Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-g7gxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T16:02:26.822Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Altered resting-state functional connectivity in emotion-processing brain regions in adults who were born very preterm

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 August 2016

C. Papini*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, De Crespigny Park, London, UK Department of Developmental Psychology and Socialisation, University of Padua, Padua, Italy
T. P. White
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, De Crespigny Park, London, UK School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Edgbaston, Birmingham, UK
A. Montagna
Affiliation:
Division of Imaging Sciences and Biomedical Engineering, Centre for the Developing Brain, King's College London, St. Thomas’ Hospital, London, UK
P. J. Brittain
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, De Crespigny Park, London, UK
S. Froudist-Walsh
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, De Crespigny Park, London, UK
J. Kroll
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, De Crespigny Park, London, UK
V. Karolis
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, De Crespigny Park, London, UK
A. Simonelli
Affiliation:
Department of Developmental Psychology and Socialisation, University of Padua, Padua, Italy
S. C. Williams
Affiliation:
Department of Neuroimaging, Centre for Neuroimaging Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, De Crespigny Park, London, UK
R. M. Murray
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, De Crespigny Park, London, UK
C. Nosarti
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, De Crespigny Park, London, UK Department of Developmental Psychology and Socialisation, University of Padua, Padua, Italy
*
*Address for correspondence: C. Papini, Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King's College London, De Crespigny Park, London, SE5 8AF, UK. (Email: chiara.papini@kcl.ac.uk)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Very preterm birth (VPT; <32 weeks of gestation) has been associated with impairments in emotion regulation, social competence and communicative skills. However, the neuroanatomical mechanisms underlying such impairments have not been systematically studied. Here we investigated the functional integrity of the amygdala connectivity network in relation to the ability to recognize emotions from facial expressions in VPT adults.

Method

Thirty-six VPT-born adults and 38 age-matched controls were scanned at rest in a 3-T MRI scanner. Resting-state functional connectivity (rs-fc) was assessed with SPM8. A seed-based analysis focusing on three amygdalar subregions (centro-medial/latero-basal/superficial) was performed. Participants’ ability to recognize emotions was assessed using dynamic stimuli of human faces expressing six emotions at different intensities with the Emotion Recognition Task (ERT).

Results

VPT individuals compared to controls showed reduced rs-fc between the superficial subregion of the left amygdala, and the right posterior cingulate cortex (p = 0.017) and the left precuneus (p = 0.002). The VPT group further showed elevated rs-fc between the left superficial amygdala and the superior temporal sulcus (p = 0.008). Performance on the ERT showed that the VPT group was less able than controls to recognize anger at low levels of intensity. Anger scores were significantly associated with rs-fc between the superficial amygdala and the posterior cingulate cortex in controls but not in VPT individuals.

Conclusions

These findings suggest that alterations in rs-fc between the amygdala, parietal and temporal cortices could represent the mechanism linking VPT birth and deficits in emotion processing.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016

Introduction

Very preterm birth (VPT, <32 weeks’ gestation) has been associated with a higher risk of neurological and cognitive deficits (Bos & Roze, Reference Bos and Roze2011; Burnett et al. Reference Burnett, Scratch and Anderson2013), behavioural problems and learning difficulties (Johnson & Marlow, Reference Johnson and Marlow2011; Harmon et al. Reference Harmon, Taylor, Minich, Wilson-Costello and Hack2015). Individuals who were born VPT are vulnerable to socio-emotional impairments, including social isolation, peer rejection, poor social competence and shyness (Rickards et al. Reference Rickards, Kelly, Doyle and Callanan2001; Dahl et al. Reference Dahl, Kaaresen, Tunby, Handegård, Kvernmo and Rønning2006; Schmidt et al. Reference Schmidt, Miskovic, Boyle and Saigal2008; Healy et al. Reference Healy, Reichenberg, Nam, Allin, Walshe, Rifkin, Murray and Nosarti2013; Williamson & Jakobson, Reference Williamson and Jakobson2014), as well as impairments in the ability to recognize facial emotions (Potharst et al. Reference Potharst, van Wassenaer-Leemhuis, Houtzager, Livesey, Kok, Last and Oosterlaan2013; Witt et al. Reference Witt, Theurel, Tolsa, Lejeune, Fernandes, de Jonge, Monnier, Graz, Barisnikov, Gentaz and Hüppi2014). Social deficits have been described in the first 2 years of life in VPT samples (Spittle et al. Reference Spittle, Treyvaud, Doyle, Roberts, Lee, Inder, Cheong, Hunt, Newnham and Anderson2009; Boyd et al. Reference Boyd, Msall, O'Shea, Allred, Hounshell and Leviton2013); and furthermore, VPT children and adolescents have been found to have a higher risk of anxiety and depression (Burnett et al. Reference Burnett, Anderson, Cheong, Doyle, Davey and Wood2011), attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; Lindström et al. Reference Lindström, Lindblad and Hjern2011) and autism (Limperopoulos et al. Reference Limperopoulos, Bassan, Sullivan, Soul, Robertson, Moore, Ringer, Volpe and du Plessis2008; Pinto-Martin et al. Reference Pinto-Martin, Levy, Feldman, Lorenz, Paneth and Whitaker2011) compared to controls. Individuals born VPT continue to be at an increased risk for psychiatric disorders in adult life, with mood and anxiety disorders being the most prevalent (Walshe et al. Reference Walshe, Rifkin, Rooney, Healy, Nosarti, Wyatt, Stahl, Murray and Allin2008; Nosarti et al. Reference Nosarti, Reichenberg, Murray, Cnattingius, Lambe, Yin, MacCabe, Rifkin and Hultman2012; D'Onofrio et al. Reference D'Onofrio, Class, Rickert, Larsson, Långström and Lichtenstein2013). Their typical personality profile, characterized by low risk-taking, neuroticism and introversion, might also predispose them to social vulnerability and difficulties in social interactions (Allin et al. Reference Allin, Rooney, Cuddy, Wyatt, Walshe, Rifkin and Murray2006; Saigal, Reference Saigal2014; Eryigit-Madzwamuse et al. Reference Eryigit-Madzwamuse, Strauss, Baumann, Bartmann and Wolke2015).

As VPT birth is associated with early brain injury and aberrant trajectories of cerebral development (Ball et al. Reference Ball, Boardman, Rueckert, Aljabar, Arichi, Merchant, Gousias, Edwards and Counsell2012), specific structural and functional brain alterations might underlie socio-emotional impairments. Volumetric alterations have been shown in VPT samples in brain regions thought to subserve emotion processing, such as the amygdala (Peterson et al. Reference Peterson, Vohr, Staib, Cannistraci, Dolberg, Schneider, Katz, Westerveld, Sparrow, Anderson, Duncan, Makuch, Gore and Ment2000), orbitofrontal cortex (Giménez et al. Reference Giménez, Junqué, Vendrell, Narberhaus, Bargalló, Botet and Mercader2006), fusiform gyrus (Nosarti et al. Reference Nosarti, Giouroukou, Healy, Rifkin, Walshe, Reichenberg, Chitnis, Williams and Murray2008; Gousias et al. Reference Gousias, Edwards, Rutherford, Counsell, Hajnal, Rueckert and Hammers2012), hippocampus (Rogers et al. Reference Rogers, Anderson, Thompson, Kidokoro, Wallendorf, Treyvaud, Roberts, Doyle, Neil and Inder2012; Omizzolo et al. Reference Omizzolo, Thompson, Scratch, Stargatt, Lee, Cheong, Roberts, Doyle and Anderson2013; Aanes et al. Reference Aanes, Bjuland, Skranes and Løhaugen2015) and insula (Nosarti et al. Reference Nosarti, Nam, Walshe, Murray, Cuddy, Rifkin and Allin2014). Moreover, both structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and diffusion MRI studies, have found significant associations between brain alterations in regions typically implicated in socio-emotion processing and specific social and emotional outcomes in VPT samples (Rogers et al. Reference Rogers, Anderson, Thompson, Kidokoro, Wallendorf, Treyvaud, Roberts, Doyle, Neil and Inder2012, Reference Rogers, Barch, Sylvester, Pagliaccio, Harms, Botteron and Luby2014; Healy et al. Reference Healy, Reichenberg, Nam, Allin, Walshe, Rifkin, Murray and Nosarti2013; Fischi-Gómez et al. Reference Fischi-Gómez, Vasung, Meskaldji, Lazeyras, Borradori-Tolsa, Hagmann, Barisnikov, Thiran and Hüppi2015). However, to our knowledge, no neuroimaging study to date has investigated resting-state functional connectivity (rs-fc) in emotion-processing networks and performance on emotional processing tasks in individuals born VPT.

