Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-ndmmz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-30T14:17:32.665Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Slowing life history (K) can account for increasing micro-innovation rates and GDP growth, but not macro-innovation rates, which declined following the end of the Industrial Revolution

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 November 2019

Michael A. Woodley of Menie
Affiliation:
Center Leo Apostel for Interdisciplinary Studies, Vrije Universiteit Brussel, B-1160 Brussels, BelgiumMichael.Woodley@vub.ac.behttps://www.vub.be/CLEA/cgi-bin/homepage.cgi?email=michael.woodley03%5bat%5dgmail.com Unz Foundation Junior Fellow, Palo Alto, CA 94301
Aurelio José Figueredo
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ 85721ajf@u.arizona.eduhttps://psychology.arizona.edu/users/aurelio-figueredo
Matthew A. Sarraf
Affiliation:
Independent Researcher.mas626@cornell.edu

Abstract

Baumard proposes that life history slowing in populations over time is the principal driver of innovation rates. We show that this is only true of micro-innovation rates, which reflect cognitive and economic specialization as an adaptation to high population density, and not macro-innovation rates, which relate more to a population's level of general intelligence.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Figueredo, A. J., Cabeza de Baca, T., Fernandes, H. B. F., Black, C. J., Peñaherrera-Aguirre, M., Hertler, S., Garcia, R. A., Meisenberg, G. & Woodley of Menie, M. A. (2017) A sequential canonical cascade model of social biogeography: Plants, parasites, and people. Evolutionary Psychological Science 1:4061.Google Scholar
Figueredo, A. J., Peñaherrera-Aguirre, M., Fernandes, H. B. F., Lomayesva, S. L., Woodley of Menie, M. A., Hertler, S. C., Sarraf, M. A. (2019) War and peace: A diachronic social biogeography of life history strategy and between-group relations in two Western European populations. Journal of Methods and Measurement in the Social Sciences 10(1):3675.Google Scholar
Fogel, R. W. (1964) Railroads and American economic growth: Essays in econometric history. Johns Hopkins Press.Google Scholar
Hertler, S. C., Figueredo, A. J., Peñaherrera-Aguirre, M., Fernandes, H. B. F. & Woodley of Menie, M. A. (2018) Life history evolution: A biological meta-theory for the social sciences. Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Huebner, J. (2005) A possible declining trend for worldwide innovation. Technological Forecasting and Social Change 72:980–86.Google Scholar
Inglehart, R. & Welzel, C. (2005) Modernization, cultural change, and democracy: The human development sequence. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mace, R. (1999) Evolutionary ecology of human life history. Animal Behavior 59:110.Google Scholar
Pietschnig, J. & Voracek, M. (2015) One century of global IQ gains: A formal meta-analysis of the Flynn effect (1909–2013). Perspectives on Psychological Science 10:282306.Google Scholar
Reeve, C. L., Heeney, M. D. & Woodley of Menie, M.A. (2018) A systematic review of the state of the literature relating parental general cognitive ability and number of offspring. Personality and Individual Differences 134:107–18.Google Scholar
Rindermann, H. & Becker, D. (2018) FLynn-effect and economic growth: Do national increases in intelligence lead to increases in GDP? Intelligence 69:8793.Google Scholar
Sarraf, M. (2017) Review of historical variability in heritable general intelligence: Its evolutionary origins and socio-cultural consequences. Personality and Individual Differences 109:238–41.Google Scholar
Woodley, M. A. (2012) The social and scientific temporal correlates of genotypic intelligence and the Flynn effect. Intelligence 40:189204.Google Scholar
Woodley, M. A., Figueredo, A. J., Ross, K. C. & Brown, S. D. (2013) Four successful tests of the cognitive differentiation-integration effort hypothesis. Intelligence 41:832–42.Google Scholar
Woodley of Menie, M. A., Cabeza de Baca, T., Fernandes, H. B. F., Madison, G. & Figueredo, A. J. (2017a) Slow and steady wins the race: K positively predicts fertility in the USA and Sweden. Evolutionary Psychological Science 3:109–17.Google Scholar
Woodley of Menie, M. A., Figueredo, A. J., Sarraf, M. A., Hertler, S. C., Fernandes, H. B. F. & Peñaherrera-Aguirre, M. (2017b) The rhythm of the west: A biohistory of the modern era AD 1600 to the present. Journal of Social Political and Economic Studies, Monograph Series, No. 37. Scott Townsend Press.Google Scholar
Woodley of Menie, M. A. & Madison, G. (2015) The association between g and K in a sample of 4246 Swedish twins: A behavior genetic analysis. Personality and Individual Differences 75:8084.Google Scholar