Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-xm8r8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-27T00:49:55.308Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Spontaneous verbal repetition in toddler-adult conversations: a longitudinal study with Spanish-speaking two- year-olds

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 March 2021

Marta CASLA*
Affiliation:
Dpto. Psicología Evolutiva y de la Educación. Facultad de Psicología. Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Spain.
Celia MÉNDEZ-CABEZAS
Affiliation:
Dpto. Psicología Evolutiva y de la Educación. Facultad de Psicología. Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Spain.
Ignacio MONTERO
Affiliation:
Dpto. Psicología Social y Metodología. Facultad de Psicología. Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Spain.
Eva MURILLO
Affiliation:
Dpto. Psicología Básica. Facultad de Psicología. Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Spain.
Silvia NIEVA
Affiliation:
Dpto. de Psicología Experimental, Procesos Cognitivos y Logopedia. Facultad de Psicología.Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Spain.
Jessica RODRÍGUEZ
Affiliation:
Dpto. Psicología Evolutiva y de la Educación. Facultad de Psicología. Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Spain.
*
Address for correspondence: Marta Casla, Dpto. Psicología Evolutiva y de la Educación, Facultad de Psicología. Universidad Autónoma de Madrid. E-mail: marta.casla@uam.es

Abstract

The role of children’s verbal repetition of parents’ utterances on vocabulary growth has been well documented (Masur, 1999). Nevertheless, few studies have analyzed adults’ and children’s spontaneous verbal repetition around the second birthday distinguishing between the types of repetition. We analyzed longitudinally Spanish-speaking parent-child dyads during spontaneous interaction at 21, 24 and 30 months. Linguistic level was measured using the Spanish version of the MacArthur CDI (López-Ornat et al., 2005). Children’s and adults’ repetitions are about 17% of the speech. Children repeated adults’ utterances in a reduced manner whereas adults produced more extended repetitions. Adults’ rate of repetition predicted children’s linguistic level at 30 months. Children’s rate of repetition did not predict linguistic level. These results suggest that parents adapt their speech to children’s communicative abilities. Since children’s rate of repetition did not predict linguistic level, we suggest that verbal imitation plays an indirect and complex role in communicative development.