In this study, we probed the integrity of functional networks that are fundamental to emotional information processing and are anchored in the amygdala (Leppänen & Nelson, Reference Leppänen and Nelson2009; Bickart et al. Reference Bickart, Dickerson and Barrett2014). Functional connectivity between three amygdalar subregions (centro-medial/latero-basal/superficial) and all other brain areas was investigated using rs-fc MRI. In addition, we conducted an exploratory analysis to investigate participants’ ability to recognize six basic emotions assessed with the Emotion Recognition Task (ERT; Montagne et al. Reference Montagne, Kessels, de Haan and Perrett2007), a computer-based test that uses morphed images of human faces at different degrees of emotional intensity. In view of previous observations that amygdalar connectivity is reduced in individuals with mood disorders, and that this dysconnectivity relates to clinical symptoms and emotion-processing performance (e.g. Peng et al. Reference Peng, Shi, Shen, Peng, Zhang, Liu, Qiu, Liu, Jiang, Fang and Shen2014), we hypothesized that VPT individuals would exhibit: (1) reduced rs-fc between the amygdala and key nodes of an emotion-processing network (Leppänen & Nelson, Reference Leppänen and Nelson2009); (2) lower accuracy and longer reaction times than controls at recognizing emotions at lower intensity levels; and (3) functional integrity of the amygdala connectivity network would be related to performance on the ERT. Post-hoc exploratory analyses investigating associations between rs-fc, emotion recognition, full-scale IQ and perinatal variables were additionally conducted.

Method

Participants

We studied 36 participants recruited from a cohort of 218 individuals who were born at <33 weeks of gestation at University College London Hospital between 1979 and 1984 and who were enrolled into a follow-up study (Nosarti et al. Reference Nosarti, Giouroukou, Healy, Rifkin, Walshe, Reichenberg, Chitnis, Williams and Murray2008, Reference Nosarti, Nam, Walshe, Murray, Cuddy, Rifkin and Allin2014). Exclusion criteria were any history of neurological conditions including meningitis, head injury and cerebral infections.

Thirty-eight age-matched controls were recruited from advertisements in the local press and universities. Inclusion criteria were full-term birth (38–42 weeks of gestation) and birth weight > 2500 g. Exclusion criteria were any history of birth complications (e.g. endotracheal mechanical ventilation), neurological conditions including meningitis, head injury and cerebral infections.

All participants gave informed written consent, were reimbursed for travel expenses and received a nominal remuneration for participation in the study. The study was given ethical approval by the Psychiatry, Nursing and Midwifery Research Ethics Subcommittee, King's College London.

Materials

IQ was assessed by the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1999), which contains four subtests (Vocabulary, Similarities, Block Design and Matrix Reasoning) and provides estimates of verbal, performance and full-scale IQ. Processing speed and sustained attention were assessed using respectively correct response reaction times and omission errors from the Conner's Continuous Performance Test II (CCPT-II; Conners, Reference Conners2000). Mental health at testing was evaluated with the General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-12; Goldberg & Williams, Reference Goldberg and Williams1988) and Peters’ Delusional Inventory (PDI-21; Peters et al. Reference Peters, Joseph and Garety1999), that assess respectively anxiety/depression symptoms and psychosis proneness.

The ERT was used to evaluate the ability to identify six universal emotions (anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness, surprise) by using dynamic stimuli of human faces at different intensities. The stimuli set was developed from colour pictures of four Caucasian actors (two males and two females) in frontal view, who were asked to look neutral and to show a full-blown emotional expression. For each identity, a computer program created intermediate morphed images starting with an inexpressive frame and ending with an emotional expression of different intensities, ranging from 20% to 100% (Montagne et al. Reference Montagne, Kessels, de Haan and Perrett2007). The ERT version used in this study included only four levels of intensity: 0–40%, 0–60%, 0–80% and 0–100%.

The presentation procedure started with an instruction screen and a short practice trial with stimuli at 0–100% intensity. The experimental task consisted of a total of 96 stimuli divided into four continuous blocks of increasing intensity. In each trial, one video-clip of a facial expression was played on the screen, the last frame of which represented a static image at the final emotional intensity. Then six labels, describing the six emotions, appeared on the screen and participants were requested to choose the one which was deemed to correspond to the preceding facial expression. No time restriction was imposed, but reaction times were recorded. Overall ERT administration duration was approximately 10 min.

Socio-demographic, perinatal, neuropsychological and ERT data analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS v. 22.0 (IBM SPSS Statistics, USA). Group comparisons in terms of age at assessment and neuropsychological test scores were performed using independent-sample t tests or their non-parametric equivalent. Sex distribution was tested with Pearson's χ2, whereas differences in socioeconomic status (SES) and ethnicity were explored with Fisher's exact test. Differences in neonatal characteristics and full-scale IQ between the VPT group and the larger sample of VPT participants studied at 19–20 years (Nosarti et al. Reference Nosarti, Nam, Walshe, Murray, Cuddy, Rifkin and Allin2014) were analysed with univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA).

ERT performance was analysed in terms of both accuracy, as the number of correctly labelled expressions per emotion per intensity (maximum = 4), and mean reaction time for each emotion-intensity level. Both measures were analysed using univariate analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) adjusting for processing speed as defined earlier (CCPT-II correct-response reaction times). First, modulation of accuracy and reaction times were investigated in separate three-way mixed ANCOVAs with emotion type (six levels: anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness, surprise) and emotion intensity (four levels: very low 0–40%, low 0–60%, high 0–80%, very high 0–100%) as within-subjects factors and group (two levels: preterm v. control) as between-subject factor. Subsequently, a two-way mixed ANCOVA was performed for each emotion separately, with emotion intensity as within-subject factor and group as between-subject factor. Greenhouse–Geisser correction was used for violations of sphericity. False discovery rate (FDR) correction was applied throughout the analyses to account for multiple comparisons.

MRI data acquisition

Neuroimaging data were acquired at the Maudsley Hospital (London) using a General Electric Signa HDx 3.0 T MR scanner (GE Healthcare, USA). Resting-state images were collected from a gradient-echo echo-planar sequence (TR/TE: 2000/30 ms, flip angle: 75°, matrix 64 × 64, FoV = 218 cm) resulting in 256 whole-brain volumes contained 37 non-contiguous slices with 2.4-mm thickness, 1-mm interslice gap and 3.4-mm voxel resolution. Before the resting-state session, participants were instructed to remain still with gaze fixed on a central cross.

For spatial normalization and localization, high-resolution T1-weighted anatomical images were also acquired using a SPGR pulse sequence with the following parameters: TR = 7.1 ms, TE = 2.8 ms, TI = 450, FOV = 280 cm, flip angle = 20°, matrix = 256 × 256, 196 slices, no interslice gap, slice thickness = 1.1 mm, isotropic resolution = 1.1 × 1.1 × 1.1 mm3.

MRI data pre-processing

Images were processed and analysed in SPM8 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm8/) running on Matlab 7.12 (MathWorks, USA). Data were slice-time corrected, spatially realigned, normalized to a sample-specific DARTEL template created on the basis of unified segmentation of individual anatomy (Ashburner, Reference Ashburner2007), and smoothed with a 6 mm full-width at half-maximum (FWHM) isotropic Gaussian kernel.

Head motion and artefact detection

As recent studies have demonstrated resting-state fMRI metrics to be particularly sensitive to confounding effects of head motion (Power et al. Reference Power, Barnes, Snyder, Schlaggar and Petersen2012; Van Dijk et al. Reference Van Dijk, Sabuncu and Buckner2012), the Artifact Detection Tools (ART, www.nitrc.org/projects/artifact_detect/) were used to identify problematic time points. In particular, an image was defined as an outlier (artefact) if a participant's head movement differed by in excess of 0.5 mm (translation) or 0.02 rad (rotation) from the previous frame, or if the global mean intensity in the image was >3 s.d. from the mean image intensity for the entire scan.

To preserve the temporal structure of the data, outlier volumes were not corrected or deleted from the time series, but rather modelled in the first level General Linear Model (GLM). A Mann–Whitney U test revealed no significant difference between VPT-born (median = 2.50) and controls (median = 2.00) in the number of outlier volumes (z = −0.607, p = 0.544).

Regions of interest (ROIs) definition

Rs-fc MRI data were analysed using a seed-based approach and taking the amygdalar subcompartments (supported by convergent structural and functional observations; Bzdok et al. Reference Bzdok, Laird, Zilles, Fox and Eickhoff2013) as ROIs. Three amygdalar seed regions were determined through stereotaxic probabilistic maps of cytoarchitectonic boundaries (Amunts et al. Reference Amunts, Kedo, Kindler, Pieperhoff, Mohlberg, Shah, Habel, Schneider and Zilles2005), implemented in the SPM Anatomy toolbox (Eickhoff et al. Reference Eickhoff, Stephan, Mohlberg, Grefkes, Fink, Amunts and Zilles2005). The centromedial group included the central and medial nuclei; the laterobasal subregion consisted of the lateral, basolateral, basomedial, and paralaminar nuclei; the superficial group included the anterior amygdaloid area, the amygdalopyriform transition area, the amygdaloid-hippocampal area and the ventral and posterior cortical nuclei.

