Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-01T20:04:12.183Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

7 - Chimpanzee behavioural diversity and the contribution of the Taï Chimpanzee Project

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 November 2019

Christophe Boesch
Affiliation:
Max-Planck-Institut für Evolutionäre Anthropologie, Germany
Roman Wittig
Affiliation:
Max-Planck-Institut für Evolutionäre Anthropologie, Germany
Catherine Crockford
Affiliation:
Max-Planck-Institut für Evolutionäre Anthropologie, Germany
Linda Vigilant
Affiliation:
Max-Planck-Institut für Evolutionäre Anthropologie, Germany
Tobias Deschner
Affiliation:
Max-Planck-Institut für Evolutionäre Anthropologie, Germany
Fabian Leendertz
Affiliation:
Robert Koch-Institut, Germany
Get access

Summary

Evolutionary theory predicts that individuals adapt to the living conditions faced during their lifespan. From the start of the Taï Chimpanzee Project, the foremost question was how living in a dense rainforest affects the behaviour patterns compared to the Gombe and Mahale chimpanzees, which lived in a more open environment dominated by mosaic habitat with dry woodland savanna patches and some gallery forest. Do Taï chimpanzees need a precise mental map to find their food in this dense rainforest? How does the more social nature of the social life of the Taï chimpanzees affect paternity in this population? How old is tool use, exemplified by the stone hammers used to crack nuts in Taï? Where do females come from and where are they going when they transfer between communities as young adults? Is there a difference in the cooperation level between populations when hunting arboreal monkeys and why? In all these domains, the Taï Chimpanzee Project presented new data and replicated them over different periods of time and different social groups. The role of the environment for each of this domain was discussed and proved to be essential in explaining population differences.

