Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-5c6d5d7d68-qks25 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-08-16T07:15:40.712Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

16 - Figurative Language Comprehension and Laterality in Autism Spectrum Disorder

from Part IV - Memory and Language

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 January 2018

Rex E. Jung
Affiliation:
University of New Mexico
Oshin Vartanian
Affiliation:
University of Toronto
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: Text revision (4th ed., rev.) Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM V) (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Au Yeung, S. K., Kaakinen, J. K., Liversedge, S. P., & Benson, V. (2015). Processing of written irony in autism spectrum disorder: An eye movement study. Autism Research, 8, 749760.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a ‘theory of mind’? Cognition, 21, 3746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beeman, M. (1998). Coarse semantic coding and discourse comprehension. In Beeman, M., & Chiarello, C. (Eds.), Right hemisphere language comprehension: Perspectives from cognitive neuroscience (pp. 255284). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Bowdle, B. F., & Gentner, D. (2005). The career of metaphor. Psychological Review, 112, 193216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, L., Sherbenou, R. J., & Johnsen, S. K. (1997). Test of nonverbal intelligence: TONI-3 (3rd ed.). Austin, TX: PRO-ED.Google Scholar
Cardinale, R. C., Shih, P., Fishman, I., Ford, L. M., & Muller, R.-A. (2013). Pervasive rightward asymmetry shifts of functional networks in autism spectrum disorder. JAMA Psychiatry, 70, 975982.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, H. H., & Gerrig, R. J. (1984). On the pretense theory of irony. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 121126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colich, N. L., Wang, A. T., Rudie, J. D., Hermandez, L. M., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Dapretto, M. (2012). Atypical neural processing of ironic and sincere remarks in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Metaphor and Symbol, 27(1), 7092.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craig, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). Creativity and imagination in autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 319326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crespi, B., & Badcock, C. (2008). Psychosis and autism as diametrical disorders of the social brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 241261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crespi, B. J. (2016). Autism as a disorder of high intelligence. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 10, 117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawson, G., Finley, C., Phillips, S., & Galpert, L. (1986). Hemispheric specialization and the language abilities of autistic children. Child Development, 57(6), 14401453.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dews, S., & Winner, E. (1995). Muting the meaning: A social function of irony. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 10, 319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dichter, G. S., Lam, K. S. L., Turner-Brown, L. M., Holtzclaw, T. N., & Bodfish, J. W. (2009). Generativity abilities predict communication deficits but not repetitive behaviors in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 12981304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eviatar, Z., & Just, M.A. (2006). Brain correlates of discourse processing: An fMRI investigation of irony and conventional metaphor comprehension. Neuropsychologia, 44, 23482359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eyler, L. T., Pierce, K., & Courchesne, E. (2012). A failure of left temporal cortex to specialize for language is an early emerging and fundamental property of autism. Brain, 135, 949960.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Filippova, E., & Astington, J. W. (2008). Further development in social reasoning in discourse irony understanding. Child Development, 79, 126138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Filippova, E., & Astington, J. W. (2010). Children’s understanding of social-cognitive and social-communicative aspects of discourse irony. Child Development, 81(3), 913928.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gage, N. M., Juranek, J., Filipek, P. A., Osann, K., Flodman, P., Isenberg, A. L., & Spence, M. A. (2009). Rightward hemispheric asymmetries in auditory language cortex in children with autistic disorder: An MRI Investigation. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 1, 205214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gernsbacher, M. A., & Pripas-Kapit, S. R. (2012). Who’s missing the point? A commentary on claims that autistic persons have a specific deficit in figurative language comprehension. Metaphor and Symbol, 27, 93105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R. W. (2000). Irony in talk among friends. Metaphor and Symbol, 15, 527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R. W., Bryant, G. A., & Colston, H. L. (2014). Where is the humor in verbal irony? Humor: International Journal of Humor Research, 27, 575595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giora, R. (1997). Understanding figurative and literal language: The graded salience hypothesis. Cognitive Linguistics, 7, 183206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giora, R. (1999). On the priority of salient meaning: Studies of literal and figurative language. Journal of Pragmatics, 31, 919929.