Only voxels with at least a 50% probability of belonging to one of these subregions were included, and those exceeding 50% probability for multiple subregions were assigned only to the region for which they had the highest probability of inclusion. Images of each subregion were created in the standard space (MNI) separately for left and right hemisphere to create a total of six ROIs.

Rs-fc MRI seed-region analysis

The Functional Connectivity toolbox (CONN v14.b) (Whitfield-Gabrieli & Nieto-Castanon, Reference Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castañón2012; http://web.mit.edu/swg/software.htm) was used to perform all seed-based analyses. To remove physiological noise and movement confounds, the CONN toolbox uses the anatomical component correction (aCompCor) strategy (Behzadi et al. Reference Behzadi, Restom, Liau and Liu2007) that increases the sensitivity and specificity of positive correlations and can detect non-artifactual anti-correlations. Global signal regression was excluded to avoid the introduction of artificial negative correlation (Murphy et al. Reference Murphy, Birn, Handwerker, Jones and Bandettini2009). Subject-specific effects of motion (defined by six realignment parameters and their first-order temporal derivatives), outliers volumes (represented by a binary regressor), and white matter and CSF signals (characterized by the first five principal components of each) were removed from the functional data using linear regression and the resulting residual blood oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) time series were band-pass filtered between 0.009 and 0.08 Hz.

In the first-level analyses, the average BOLD time-course was extracted from each seed region separately and Pearson's correlation coefficients were computed between that time course and the time course of every other voxel in the brain. Seed-to-voxel connectivity maps were constructed for each subject. Correlation coefficients were then converted to normally distributed z scores using the Fisher transformation to allow the second-level GLM analyses. Within-group statistical parametric maps (SPMs) and connectivity differences between groups were further explored and examined with independent-sample t tests. Reported clusters survived a height threshold of uncorrected p < 0.001 and an extent threshold of FDR-corrected p < 0.05 at cluster level.

Associations between rs-fc MRI and ERT, socio-demographic, neuropsychological and perinatal data

To evaluate relationships between rs-fc MRI data and emotion-processing performance, post-hoc correlation analyses were conducted. In these analyses, correlations between connectivity of significant clusters obtained during second-level analyses and ERT emotion recognition accuracy were assessed with Spearman's rank test.

Multiple analyses of variance were used to test for the effects of sex and SES, as well as interactions with group, in terms of ERT metrics and rs-fc patterns found to be different between preterm and control participants.

Other associations between rs-fc, full-scale IQ and perinatal factors were assessed using Pearson's correlation coefficient for parametric continuous variables.

Results

Sample characteristics

Characteristics of study sample are reported in Table 1. Controls tended to belong to higher SES compared to VPT individuals and about one quarter (26%) of controls was recruited from the university campus (three post-doctoral researchers, three research workers/technicians, four postgraduate students). In terms of neuropsychological and mental health outcomes, compared to controls, VPT participants had significantly lower verbal, performance and full-scale IQ, despite their values falling within the test norm. No statistically significant between group differences were found in processing speed, sustained attention, or current mental health (GHQ-12 and PDI-21).

Table 1. Sample characteristics. Frequencies, percentages and mean values (standard deviations) are given, unless otherwise specified

VPT, Very preterm birth.

a Socioeconomic status classified according to the Standard Occupational Classification 1980 (SOC1980). The following categories were used: I−II = managerial and professional; III = intermediate (e.g. small employers and own account workers); IV−V = working (e.g. lower supervisory and technical occupations, routine and semi-routine occupations).

b Periventricular haemorrhage was classified as haemorrhage into the germinal layer or lateral ventricles and ventricular dilatation as clear dilatation of one or both lateral ventricles with cerebrospinal fluid, although not sufficient to meet the conditions for a diagnosis of hydrocephalus (Stewart et al. Reference Stewart, Thorburn, Hope, Goldsmith, Lipscomb and Reynolds1983).

c Median and 95% confidence intervals.

In order to assess selection bias in the VPT sample, we compared demographic and perinatal characteristics between the current VPT sample and a sample of 68 individuals who were assessed at age 19–20 (Nosarti et al. Reference Nosarti, Nam, Walshe, Murray, Cuddy, Rifkin and Allin2014). There were no statistically significant differences between the two VPT cohorts in terms of gestational age (F 1,102 = 0.24, p > 0.05) and birth weight (F 1,102 = 0.85, p > 0.05), although the current cohort had higher full-scale IQ than the VPT cohort assessed at age 19–20 (F 1,102 = 7.29, p < 0.01).

Emotion Recognition Task

Preterm participants’ and controls’ performance on the ERT in terms of accuracy and reaction time are displayed in Fig. 1 (a, b).

Fig. 1. Emotion Recognition Task (ERT) performance. (a) Mean scores and (b) mean reaction times of very preterm birth (VPT) participants and controls for the six ERT emotions. Error bars represent 1 standard deviation. (c) Mean scores and (d) mean reaction times of VPT participants (white dots) and controls (black triangles) at the four levels of intensity for anger. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.001.

Regarding accuracy, a three-way mixed ANCOVA demonstrated a trend towards a significant main effect for group (F 1,71 = 3.13, p = 0.081), with controls tending to perform better than VPT adults. Interactions between group and emotion type (F 5,355 = 1.11, p = 0.352) and emotion intensity (F 3,213 = 1.84, p = 0.149) were not significant. When analysing each emotion separately, a significant main effect for group (F 1,71 = 9.05, p = 0.024) and the interaction between group and emotion intensity (F 3,213 = 5.86, p = 0.006) were found only for anger. Further ANCOVAs showed that VPT adults were less able than controls to recognize anger at the lowest level of intensity; i.e. the most difficult condition (F 1,71 = 23.41, p < 0.0001; Fig. 1c ).

Considering reaction time, a three-way mixed ANCOVA demonstrated a significant main effect for group (F 1,71 = 8.48, p = 0.005), with controls being faster than VPT participants. There was also a significant interaction between group and emotion intensity (F 3,213 = 7.01, p = 0.005), showing that controls were faster than VPT participants at each level of intensity, with reaction times progressively decreasing as the emotion became more obvious (e.g. at higher levels of intensity). When analysing each emotion separately, as it was done for accuracy, there was a significant interaction between emotion intensity and group only for anger, after FDR-correction (F 3,213 = 5.55, p = 0.042). Further ANCOVAs showed that VPT adults were slower than controls at recognizing anger at the lowest level of intensity (F 1,71 = 7.15, p = 0.036; Fig. 1d ).

Rs-fc MRI results

Within group rs-fc MRI results

Within-group connectivity maps and significant areas of activation across the six amygdalar subregions (three in each hemisphere) are presented in Supplementary Fig. S1. In brief, spontaneous activity in the amygdala subregions was positively associated with neuronal activity in surrounding structures and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, whereas it was negatively associated with neuronal activity in dorsal prefrontal cortex, parietal lobe and posterior brain regions, with strong inter-hemispheric similarities.

Between-group rs-fc MRI results

Significant between-group differences in patterns of rs-fc were found only for connectivity of the left superficial amygdalar subregion. This area showed hypoconnectivity with the right posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) and the left precuneus (pC) and hyperconnectivity with the superior temporal sulcus (STS) in VPT adults compared to controls (Table 2, Fig. 2).

Fig. 2. Alterations in functional connectivity of the left superficial amygdala in very preterm birth (VPT)  individuals compared to controls. (a) Location of 50% probabilistic masks of the superficial amygdala of the left hemisphere projected on the MNI template with neurological convention. (b) Brain areas that showed significantly greater functional connectivity of the left superficial amygdala in VPT individuals (hot colour scale) comprised the right superior temporal sulcus (STS); brain areas that showed significantly smaller functional connectivity of the left superficial amygdala in VPT individuals (cool colour scale) comprised the right posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) and the left precuneus (pC). Brain slices are displayed axially according to neurological convention, with z coordinates in MNI space above each slice. The figure on the right depicts these slices in blue on a midline, sagittal slice. Reported clusters survived a height threshold of uncorrected p < 0.001 and an extent threshold of FDR-corrected p < 0.05 at the cluster level. (c) Bar graph showing direct comparison of functional connectivity strength between the two groups. The y-axis indicates correlation coefficients between the time series of the seed region (superficial subregion of the left amygdala) and the time series extracted from the regions displaying significant between-group differences; the error bars represent 1 standard error.

Table 2. Differences in functional connectivity of the left superficial amygdala between VPT individuals and controls

VPT, Very preterm birth; R, right hemisphere; L, left hemisphere.

Reported clusters survived a height threshold of uncorrected p < 0.001 and an extent threshold of FDR-corrected p < 0.05 at the cluster level.

Investigating associations between rs-fc MRI and facial emotion processing, sex, SES, IQ and perinatal risk factors

In controls, anger scores were significantly associated with rs-fc between the left superficial amygdala and the PCC (Spearman's ρ = 0.426, p = 0.008), whereas in the VPT group this association was not significant (Spearman's ρ = 0.172, p = 0.316; Fig. 3). However, the correlation was not significantly different between the groups (z = 1.16, p = 0.246).

