized units) in patients with BPD + PTSD, BPD patients without comorbid PTSD (BPD), and HC during baseline conditions and the ER task. The left graph shows HF-HRV during baseline and the passive viewing conditions of the ER task (baseline, negative_attend, positive_attend, neutral_attend, and emotional reactivity). The right graph depicts changes in HF-HRV for the ‘down-regulate’ minus ‘attend’ condition (regulate_minus_attend_negative, regulate_minus_attend_positive, and ER).

Supplementary material: File

Krause-Utz et al. supplementary material

Krause-Utz et al. supplementary material 1

Download Krause-Utz et al. supplementary material(File)
File 258.3 KB