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giora, R. (2003). On our mind: Salience, context, and figurative language. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giora, R., Gazal, O., Goldstein, I., Fein, O., & Stringarisi, A. (2012). Salience and context: Interpretation of metaphorical and literal language by young adults diagnosed with Asperger’s syndrome. Metaphor and Symbol, 27, 2254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gold, R., & Faust, M. (2010). Right hemisphere dysfunction and metaphor comprehension in Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(7), 800811.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gold, R., & Faust, M. (2012) Metaphor comprehension in persons with Asperger syndrome: Systemized versus non-systemized semantic processing. Metaphor and Symbol, 27, 5569.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gold, R., Faust, M., & Ben-Artzi, E. (2012). Metaphors and verbal creativity: The role of the right hemisphere. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition, 17, 602614.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Happé, F. (1993).Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: A test of relevance theory. Cognition, 48, 101119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Happé, F., & Vital, P. (2009). What aspects of autism predispose to talent? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364, 13691375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hermann, I., Haser, V., Van, E., Ludger, T., Ebert, D., Müller-Feldmeth, D., … Konieczny, L. (2013). Automatic metaphor processing in adults with Asperger syndrome: A metaphor interference effect task. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 263, S177S187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iakimova, G., Passerieux, C., Denhiere, G., Laurent, J. P., Vistoli, D., Vilain, J., & Hardy-Bayle, M. C. (2010). The influence of idiomatic salience during the comprehension of ambiguous idioms by patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 177, 4654.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jung, R. E. (2014). Evolution, creativity, intelligence, and madness: “Here Be Dragons”. Frontiers in Psychology, 15, 13.Google Scholar
Jung, R. E., Segall, J. M., Jeremy Bockholt, H., Flores, R. A., Smith, S. M., Chavez, R. S., & Haier, R. J. (2010). Neuroanatomy of creativity. Human Brain Mapping, 31, 398409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kasirer, A., & Mashal, N. (2014). Verbal creativity in autism: Comprehension and generation of metaphoric language in high functioning autism spectrum disorder and typical development. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 818.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kavé, G. (2005). Phonemic fluency, semantic fluency, and difference scores: Normative data for adult Hebrew speakers. Journal of Clinical and Experimental. Neuropsychology, 27, 690699.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kavé, G., Avraham, , Kukulansky-Segal, A., , D., & Herzberg, O. (2007). How does the homophone meaning generation test associate with the phonemic and semantic fluency tests? A quantitative and qualitative analysis. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13, 424432.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kleinhans, N. M., Muller, R.-A., Cohen, D. N., & Courchesne, E. (2008). Atypical functional lateralization of language in autism spectrum disorders. Brain Research, 1221, 115125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levorato, M. C., & Cacciari, C. (1999). Idiom comprehension in children: Are the effects of semantic analysability and context separable? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 11(1), 5166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindell, A.K. (2006). In your right mind: Right hemisphere contributions to language processing and production. Neuropsychology Review, 16, 131148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacKay, G., & Shaw, A. (2004). A comparative study of figurative language in children with autistic spectrum disorders. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 20, 1332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, I., & McDonald, S. (2004). An exploration of causes of non-literal language problems in individuals with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism Developmental Disorders, 34, 311328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mashal, N. (2013). The role of working memory in the comprehension of unfamiliar and familiar metaphors. Language and Cognition, 5(4), 409436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mashal, N., Faust, M., & Hendler, T. (2005). The role of the right hemisphere in processing nonsalient metaphorical meaning: Application of principal components analysis to fMRI data. Neuropsychologia, 43(14), 20842100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mashal, N., Faust, M., Hendler, T., & Jung-Beeman, M. (2007). An fMRI investigation of the neural correlates underlying the processing of novel metaphorical expressions. Brain and Language, 100, 115126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mashal, N., & Kasirer, A. (2011). Thinking maps enhance metaphoric competence in children with autism and learning disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32, 20452054.