Fig. 3. Correlation between superficial amygdala-posterior cingulate connectivity and Emotion Recognition Task score for anger in very preterm birth (VPT) participants (white dots; Spearman's ρ = 0.172) and controls (black triangles; Spearman's ρ = 0.426). Least-squared lines are shown for illustrative purposes only and do not constitute a formal test.

Post-hoc analyses to assess the effect of sex and SES demonstrated no statistically significant differences between males and females, nor between SES categories on rs-fc patterns and ERT emotions where differences between preterm participants and controls were detected (all p > 0.05).

Analysis of the association between rs-fc and full-scale IQ in VPT participants and controls did not show any statistically significant result (see Supplementary Table S1).

Within the VPT participants, perinatal risk factors (e.g. gestational age, birth weight and neonatal ultrasound classification) did not significantly correlate with either ERT performance or rs-fc patterns.

Discussion

Several investigations have suggested an association between atypical social development and VPT. This study provides two important contributions that help to elucidate the mechanisms underlying a possible link between the two. First, our results suggest that in adulthood VPT individuals display functional alterations in brain circuits fundamental to emotion processing; second, they demonstrate that VPT adults are worse than controls at recognizing subtle specific emotions from facial expressions.

Recognizing facial expressions denoting specific emotions is a crucial skill for successful interpersonal interactions and is compromised in many psychiatric disorders with onset in childhood/adolescence (Law Smith et al. Reference Law Smith, Montagne, Perrett, Gill and Gallagher2010; Jarros et al. Reference Jarros, Salum, da Silva, Toazza, de Abreu Costa, de Salles and Manfro2012; Collin et al. Reference Collin, Bindra, Raju, Gillberg and Minnis2013) and adulthood (Scholten et al. Reference Scholten, Aleman, Montagne and Kahn2005; Montagne et al. Reference Montagne, Schutters, Westenberg, van Honk, Kessels and de Haan2006, Reference Montagne, de Geus, Kessels, Denys, de Haan and Westenberg2008; Poljac et al. Reference Poljac, Montagne and de Haan2011). In this study, findings from the overall ERT demonstrated that, compared to controls, VPT-born individuals had almost intact ability, but longer reaction times, at recognizing emotion expressions. This could be interpreted as a compensatory mechanism in VPT adults, who need more time to adequately process emotional stimuli. A more detailed analysis of emotion type found specific and subtle impairments in VPT adults in recognizing anger at very low intensity, with their performance being both slower and less accurate than controls. Importantly, in the current study these results are unlikely to depend on VPT participants’ deficits in attention/processing speed or psychological distress, as their scores on such measures were comparable to controls’. These findings suggest that deficits in emotion recognition – previously reported generically at the age of ⩽5 years (Potharst et al. Reference Potharst, van Wassenaer-Leemhuis, Houtzager, Livesey, Kok, Last and Oosterlaan2013; Witt et al. Reference Witt, Theurel, Tolsa, Lejeune, Fernandes, de Jonge, Monnier, Graz, Barisnikov, Gentaz and Hüppi2014) and specifically for anger and low-intensity stimuli at 8 years (Wocadlo & Rieger, Reference Wocadlo and Rieger2006) – persist into adulthood in VPT-born individuals. In the literature, a selective impairment in recognizing angry expressions has been associated with lower social competence in children across different ages (Maxim & Nowicki, Reference Maxim and Nowicki2003). Diminished social competence could further result in social isolation and rejection by peers, as reported in VPT adolescent samples (Rickards et al. Reference Rickards, Kelly, Doyle and Callanan2001; Dahl et al. Reference Dahl, Kaaresen, Tunby, Handegård, Kvernmo and Rønning2006). Additionally, a reduced ability to recognize a potential threat conveyed by angry faces may make children more vulnerable to being bullied by peers (Woods et al. Reference Woods, Wolke, Nowicki and Hall2009), placing VPT samples at risk of experiencing bullying and related emotional problems (Wolke et al. Reference Wolke, Baumann, Strauss, Johnson and Marlow2015).

Even though these results suggest a selective difficulty for anger, the null findings in other emotions are likely to be due to a small sample size and the fact that the current VPT participants were cognitively unimpaired individuals (i.e. had IQ scores within the test norm). The possibility that VPT participants would also show subtle deficits for other emotions cannot be excluded by the current results and warrants testing in a larger sample with more variable psychosocial functioning. Moreover, a precise measure of participants’ mood at testing would potentially mitigate potential bias effects on emotion processing.

To investigate the neural underpinnings of the observed behavioural deficits, whole-brain rs-fc analyses were conducted using seeds in three cytoarchitectonic subdivisions of the amygdala (and thus permitting dissociation of regionally specific connectivity patterns). Indeed, the superficial subregion of the left amygdala was unique in showing evidence of altered rs-fc in VPT participants compared to controls. While control subjects exhibited significant positive connectivity between this region and pC as well as PCC, VPT-born individuals displayed significant negative functional connectivity between the same regions. VPT-born participants also showed increased connectivity between left superficial amygdala and the STS. Functional aberrations specific to this amygdalar compartment are not unexpected in light of recent meta-analytical findings (Bzdok et al. Reference Bzdok, Laird, Zilles, Fox and Eickhoff2013) which reported that the superficial amygdala is highly tuned for processing both static and dynamic social stimuli in the form of emotional expression of human faces (Hurlemann et al. Reference Hurlemann, Rehme, Diessel, Kukolja, Maier, Walter and Cohen2008; Goossens et al. Reference Goossens, Kukolja, Onur, Fink, Maier, Griez, Schruers and Hurlemann2009), and structural alterations of this subregion have been described in anxiety disorder (Qin et al. Reference Qin, Young, Duan, Chen, Supekar and Menon2014).

Consistent with previous findings from healthy subjects (Roy et al. Reference Roy, Shehzad, Margulies, Kelly, Uddin, Gotimer, Biswal, Castellanos and Milham2009), both groups in the current study displayed negative correlations between activity in amygdala and dorsolateral prefrontal and posterior parietal regions typically involved in cognitive control of emotions. However, VPT-born individuals displayed a wider suppression of posterior areas, which extended to a larger proportion of the PCC and the bilateral pC. Aberrant amygdala–PCC/pC coupling has been previously reported in psychiatric samples with reduced connectivity associated with state anxiety scores (Hahn et al. Reference Hahn, Stein, Windischberger, Weissenbacher, Spindelegger, Moser, Kasper and Lanzenberger2011) and elevated connectivity found in individuals diagnosed with major depressive disorder (Cullen et al. Reference Cullen, Westlund, Klimes-Dougan, Mueller, Houri, Eberly and Lim2014). The PCC and pC are involved in emotional evaluation (Wright et al. Reference Wright, Albarracin, Brown, Li, He and Liu2008) and modulation (Adolphs, Reference Adolphs2003), and also constitute core regions of the ‘default mode network’ (DMN), a system that is more active at rest than during goal-oriented tasks (Raichle et al. Reference Raichle, MacLeod, Snyder, Powers, Gusnard and Shulman2001; Greicius et al. Reference Greicius, Krasnow, Reiss and Menon2003). Increasing evidence demonstrates a substantial overlap between the DMN and the ‘social brain’ (Mars et al. Reference Mars, Neubert, Noonan, Sallet, Toni and Rushworth2012) and a relationship of reciprocal inhibition has been proposed between the cognitive and emotional systems (Drevets & Raichle, Reference Drevets and Raichle1998). We previously showed alterations in rs-fc between posterior DMN and salience network in VPT adults from the same cohort compared to controls (White et al. Reference White, Symington, Castellanos, Brittain, Walsh, Nam, Sato, Allin, Shergill, Murray, Williams and Nosarti2014). In this framework, hypoconnectivity between the amygdala and the PCC/pC in VPT-born individuals could hint at an altered coupling of these two regions. It is feasible that amygdalar activation in socio-emotional relevant conditions might excessively suppress the activity of DMN brain regions responsible for cognitive evaluation of a given situation. This interpretation is in line with the study by Sreenivas et al. (Reference Sreenivas, Boehm and Linden2012), which reported a deactivation of DMN regions during the processing of emotional faces and, in particular, a greater deactivation for negative compared with positive emotions, as a demonstration of their higher evolutionary saliency. An increased negative coupling between the amygdala and the PCC/pC could represent an overactive inhibitory connection that could become disruptive and maladaptive, not only in ‘spectatorial’ and experimental tasks, but also in emotionally-engaged and interactive settings in real life (Schilbach et al. Reference Schilbach, Timmermans, Reddy, Costall, Bente, Schlicht and Vogeley2013), as there exists a large topological overlap of brain regions involving such processes.