Type
Chapter
Information
The Chimpanzees of the Taï Forest
40 Years of Research
, pp. 89 - 103
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alcock, J. (1989). Animal Behavior: An Evolutionary Approach, 4th edn. Sunderland:Sinauer Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
Birkhead, T. & Møller, P. (1992). Sperm Competition in Birds: Evolutionary Causes and Consequences. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Boesch, C. (1991). Symbolic communication in wild chimpanzees? Human Evolution, 6, 8190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boesch, C. (1994a). Chimpanzees–red colobus: A predator–prey system. Animal Behaviour, 47, 11351148.Google Scholar
Boesch, C. (1994b). Cooperative hunting in wild chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 48, 653667.Google Scholar
Boesch, C. (2002). Cooperative hunting roles among Taï chimpanzees. Human Nature, 13, 2746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boesch, C. & Boesch, H. (1989). Hunting behavior of wild chimpanzees in the Taï National Park. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 78, 547573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boesch, C. & Boesch-Achermann, H. (2000). The Chimpanzees of the Taï Forest: Behavioural Ecology and Evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boesch, C., Crockford, C., Herbinger, I., Wittig, R., Moebius, Y. & Normand, E. (2008). Intergroup conflicts among chimpanzees in Taï National Park: Lethal violence and the female perspective. American Journal of Primatology, 70, 114.Google Scholar
Boesch, C., Head, J. & Robbins, M. M. (2009). Complex tool sets for honey extraction among chimpanzees in Loango National Park, Gabon. Journal of Human Evolution, 56, 560569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boesch, C., Kalan, A., Agbor, A., Arandjelovic, M., Dieguez, P., Lapeyre, V., et al. (2017). Chimpanzees routinely fish for algae with tools during the dry season in Bakoun, Guinea. American Journal of Primatology, 79(3), e22613.Google Scholar
Boesch, C., Kohou, G., Néné, H. & Vigilant, L. (2006). Male competition and paternity in wild chimpanzees of the Taï forest. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 130, 103115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boesch, C., Marchesi, P., Marchesi, N., Fruth, B. & Joulian, F. (1994). Is nut cracking in wild chimpanzees a cultural behaviour? Journal of Human Evolution, 26, 325338.Google Scholar
Boesch-Achermann, H. & Boesch, C. (1994). Hominization in the rainforest: The chimpanzee’s piece of the puzzle. Evolutionary Anthropology, 3, 916.Google Scholar
Bradley, B. J., Boesch, C. & Vigilant, L. (2000). Identification and redesign of human microsatellite markers for genotyping wild chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes verus) and gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) DNA from faeces. Conservation Genetics, 1, 289292.Google Scholar
Bradley, B. & Vigilant, L. (2002). False alleles derived from microbial DNA pose a potential source of error in microsatellite genotyping of DNA from faeces. Molecular Ecology Resources, 2, 602605.Google Scholar
Burke, T., Davies, N., Bruford, M. & Hatchwell, B. (1989). Parental care and mating behaviour of polyandrous dunnocks Prunella modularis related to paternity by DNA fingerprinting. Nature, 338, 249251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, M. (2002). In Search of Human Nature. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Constable, J., Ashley, M., Goodall, J. & Pusey, A. (2001). Noninvasive paternity assignment in Gombe chimpanzees. Molecular Ecology, 10, 12791300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fahy, G., Richards, M., Riedel, J., Hublin, J-J. & Boesch, C. (2013). Stable isotope evidence of meat eating and hunting specialization in adult male chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110, 58295833.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fehr, E. & Fischbacher, U. (2004). Social norms and human cooperation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 185190.Google Scholar
Gagneux, P., Woodruff, D. & Boesch, C. (1997). Furtive mating in female chimpanzees. Nature, 387, 358359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodall, J. (1963). Feeding behaviour of wild chimpanzees: A preliminary report. Symposium of the Zoological Society, London, 10, 3948.Google Scholar
Goodall, J. (1964). Tool-using and aimed throwing in a community of free-living chimpanzees. Nature, 201, 12641266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodall, J. (1986). The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behavior. Cambridge, MA: The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Janmaat, K., Ban, S. & Boesch, C. (2013a). Taï chimpanzees use botanical skills to discover fruit: What we can learn from their mistakes. Animal Cognition, 16, 851860.Google Scholar
Janmaat, K., Ban, S. & Boesch, C. (2013b). Chimpanzees use long-term spatial memory to monitor large fruit trees and remember feeding experiences across seasons. Animal Behaviour, 86, 11831205.Google Scholar
Janmaat, K., Boesch, C., Byrne, R., Chapman, C., Gone Bi, Z., Head, J., et al. (2016). Spatio-temporal complexity of chimpanzee food: How cognitive adaptations can counteract the ephemeral nature of ripe fruit. American Journal of Primatology, 78, 626645.Google Scholar
Krebs, J. R. & Davies, N. B. (1998). Behavioural Ecology, 4th edn. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications.Google Scholar