Type
Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ambridge, B., Kidd, E., Rowland, C. F., & Theakston, A. L. (2015). The ubiquity of frequency effects in first language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 42, 239273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bannard, C., Klinger, J., & Tomasello, M. (2013). How selective are 3-year-olds in imitating novel linguistic material? Developmental Psychology, 49(12), 2344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruner, J. (1978). From communication to language: A psychological perspective. Cognition, 3, 255287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Che, E. S., Brooks, P. J., Alarcon, M. F., Yannaco, F. D., & Donnelly, S. (2018). Assessing the impact of conversational overlap in content on child language growth. Journal of Child Language, 45, 7296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, E.V. (2007). Young children's uptake of new words in conversation. Language in Society, 36 (2), 157182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V., & Bernicot, J. (2008). Repetition as ratification: how parents and children place information in common ground. Journal of Child Language, 35, 349371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conway, L. J., Lvickis, P. A., Smith, J., Mensah, F., Wake, M., & Reilly, S. (2018). Maternal communicative behaviours and interaction quality as predictors of language development: findings from a community-based study of slow-to-talk toddlers. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 53 (2), 339354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dale, R., & Spivey, M. (2006). Unraveling the dyad: using recurrence analysis to explore patterns of syntactic coordination between children and caregivers in conversation. Language Learning, 56 (3), 391430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freudenthal, D., Pine, J. M., Aguado-Orea, J. A., & Gobet, F. (2007). Modeling the Developmental Patterning of Finiteness Marking in English, Dutch, German, and Spanish Using MOSAIC. Cognitive Science, 31, 311341.Google ScholarPubMed
Herr-Israel, E., & McCune, L. (2011). Successive single-word utterances and use of conversational input: a pre-syntactic route to multiword utterances. Journal of child language, 38(1), 166180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hodges, R.M., Munro, N., Baker, E., McGregor, K., Docking, K., & Arciuli, J. (2016). The role of elicited imitation in toddlers’ word learning. Journal of Child Language, 43, 457471.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, S. S. (2007). Imitation in infancy: The development of mimicry. Psychological Science, 18(7), 593599.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuchirko, Y., Tafuro, L., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. (2018). Becoming a Communicative Partner: Infant Contigent Responsiveness to Maternal Language and Gestures. Infancy, 23(4), 558576.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Küntay, A., & Slobin, D. I. (1996). Listening to a Turkish mother: Some puzzles for acquisition. In Slobin, D. I., Gerhardt, J., Kyratzis, A., & Guo, J. (Eds.), Social interaction, social context, and language: Essays in honor of Susan Ervin-Tripp (pp. 265286). Erlbaum.Google Scholar
López-Ornat, S., Gallego, C., Gallo, P., Karousou, A., & Mariscal, S. (2005). Inventario de desarrollo comunicativo MacArthur. TEA Ediciones.Google Scholar
MacGillion, M. L., Herbert, J. S., Pine, J. M., Keren-Portnoy, T., Vihman, M. M., & Matthews, D. E. (2013). Supporting Early Vocabulary Development: What Sort of Responsiveness Matters?. IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development, 5(3), 240248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES Project: Tools for Analyzing Talk. 3rd Edition.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Masur, E. F. (1995). Infants’ early verbal imitation and their later lexical Development. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 41(3), 286306.Google Scholar
Masur, E. F., & Eichorst, D. L. (2002). Infants' spontaneous imitation of novel versus familiar words: Relations to observational and maternal report measures of their lexicons. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 48(4), 405426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masur, E. F., & Rodemaker, J. E. (1999). Mothers' and infants' spontaneous vocal, verbal, and action imitation during the second year. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 45(3), 392412.Google Scholar
Olson, J., & Masur, E. F. (2012). Mothers respond differently to infants’ familiar versus non-familiar verbal imitations. Journal of Child Language, 39, 731752.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Onnis, L., Waterfall, H. R., & Edelman, S. (2008). Learn locally, act globally: Learning language from variation set cues. Cognition, 109(3), 423430.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rujas, I., Casla, M., Mariscal, S., Lázaro López-Villaseñor, M., & Murillo, E. (2019). Effects of grammatical category and morphology on fast mapping in typically developing and late talking toddlers. First Language, 39(3):249280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwab, J. F., & Lew-Williams, C. (2016). Repetition across successive sentences facilitates young children's word learning. Developmental Psychology, 52, 879886.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwab, J. F., Rowe, M. L., Cabrera, N., & Lew-Williams, C. (2018). Fathers’ repetition of words is coupled with children's vocabularies, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 166, 437450.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, C. E. (1981). The uses of imitation. Journal of Child Language, 8(1), 205212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sokolov, J. L., & MacWhinney, B. (1990). The CHIP framework: automatic coding and analysis of parent–child conversational interaction. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 22(2), 151161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stine, E. L., & Bohannon, J. N. (1983). Imitations, interactions, and language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 10(3), 589603.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suanda, S. S., Smith, L. B., & Yu, C. (2016). The Multisensory Nature of Verbal Discourse in Parent–Toddler Interactions. Developmental Neuropsychology, 41 (5–8), 324341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., & Baumwell, L. (2001). Maternal responsiveness and children's achievement of language milestones. Child Development, 72 (3) 748767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Kuchirko, Y., & Song, L. (2014). Why Is Infant Language Learning Facilitated by Parental Responsiveness? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(2), 121126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Užgiris, I. Č., Broome, S., & Kruper, J. C. (1989). Imitation in mother-child conversations: a focus on the mother. In Speidel, G. E. & Nelson, K. E. (Eds.), The many faces of imitation in language learning (pp. 91120). Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veneziano, E. (2013). A Cognitive-Pragmatic Model for the change from single-word to multiword speech: A constructivist approach. Journal of Pragmatics, 56, 133150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wirén, M., Björkenstam, K. N., Grigonyté, G., & Eir Cortes, E. (2016) Longitudinal Studies of Variation Sets in Child-directed Speech. Proceedings of the 7th Workshop on Cognitive Aspects of Computational Language Learning, (44–52).CrossRefGoogle Scholar