The current study suggests that the pathway between PCC and amygdala importantly contributes to emotion recognition. Connectivity between PCC and the left superficial amygdala was significantly associated with the ability to recognize angry faces in controls but not in VPT individuals, and suggests this as a potential substrate for selective ERT impairments in the latter. As the dorsal PCC plays a key modulatory role in the amygdala network for emotion processing (Pessoa et al. Reference Pessoa, Padmala and Morland2005; Stein et al. Reference Stein, Wiedholz, Bassett, Weinberger, Zink, Mattay and Meyer-Lindenberg2007), it can be reasonably hypothesized that the inability to recognize low intensity levels of anger may result from inadequate amygdalar modulation in VPT-born individuals. This has potential implications for social cognition, especially in situations where it is necessary to attribute mental states to others (Saxe & Powell, Reference Saxe and Powell2006). The PCC is a highly connected structure (Hagmann et al. Reference Hagmann, Cammoun, Gigandet, Meuli, Honey, Wedeen and Sporns2008) responsible for self-referential thought (Johnson et al. Reference Johnson, Baxter, Wilder, Pipe, Heiserman and Prigatano2002; Mason et al. Reference Mason, Norton, Van Horn, Wegner, Grafton and Macrae2007; Buckner et al. Reference Buckner, Andrews-Hanna and Schacter2008; Whitfield-Gabrieli et al. Reference Whitfield-Gabrieli, Moran, Nieto-Castañón, Triantafyllou, Saxe and Gabrieli2011), but is also involved in cognitive processes (Pearson et al. Reference Pearson, Heilbronner, Barack, Hayden and Platt2011). It acts as a connector hub in cortico-subcortical networks (Leech et al. Reference Leech, Braga and Sharp2012); and has been found to influence activity in distributed networks responsible for performing goal-directed actions, such as the dorsal attention network, fronto-parietal control network and salience network (Leech & Sharp, Reference Leech and Sharp2014). In particular, the dorsal PCC is involved in allocating attentional focus and balancing between internally- and externally-oriented cognition (Leech et al. Reference Leech, Kamourieh, Beckmann and Sharp2011, Reference Leech, Braga and Sharp2012). Thus, socio-emotional deficits in VPT-born individuals may result from difficulties in coordinating activity between systems responsible for processing emotion and those responsible for attention modulation. Impairments in executive function are widely recognized in VPT-born individuals across the life span (Aarnoudse-Moens et al. Reference Aarnoudse-Moens, Smidts, Oosterlaan, Duivenvoorden and Weisglas-Kuperus2009; Burnett et al. Reference Burnett, Scratch and Anderson2013; Sølsnes et al. Reference Sølsnes, Skranes, Brubakk and Løhaugen2014) and these have been hypothesized as a potential mechanism to understand socio-emotional difficulties in VPT children (Hille et al. Reference Hille, den Ouden, Saigal, Wolke, Lambert, Whitaker, Pinto-Martin, Hoult, Meyer, Feldman, Verloove-Vanhorick and Paneth2001). In other words, difficulties with adaptability, impulsivity, and attention could affect social interactions. Although our current analyses showed significant between group differences in ERT performance after accounting for sustained attention, it remains to be investigated whether impaired modulation of different aspects of attention (e.g. attentional control) which were not measured in the current study affect emotion recognition in VPT samples (Eack et al. Reference Eack, Wojtalik, Barb, Newhill, Keshavan and Phillips2016).

This study additionally presents evidence of connective pathways supporting emotional processing that are specific to individuals born VPT. Most notably, increased connectivity was found between the left superficial amygdala and the STS in this group but not in controls. Structural alterations in the STS have been previously reported in VPT samples (Nosarti et al. Reference Nosarti, Giouroukou, Healy, Rifkin, Walshe, Reichenberg, Chitnis, Williams and Murray2008; Rogers et al. Reference Rogers, Barch, Sylvester, Pagliaccio, Harms, Botteron and Luby2014). This region – and in particular its posterior portion (Hein & Knight, Reference Hein and Knight2008)- is well known as a face-sensitive region specialized for dynamic feature recognition (Haxby et al. Reference Haxby, Hoffman and Gobbini2000; Andrews & Ewbank, Reference Andrews and Ewbank2004; Engell & Haxby, Reference Engell and Haxby2007), including eye and mouth movements (Puce et al. Reference Puce, Allison, Bentin, Gore and McCarthy1998; Hoffman & Haxby, Reference Hoffman and Haxby2000) and emotional expressions (LaBar et al. Reference LaBar, Crupain, Voyvodic and McCarthy2003). Together with the amygdala and the orbitofrontal cortex, the STS has been proposed as a key region in social cognition (Brothers, Reference Brothers1990; Allison et al. Reference Allison, Puce and McCarthy2000). Decreased connectivity between the amygdala and the STS has been hypothesized as the underlying mechanism of emotion and face processing disturbance in various psychiatric disorders, including autism spectrum disorder (Monk et al. Reference Monk, Weng, Wiggins, Kurapati, Louro, Carrasco, Maslowsky, Risi and Lord2010) and major depressive disorder (Ramasubbu et al. Reference Ramasubbu, Konduru, Cortese, Bray, Gaxiola-Valdez and Goodyear2014). By contrast, the increased amygdala–STS connectivity found in our VPT-born sample could represent a compensatory strategy in a suboptimally efficient emotional network, facilitating emotion recognition by means of stronger integration between decoding of facial characteristics by the temporal areas and emotional salience by the amygdala. This interpretation could also explain the overall slower and less accurate performance of VPT adults on the ERT. Moreover, as the STS is crucial for theory of mind (Gallagher & Frith, Reference Gallagher and Frith2003; Vander Wyk et al. Reference Vander Wyk, Hudac, Carter, Sobel and Pelphrey2009), hyperconnectivity of this region with the amygdala could also support higher cognitive processing of emotional information.

Strengths, limitations and future directions

There are several limitations in this study. First, the amygdala, which was chosen as a seed region for rs-fc analyses, is susceptible to EPI image distortion and signal dropout, which may cause relevant problems of spatial localization (Merboldt et al. Reference Merboldt, Fransson, Bruhn and Frahm2001). To deal with this potential bias, only the core of each amygdalar subregion was considered by using a probabilistic map at 50%, as in Roy et al. Reference Roy, Shehzad, Margulies, Kelly, Uddin, Gotimer, Biswal, Castellanos and Milham2009, but a stricter threshold could have been used (e.g. 90%, as in Ball et al. Reference Ball, Rahm, Eickhoff, Schulze-Bonhage, Speck and Mutschler2007). Second, a score range between 0 and 4 at each ERT level of emotion did not allow us to explore subtler impairments in emotion recognition in relation to altered patterns of amygdalar connectivity. An increased number of trials or the use of more numerous levels of emotional intensity to further aggregate could have refined our analyses. Finally, the fact that controls tended to belong to higher SES bands compared to VPT individuals could have influenced our findings, although the current sample included high performing VPT adults (reflected by their higher IQ scores compared to a larger VPT sample assessed at age 18–19 years), who nevertheless had significantly lower IQ than controls. Replication of the study with a larger sample size might address these issues and provide a means of uncovering additional subtle features on account of increase statistical power.

Strengths of our study include: (1) a choice of amygdalar subdivisions used to define seed regions for rs-fc analysis, that permitted the investigation of between-group differences that could not have been detected considering the whole amygdala; (2) the use of DARTEL toolbox, that achieved a more accurate registration and normalization to account for structural differences expected between VPT-born individuals and born-at-term controls; and (3) the use of the ERT, which contains dynamic facial information, rather than fully blown and/or static information, which has proved advantageous in terms of identification of emotional expressions, evaluations of intensity and arousal, and discrimination of authenticity (Krumhuber et al. Reference Krumhuber, Kappas and Manstead2013), making it a useful tool to detect subtle and selective impairment that previous research failed to find (e.g. Ogai et al. Reference Ogai, Matsumoto, Suzuki, Ozawa, Fukuda, Uchiyama, Suckling, Isoda, Mori and Takei2003).

Future studies in VPT samples could investigate other critical skills that rely on emotion perception and contribute to the understanding of the complexity of the social world, such as attributing mental states, deciphering emotional meanings, using emotions in thought, and managing emotions (Mayer et al. Reference Mayer, Salovey, Caruso and Sitarenios2001). These components could differentially contribute to behavioural and psychiatric problems and they are still unexplored in VPT populations. Potential interventions that aim at improving socio-emotional skills could then strengthen different components of emotional intelligence (e.g. Bölte et al. Reference Bölte, Ciaramidaro, Schlitt, Hainz, Kliemann, Beyer, Poustka, Freitag and Walter2015).

Conclusion

VPT individuals continue to display subtle emotion recognition deficits in adulthood, which are associated with functional alterations in brain circuits fundamental to emotion processing, characterized by suboptimal amygdala modulation. These findings provide a potential foundation for future studies investigating how emotion-processing impairments may interact with biological, environmental and genetic risk and contribute to the increased vulnerability to psychiatric disorder in VPT individuals.

Supplementary material

For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291716001604.

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by the Medical Research Council (MRC MR/K004867/1). We would like to thank our participants and the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King's College London for their continuous support.

Declaration of Interest

None.