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melogno, S.. D’Ardia, C., Pinto, M. A., & Levi, G. (2012). Explaining metaphors in high-functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder children: A brief report. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6(2), 683689.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, R. L. C., & Crow, T. J. (2005). Right hemisphere language functions and schizophrenia: The forgotten hemisphere? Brain, 128, 963978.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muller, R. A., Behen, M. E., Rothermel, R. D., Chugani, D. C., Muzik, O., Mangner, T. J., & Chugani, H. T. (1999). Brain mapping of language and auditory perception in high-functioning autistic adults: A PET study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 1931.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nippold, M. A., & Taylor, C. L. (2002). Judgments of idiom familiarity and transparency: A comparison of children and adolescents. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45(2), 384391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norbury, C. F. (2004) Factors supporting idiom comprehension in children with communication disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47(5), 11791193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olofson, E. L., Casey, D., Oluyedun, O. A., Van Herwegen, J., Becerra, A., & Rundblad, G. (2014). Youth with autism spectrum disorder comprehend lexicalized and novel primary conceptual metaphors. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 25682583.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ozonoff, S., & Miller, J. N. (1996). An exploration of right hemisphere contributions to the pragmatic impairments of autism. Brain and Language, 52, 411434.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pexman, P. M., & Glenwright, M.(2007). How do typically developing children grasp the meaning of verbal irony? Journal of Neurolinguistics, 20(2), 178196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pexman, P. M., Rostad, K. R., McMorris, C. A., Climie, E. A., Stowkowy, J., & Glenwright, M. R. (2011). Processing of ironic language in children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism Developmental Disordered, 41, 10971112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rapp, A. (2009). The role of the right hemisphere for language in schizophrenia. In Sommer, I. E. C., & Khan, R. S. (Eds.), Language lateralization in psychosis (pp. 147156). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reitan, R. M., & Davison, L. A. (Eds.). (1974). Clinical neuropsychology: Current status and applications. Washington, DC: Winston.Google Scholar
Rinehart, N. J., Bradshaw, J. L., Brereto, A. V., & Tonge, B. J. (2002). Lateralization in individuals with high-functioning autism and Asperger disorder: A frontostriatal model. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 321331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, R. M., & Kreuz, R. J. (1994).Why do people use figurative language? Psychological Science, 5, 159163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rundblad, G., & Annaz, D. (2010). The atypical development of metaphor and metonymy comprehension in children with autism. Autism, 14, 2946.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saban-Bezalel, R., & Mashal, N. (2015a). Hemispheric processing of idioms and irony in adults with and without pervasive developmental disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45, 34963508.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saban-Bezalel, R., & Mashal, N. (2015b).The effects of intervention on the comprehension of irony and on hemispheric processing of irony in adults with ASD. Neuropsychologia, 77, 233241.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silvia, P. J., & Beaty, R. E. (2012). Making creative metaphors: The importance of fluid intelligence for creative thought. Intelligence, 40(4), 343351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swinney, D. A., & Cutler, A. (1979). The access and processing of idiomatic expressions. Journal of Verbal and Learning Behavior, 18, 523534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thoma, P., & Daum, I. (2006). Neurocognitive of figurative language processing – Evidence from clinical dysfunction. Neuroscience Biobehavioral Review, 30(8), 11821205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vulchanova, M., Saldaña, D., Chahboun, , , S., & Vulchanov, V. (2015). Figurative language processing in atypical populations: The ASD perspective. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, A. T., Lee, S. S., Sigman, M., & Dapretto, M. (2006). Neural basis of irony comprehension in children with autism: The role of prosody and context. Brain, 129, 932943.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (2001). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Whyte, E. M., Nelson, K. E., & Scherf, K. S. (2014). Idiom, syntax, and advanced theory of mind abilities in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57(1), 120130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, D. L., Cherkassky, V. L., Mason, R. A., Keller, T. A., Minshew, N. J., & Just, A. M. (2013). Brain function differences in language processing in children and adult with autism. Autism Research, 6, 288302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winner, E., Levy, J., Kaplan, J., & Rosenblatt, E. (1988). Children’s understanding of nonliteral language. Journal of Aesthetic Education, 22, 5163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×