References

Aanes, S, Bjuland, KJ, Skranes, J, Løhaugen, GCC (2015). Memory function and hippocampal volumes in preterm born very-low-birth-weight (VLBW) young adults. NeuroImage 105, 7683.Google Scholar
Aarnoudse-Moens, CSH, Smidts, DP, Oosterlaan, J, Duivenvoorden, HJ, Weisglas-Kuperus, N (2009). Executive function in very preterm children at early school age. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 37, 981993.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adolphs, R (2003). Is the human amygdala specialized for processing social information? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 985, 326340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allin, M, Rooney, M, Cuddy, M, Wyatt, J, Walshe, M, Rifkin, L, Murray, R (2006). Personality in young adults who are born preterm. Pediatrics 117, 309316.Google Scholar
Allison, T, Puce, A, McCarthy, G (2000). Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4, 267278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amunts, K, Kedo, O, Kindler, M, Pieperhoff, P, Mohlberg, H, Shah, NJ, Habel, U, Schneider, F, Zilles, K (2005). Cytoarchitectonic mapping of the human amygdala, hippocampal region and entorhinal cortex: intersubject variability and probability maps. Anatomy and Embryology 210, 343352.Google Scholar
Andrews, TJ, Ewbank, MP (2004). Distinct representations for facial identity and changeable aspects of faces in the human temporal lobe. NeuroImage 23, 905913.Google Scholar
Ashburner, J (2007). A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. NeuroImage 38, 95113.Google Scholar
Ball, G, Boardman, JP, Rueckert, D, Aljabar, P, Arichi, T, Merchant, N, Gousias, IS, Edwards, AD, Counsell, SJ (2012). The effect of preterm birth on thalamic and cortical development. Cerebral Cortex 22, 10161024.Google Scholar
Ball, T, Rahm, B, Eickhoff, SB, Schulze-Bonhage, A, Speck, O, Mutschler, I (2007). Response properties of human amygdala subregions: evidence based on functional MRI combined with probabilistic anatomical maps. PLoS ONE 2, e307.Google Scholar
Behzadi, Y, Restom, K, Liau, J, Liu, TT (2007). A component based noise correction method (CompCor) for BOLD and perfusion based fMRI. NeuroImage 37, 90101.Google Scholar
Bickart, KC, Dickerson, BC, Barrett, LF (2014). The amygdala as a hub in brain networks that support social life. Neuropsychologia 63, 235248.Google Scholar
Bölte, S, Ciaramidaro, A, Schlitt, S, Hainz, D, Kliemann, D, Beyer, A, Poustka, F, Freitag, C, Walter, H (2015). Training-induced plasticity of the social brain in autism spectrum disorder. The British Journal of Psychiatry 207, 149157.Google Scholar
Bos, AF, Roze, E (2011). Neurodevelopmental outcome in preterm infants. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 53, 3539.Google Scholar
Boyd, LAC, Msall, ME, O'Shea, TM, Allred, EN, Hounshell, G, Leviton, A (2013). Social–emotional delays at 2 years in extremely low gestational age survivors: correlates of impaired orientation/engagement and emotional regulation. Early Human Development 89, 925930.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brothers, L (1990). The social brain: a project for integrating primate behavior and neurophysiology in a new domain. Concepts in Neuroscience 1, 2751.Google Scholar
Buckner, RL, Andrews-Hanna, JR, Schacter, DL (2008). The brain's default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1124, 138.Google Scholar
Burnett, AC, Anderson, PJ, Cheong, J, Doyle, LW, Davey, CG, Wood, SJ (2011). Prevalence of psychiatric diagnoses in preterm and full-term children, adolescents and young adults: a meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine 41, 24632474.Google Scholar
Burnett, AC, Scratch, SE, Anderson, PJ (2013). Executive function outcome in preterm adolescents. Early Human Development 89, 215220.Google Scholar
Bzdok, D, Laird, AR, Zilles, K, Fox, PT, Eickhoff, SB (2013). An investigation of the structural, connectional, and functional subspecialization in the human amygdala. Human Brain Mapping 34, 32473266.Google Scholar
Collin, L, Bindra, J, Raju, M, Gillberg, C, Minnis, H (2013). Facial emotion recognition in child psychiatry: a systematic review. Research in Developmental Disabilities 34, 15051520.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conners, C (2000). Conners’ Continuous Performance Test II Manual. Multi-Health Systems Inc.: Toronto.Google Scholar
Cullen, KR, Westlund, M, Klimes-Dougan, B, Mueller, BA, Houri, A, Eberly, LE, Lim, KO (2014). Abnormal amygdala resting-state functional connectivity in adolescent depression. JAMA Psychiatry 71, 11381147.Google ScholarPubMed
Dahl, LB, Kaaresen, PI, Tunby, J, Handegård, BH, Kvernmo, S, Rønning, JA (2006). Emotional, behavioral, social, and academic outcomes in adolescents born with very low birth weight. Pediatrics 118, e449e459.Google Scholar
D'Onofrio, BM, Class, QA, Rickert, ME, Larsson, H, Långström, N, Lichtenstein, P (2013). Preterm birth and mortality and morbidity: a population-based quasi-experimental study. JAMA Psychiatry 70, 12311240.Google Scholar
Drevets, WC, Raichle, ME (1998). Suppression of regional cerebral blood during emotional versus higher cognitive: implications for interactions between emotion and cognition. Cognition and Emotion 12, 353385.Google Scholar
Eack, SM, Wojtalik, JA, Barb, SM, Newhill, CE, Keshavan, MS, Phillips, ML (2016). Fronto-limbic brain dysfunction during the regulation of emotion in schizophrenia. PLoS ONE 11, e0149297.Google Scholar
Eickhoff, SB, Stephan, KE, Mohlberg, H, Grefkes, C, Fink, GR, Amunts, K, Zilles, K (2005). A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data. NeuroImage 25, 13251335.Google Scholar
Engell, AD, Haxby, JV (2007). Facial expression and gaze-direction in human superior temporal sulcus. Neuropsychologia 45, 32343241.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eryigit-Madzwamuse, S, Strauss, V, Baumann, N, Bartmann, P, Wolke, D (2015). Personality of adults who were born very preterm. Archives of Disease in Childhood-Fetal and Neonatal Edition 100, F524F529.Google Scholar
Fischi-Gómez, E, Vasung, L, Meskaldji, DE, Lazeyras, F, Borradori-Tolsa, C, Hagmann, P, Barisnikov, K, Thiran, JP, Hüppi, PS (2015). Structural brain connectivity in school-age preterm infants provides evidence for impaired networks relevant for higher order cognitive skills and social cognition. Cerebral Cortex 25, 27932805.Google Scholar
Gallagher, HL, Frith, CD (2003). Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7, 7783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giménez, M, Junqué, C, Vendrell, P, Narberhaus, A, Bargalló, N, Botet, F, Mercader, JM (2006). Abnormal orbitofrontal development due to prematurity. Neurology 67, 18181822.Google Scholar
Goldberg, DP, Williams, P (1988). The User's Guide to the General Health Questionnaire. NFER-Nelson: Windsor.Google Scholar
Goossens, L, Kukolja, J, Onur, OA, Fink, GR, Maier, W, Griez, E, Schruers, K, Hurlemann, R (2009). Selective processing of social stimuli in the superficial amygdala. Human Brain Mapping 30, 33323338.Google Scholar
Gousias, IS, Edwards, AD, Rutherford, MA, Counsell, SJ, Hajnal, JV, Rueckert, D, Hammers, A (2012). Magnetic resonance imaging of the newborn brain: manual segmentation of labelled atlases in term-born and preterm infants. NeuroImage 62, 14991509.Google Scholar
Greicius, MD, Krasnow, B, Reiss, AL, Menon, V (2003). Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 100, 253258.Google Scholar
Hagmann, P, Cammoun, L, Gigandet, X, Meuli, R, Honey, CJ, Wedeen, VJ, Sporns, O (2008). Mapping the structural core of human cerebral cortex. PLoS Biology 6, e159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hahn, A, Stein, P, Windischberger, C, Weissenbacher, A, Spindelegger, C, Moser, E, Kasper, S, Lanzenberger, R (2011). Reduced resting-state functional connectivity between amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex in social anxiety disorder. NeuroImage 56, 881889.Google Scholar
Harmon, HM, Taylor, HG, Minich, N, Wilson-Costello, D, Hack, M (2015). Early school outcomes for extremely preterm infants with transient neurological abnormalities. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 57, 865871.Google Scholar
Haxby, JV, Hoffman, EA, Gobbini, MI (2000). The distributed human neural system for face perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4, 223233.Google Scholar
Healy, E, Reichenberg, A, Nam, KW, Allin, MPG, Walshe, M, Rifkin, L, Murray, RM, Nosarti, C (2013). Preterm birth and adolescent social functioning – alterations in emotion-processing brain areas. Journal of Pediatrics 163, 15961604.Google Scholar
Hein, G, Knight, RT (2008). Superior temporal sulcus – it's my area: or is it? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 20, 21252136.Google Scholar
Hille, ETM, den Ouden, AL, Saigal, S, Wolke, D, Lambert, M, Whitaker, A, Pinto-Martin, JA, Hoult, L, Meyer, R, Feldman, JF, Verloove-Vanhorick, SP, Paneth, N (2001). Behavioural problems in children who weigh 1000 g or less at birth in four countries. Lancet 357, 16411643.Google Scholar
Hoffman, EA, Haxby, JV (2000). Distinct representations of eye gaze and identity in the distributed human neural system for face perception. Nature Neuroscience 3, 8084.Google Scholar
Hurlemann, R, Rehme, AK, Diessel, M, Kukolja, J, Maier, W, Walter, H, Cohen, MX (2008). Segregating intra-amygdalar responses to dynamic facial emotion with cytoarchitectonic maximum probability maps. Journal of Neuroscience Methods 172, 1320.Google Scholar
Jarros, RB, Salum, GA, da Silva, CTB, Toazza, R, de Abreu Costa, M, de Salles, JF, Manfro, GG (2012). Anxiety disorders in adolescence are associated with impaired facial expression recognition to negative valence. Journal of Psychiatric Research 46, 147151.Google Scholar
Johnson, S, Marlow, N (2011). Preterm birth and childhood psychiatric disorders. Pediatric Research 69, 11R18R.Google Scholar
Johnson, SC, Baxter, LC, Wilder, LS, Pipe, JG, Heiserman, JE, Prigatano, GP (2002). Neural correlates of self-reflection. Brain 125, 18081814.Google Scholar
Krumhuber, EG, Kappas, A, Manstead, AS (2013). Effects of dynamic aspects of facial expressions: a review. Emotion Review 5, 4146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LaBar, KS, Crupain, MJ, Voyvodic, JT, McCarthy, G (2003). Dynamic perception of facial affect and identity in the human brain. Cerebral Cortex 13, 10231033.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Law Smith, MJ, Montagne, B, Perrett, DI, Gill, M, Gallagher, L (2010). Detecting subtle facial emotion recognition deficits in high-functioning autism using dynamic stimuli of varying intensities. Neuropsychologia 48, 27772781.Google Scholar
Leech, R, Braga, R, Sharp, DJ (2012). Echoes of the brain within the posterior cingulate cortex. Journal of Neuroscience 32, 215222.Google Scholar
Leech, R, Kamourieh, S, Beckmann, CF, Sharp, DJ (2011). Fractionating the default mode network: distinct contributions of the ventral and dorsal posterior cingulate cortex to cognitive control. Journal of Neuroscience 31, 32173224.Google Scholar
Leech, R, Sharp, DJ (2014). The role of the posterior cingulate cortex in cognition and disease. Brain 137, 1232.Google Scholar
Leppänen, JM, Nelson, CA (2009). Tuning the developing brain to social signals of emotions. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 10, 3747.Google Scholar
Limperopoulos, C, Bassan, H, Sullivan, NR, Soul, JS, Robertson, RL, Moore, M, Ringer, SA, Volpe, JJ, du Plessis, AJ (2008). Positive screening for autism in ex-preterm infants: prevalence and risk factors. Pediatrics 121, 758765.Google Scholar
Lindström, K, Lindblad, F, Hjern, A (2011). Preterm birth and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in schoolchildren. Pediatrics 127, 858865.Google Scholar
Mars, RB, Neubert, FX, Noonan, MP, Sallet, J, Toni, I, Rushworth, MFS (2012). On the relationship between the ‘default mode network’ and the ‘social brain’. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6, 189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mason, MF, Norton, MI, Van Horn, JD, Wegner, DM, Grafton, ST, Macrae, CN (2007). Wandering minds: the default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science 315, 393395.Google Scholar
Maxim, LA, Nowicki, SJ (2003). Developmental associations between nonverbal ability and social competence. Facta Universitatis - Series Philosophy, Sociology, Psychology and History 2, 745758.Google Scholar
Mayer, JD, Salovey, P, Caruso, DR, Sitarenios, G (2001). Emotional intelligence as a standard intelligence. Emotions 1, 232242.Google Scholar
Merboldt, KD, Fransson, P, Bruhn, H, Frahm, J (2001). Functional MRI of the human amygdala? NeuroImage 14, 253257.Google Scholar
Monk, CS, Weng, SJ, Wiggins, JL, Kurapati, N, Louro, HMC, Carrasco, M, Maslowsky, J, Risi, S, Lord, C (2010). Neural circuitry of emotional face processing in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience 35, 105114.Google Scholar
Montagne, B, de Geus, F, Kessels, RPC, Denys, D, de Haan, EHF, Westenberg, HGM (2008). Perception of facial expressions in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a dimensional approach. European Psychiatry 23, 2628.Google Scholar
Montagne, B, Kessels, RPC, de Haan, EHF, Perrett, DI (2007). The Emotion Recognition Task: a paradigm to measure the perception of facial emotional expressions at different intensities. Perceptual and Motor Skills 104, 589598.Google Scholar
Montagne, B, Schutters, S, Westenberg, HGM, van Honk, J, Kessels, RPC, de Haan, EHF (2006). Reduced sensitivity in the recognition of anger and disgust in social anxiety disorder. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 11, 389401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, K, Birn, RM, Handwerker, DA, Jones, TB, Bandettini, PA (2009). The impact of global signal regression on resting state correlations: are anti-correlated networks introduced? NeuroImage 44, 893905.Google Scholar
Nosarti, C, Giouroukou, E, Healy, E, Rifkin, L, Walshe, M, Reichenberg, A, Chitnis, X, Williams, SCR, Murray, RM (2008). Grey and white matter distribution in very preterm adolescents mediates neurodevelopmental outcome. Brain 131, 205217.Google Scholar
Nosarti, C, Nam, KW, Walshe, M, Murray, RM, Cuddy, M, Rifkin, L, Allin, MPG (2014). Preterm birth and structural brain alterations in early adulthood. NeuroImage: Clinical 6, 180191.Google Scholar
Nosarti, C, Reichenberg, A, Murray, RM, Cnattingius, S, Lambe, MP, Yin, L, MacCabe, J, Rifkin, L, Hultman, CM (2012). Preterm birth and psychiatric disorders in young adult life. Archives of General Psychiatry 69, 610617.Google Scholar
Ogai, M, Matsumoto, H, Suzuki, K, Ozawa, F, Fukuda, R, Uchiyama, I, Suckling, J, Isoda, H, Mori, N, Takei, N (2003). fMRI study of recognition of facial expressions in high-functioning autistic patients. NeuroReport 14, 559563.Google Scholar
Omizzolo, C, Thompson, DK, Scratch, SE, Stargatt, R, Lee, KJ, Cheong, J, Roberts, G, Doyle, LW, Anderson, PJ (2013). Hippocampal volume and memory and learning outcomes at 7 years in children born very preterm. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 19, 10651075.Google Scholar
Pearson, JM, Heilbronner, SR, Barack, DL, Hayden, BY, Platt, ML (2011). Posterior cingulate cortex: adapting behavior to a changing world. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15, 143151.Google Scholar
Peng, D, Shi, F, Shen, T, Peng, Z, Zhang, C, Liu, X, Qiu, M, Liu, J, Jiang, K, Fang, Y, Shen, D (2014). Altered brain network modules induce helplessness in major depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders 168, 2129.Google Scholar
Pessoa, L, Padmala, S, Morland, T (2005). Fate of unattended fearful faces in the amygdala is determined by both attentional resources and cognitive modulation. NeuroImage 28, 249255.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peters, ER, Joseph, SA, Garety, PA (1999). Measurement of delusional ideation in the normal population: introducing the PDI (Peters et al. Delusions Inventory). Schizophrenia Bulletin 25, 553576.Google Scholar
Peterson, BS, Vohr, B, Staib, LH, Cannistraci, CJ, Dolberg, A, Schneider, KC, Katz, KH, Westerveld, M, Sparrow, S, Anderson, AW, Duncan, CC, Makuch, RW, Gore, JC, Ment, LR (2000). Regional brain volume abnormalities and long-term cognitive outcome in preterm infants. Journal of the American Medical Association 284, 19391947.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinto-Martin, JA, Levy, SE, Feldman, JF, Lorenz, JM, Paneth, N, Whitaker, AH (2011). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder in adolescents born weighing <2000 grams. Pediatrics 128, 883891.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poljac, E, Montagne, B, de Haan, EHF (2011). Reduced recognition of fear and sadness in post-traumatic stress disorder. Cortex 47, 974980.Google Scholar
Potharst, ES, van Wassenaer-Leemhuis, AG, Houtzager, BA, Livesey, D, Kok, JH, Last, BF, Oosterlaan, J (2013). Perinatal risk factors for neurocognitive impairments in preschool children born very preterm. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 55, 178184.Google Scholar
Power, JD, Barnes, KA, Snyder, AZ, Schlaggar, BL, Petersen, SE (2012). Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. NeuroImage 59, 21422154.Google Scholar
Puce, A, Allison, T, Bentin, S, Gore, JC, McCarthy, G (1998). Temporal cortex activation in humans viewing eye and mouth movements. Journal of Neuroscience 18, 21882199.Google Scholar
Qin, S, Young, CB, Duan, X, Chen, T, Supekar, K, Menon, V (2014). Amygdala subregional structure and intrinsic functional connectivity predicts individual differences in anxiety during early childhood. Biological Psychiatry 75, 892900.Google Scholar
Raichle, ME, MacLeod, AM, Snyder, AZ, Powers, WJ, Gusnard, DA, Shulman, GL (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 98, 676682.Google Scholar
Ramasubbu, R, Konduru, N, Cortese, F, Bray, S, Gaxiola-Valdez, I, Goodyear, B (2014). Reduced intrinsic connectivity of amygdala in adults with major depressive disorder. Frontiers in Psychiatry 5, 17.Google Scholar
Rickards, AL, Kelly, EA, Doyle, LW, Callanan, C (2001). Cognition, academic progress, behavior and self-concept at 14 years of very low birth weight children. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics 22, 1118.Google Scholar
Rogers, CE, Anderson, PJ, Thompson, DK, Kidokoro, H, Wallendorf, M, Treyvaud, K, Roberts, G, Doyle, LW, Neil, JJ, Inder, TE (2012). Regional cerebral development at term relates to school-age social–emotional development in very preterm children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 51, 181191.Google Scholar
Rogers, CE, Barch, DM, Sylvester, CM, Pagliaccio, D, Harms, MP, Botteron, KN, Luby, JL (2014). Altered gray matter volume and school age anxiety in children born late preterm. Journal of Pediatrics 165, 928935.Google Scholar
Roy, AK, Shehzad, Z, Margulies, DS, Kelly, AMC, Uddin, LQ, Gotimer, K, Biswal, BB, Castellanos, FX, Milham, MP (2009). Functional connectivity of the human amygdala using resting state fMRI. NeuroImage 45, 614626.Google Scholar
Saigal, S (2014). Functional outcomes of very premature infants into adulthood. Seminars in Fetal and Neonatal Medicine 19, 125130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saxe, R, Powell, LJ (2006). It's the thought that counts specific brain regions for one component of theory of mind. Psychological Science 17, 692699.Google Scholar
Schilbach, L, Timmermans, B, Reddy, V, Costall, A, Bente, G, Schlicht, T, & Vogeley, K (2013). Toward a second-person neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36, 393462.Google Scholar
Schmidt, LA, Miskovic, V, Boyle, MH, Saigal, S (2008). Shyness and timidity in young adults who were born at extremely low birth weight. Pediatrics 122, e181e187.Google Scholar
Scholten, MRM, Aleman, A, Montagne, B, Kahn, RS (2005). Schizophrenia and processing of facial emotions: sex matters. Schizophrenia Research 78, 6167.Google Scholar
Sølsnes, AE, Skranes, J, Brubakk, AM, Løhaugen, GCC (2014). Executive functions in very-low-birth-weight young adults: a comparison between self-report and neuropsychological test results. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 20, 506515.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spittle, AJ, Treyvaud, K, Doyle, LW, Roberts, G, Lee, KJ, Inder, TE, Cheong, JLY, Hunt, RW, Newnham, CA, Anderson, PJ (2009). Early emergence of behavior and social-emotional problems in very preterm infants. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 48, 909918.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sreenivas, S, Boehm, SG, Linden, DEJ (2012). Emotional faces and the default mode network. Neuroscience Letters 506, 229234.Google Scholar
Stein, JL, Wiedholz, LM, Bassett, DS, Weinberger, DR, Zink, CF, Mattay, VS, Meyer-Lindenberg, A (2007). A validated network of effective amygdala connectivity. NeuroImage 36, 736745.Google Scholar
Stewart, AL, Thorburn, RJ, Hope, PL, Goldsmith, M, Lipscomb, AP, Reynolds, EOR (1983). Ultrasound appearance of the brain in very preterm infants and neurodevelopmental outcome at 18 months of age. Archives of Disease in Childhood 58, 598604.Google Scholar
Van Dijk, KRA, Sabuncu, MR, Buckner, RL (2012). The influence of head motion on intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. NeuroImage 59, 431438.Google Scholar
Vander Wyk, BC, Hudac, CM, Carter, EJ, Sobel, DM, Pelphrey, KA (2009). Action understanding in the superior temporal sulcus region. Psychological Science 20, 771777.Google Scholar
Walshe, M, Rifkin, L, Rooney, M, Healy, E, Nosarti, C, Wyatt, J, Stahl, D, Murray, RM, Allin, M (2008). Psychiatric disorder in young adults born very preterm: role of family history. European Psychiatry 23, 527531.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. The Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX.Google Scholar
White, TP, Symington, I, Castellanos, NP, Brittain, PJ, Walsh, SF, Nam, KW, Sato, JR, Allin, MPG, Shergill, SS, Murray, RM, Williams, SCR, Nosarti, C (2014). Dysconnectivity of neurocognitive networks at rest in very-preterm born adults. NeuroImage: Clinical 4, 352365.Google Scholar
Whitfield-Gabrieli, S, Moran, JM, Nieto-Castañón, A, Triantafyllou, C, Saxe, R, Gabrieli, JDE (2011). Associations and dissociations between default and self-reference networks in the human brain. NeuroImage 55, 225232.Google Scholar
Whitfield-Gabrieli, S, Nieto-Castañón, A (2012). Conn: a functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks. Brain Connectivity 2, 125141.Google Scholar
Williamson, KE, Jakobson, LS (2014). Social perception in children born at very low birthweight and its relationship with social/behavioral outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 55, 990998.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Witt, A, Theurel, A, Tolsa, CB, Lejeune, F, Fernandes, L, de Jonge, LvH, Monnier, M, Graz, MB, Barisnikov, K, Gentaz, E, Hüppi, PS (2014). Emotional and effortful control abilities in 42-month-old very preterm and full-term children. Early Human Development 90, 565569.Google Scholar
Wocadlo, C, Rieger, I (2006). Social skills and nonverbal decoding of emotions in very preterm children at early school age. European Journal of Developmental Psychology 3, 4870.Google Scholar
Wolke, D, Baumann, N, Strauss, V, Johnson, S, Marlow, N (2015). Bullying of preterm children and emotional problems at school age: cross-culturally invariant effects. Journal of Pediatrics 166, 14171422.Google Scholar
Woods, S, Wolke, D, Nowicki, S, Hall, L (2009). Emotion recognition abilities and empathy of victims of bullying. Child Abuse and Neglect 33, 307311.Google Scholar
Wright, P, Albarracin, D, Brown, RD, Li, H, He, G, Liu, Y (2008). Dissociated responses in the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex to bottom-up and top-down components of emotional evaluation. NeuroImage 39, 894902.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Sample characteristics. Frequencies, percentages and mean values (standard deviations) are given, unless otherwise specified

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Emotion Recognition Task (ERT) performance. (a) Mean scores and (b) mean reaction times of very preterm birth (VPT) participants and controls for the six ERT emotions. Error bars represent 1 standard deviation. (c) Mean scores and (d) mean reaction times of VPT participants (white dots) and controls (black triangles) at the four levels of intensity for anger. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.001.

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Alterations in functional connectivity of the left superficial amygdala in very preterm birth (VPT)  individuals compared to controls. (a) Location of 50% probabilistic masks of the superficial amygdala of the left hemisphere projected on the MNI template with neurological convention. (b) Brain areas that showed significantly greater functional connectivity of the left superficial amygdala in VPT individuals (hot colour scale) comprised the right superior temporal sulcus (STS); brain areas that showed significantly smaller functional connectivity of the left superficial amygdala in VPT individuals (cool colour scale) comprised the right posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) and the left precuneus (pC). Brain slices are displayed axially according to neurological convention, with z coordinates in MNI space above each slice. The figure on the right depicts these slices in blue on a midline, sagittal slice. Reported clusters survived a height threshold of uncorrected p < 0.001 and an extent threshold of FDR-corrected p < 0.05 at the cluster level. (c) Bar graph showing direct comparison of functional connectivity strength between the two groups. The y-axis indicates correlation coefficients between the time series of the seed region (superficial subregion of the left amygdala) and the time series extracted from the regions displaying significant between-group differences; the error bars represent 1 standard error.

Figure 3

Table 2. Differences in functional connectivity of the left superficial amygdala between VPT individuals and controls

Figure 4

Fig. 3. Correlation between superficial amygdala-posterior cingulate connectivity and Emotion Recognition Task score for anger in very preterm birth (VPT) participants (white dots; Spearman's ρ = 0.172) and controls (black triangles; Spearman's ρ = 0.426). Least-squared lines are shown for illustrative purposes only and do not constitute a formal test.

Supplementary material: File

Papini supplementary material

Papini supplementary material

Download Papini supplementary material(File)
File 699.3 KB