Kühl, H. S., Kalan, A. K., Arandjelovic, M., Aubert, F., D’Auvergne, L., Goedmakers, A., et al. (2016). Chimpanzee accumulative stone throwing. Scientific Reports, 6, 22219.Google Scholar
Luncz, L. & Boesch, C. (2014). Tradition over trend: Neighboring chimpanzee communities maintain differences in cultural behavior despite frequent immigration of adult females. American Journal of Primatology, 76, 649657.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luncz, L. & Boesch, C. (2015). The extent of cultural variations between adjacent chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes verus) communities; a microecological approach. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 156, 6775.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luncz, L., Mundry, R. & Boesch, C. (2012). Evidence for cultural differences between neighboring chimpanzee communities. Current Biology, 22, 922926.Google Scholar
Luncz, L., Wittig, R. & Boesch, C. (2015). Primate archaeology reveals cultural transmission in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus). Philosophical Transactions from the Royal Society B, 370, 20140348.Google Scholar
Matsuzawa, T. (2002). Chimpanzees of Bossou and Nimba 1976–2001. Kyoto: Primate Research Institute.Google Scholar
Maynard Smith, J. (1982). Evolution and the Theory of Games. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mercader, J., Barton, H., Gillespie, J., Harris, J., Kuhn, S., Tyler, R., et al. (2007). 4,300-year-old chimpanzee sites and the origins of percussive stone technology. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 30433048.Google Scholar
Mercader, J., Panger, M. & Boesch, C. (2002). Excavation of a chimpanzee stone tool site in the African rainforest. Science, 296, 14521455.Google Scholar
Mitani, J. & Watts, D. (2001) Why do chimpanzees hunt and share meat? Animal Behaviour, 61, 110.Google Scholar
Mitani, J., Watts, D. & Muller, M. (2002). Recent development in the study of wild chimpanzee behaviour. Evolutionary Anthropology, 11, 925.Google Scholar
Morgan, B. & Abwe, E. (2006). Chimpanzees use stone hammers in Cameroon. Current Biology, 16, R632R633.Google Scholar
Morin, P. A., Wallis, J. J., Moore, J. J. & Woodruff, D. S. (1994). Paternity exclusion in a community of wild chimpanzees using hypervariable simple sequence repeats. Molecular Ecology, 3, 469478.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muller, M. & Mitani, J. (2005). Conflict and cooperation in wild chimpanzees. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 35, 275331.Google Scholar
Newton-Fisher, N. E., Emery Thompson, M., Reynolds, V., Boesch, C. & Vigilant, L. (2009). Paternity and social rank in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) from the Budongo Forest, Uganda. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 142, 417428.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. (1968). The social group of wild chimpanzees in the Mahali Mountains. Primates, 9, 167224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. (1990). The Chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains: Sexual and Life History Strategies. Tokyo: University of Tokyo Press.Google Scholar
Normand, E. & Boesch, C. (2009). Sophisticated Euclidian maps in forest chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 77, 11951201.Google Scholar
Normand, E., Ban, S. & Boesch, C. (2009). Forest chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) remember the location of numerous fruit trees. Animal Cognition, 12, 797807.Google Scholar
Power, M. (1991). The Egalitarians: Human and Chimpanzee. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Pusey, A., Williams, J. & Goodall, J. (1997). The influence of dominance rank on the reproductive success of female chimpanzees. Science, 277, 828831.Google Scholar
Reynolds, V. (2005). The Chimpanzees of the Budongo Forest: Ecology, Behaviour and Conservation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ruvolo, M., Disotell, T., Allard, M., Brown, W. & Honeycutt, R. (1991). Resolution of the African hominoid trichotomy by use of a mitochondrial gene sequence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 88, 15701574.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruvolo, M., Pan, D., Zehr, S., Goldberg, T., Disotell, T. & von Dornum, M. (1994). Gene trees and hominoid phylogeny. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 91, 89008904.Google Scholar
Samuni, L., Preis, A., Deschner, T., Crockford, C. & Wittig, R. (2018). Reward of labor coordination and hunting success in wild chimpanzees. Communications Biology, 1, 138.Google Scholar
Sanz, C. & Morgan, D. (2007). Chimpanzee tool technology in the Goualougo Triangle, Republic of Congo. Journal of Human Evolution, 52, 420433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanz, C., Morgan, D. & Gulick, S. (2004). New insights into chimpanzees, tools, and termites from the Congo Basin. American Naturalist, 164(5), 567581.Google Scholar
Stearns, S. & Hoekstra, R. (2000). Evolution: An Introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sugiyama, Y. & Koman, J. (1979). Social structure and dynamics of wild chimpanzees at Bossou, Guinea. Primates, 20, 323339.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2009). Why We Co-operate. Boston: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Van Lawick-Goodall, J. (1968). Behaviour of free-living chimpanzees of the Gombe Stream area. Animal Behaviour Monograph, 1, 163311.Google Scholar
Vigilant, L. (1999). An evaluation of techniques for the extraction and amplification of DNA from naturally shed hairs. Biological Chemistry, 380, 13291331.Google Scholar
Vigilant, L., Hofreiter, M., Siedel, H. & Boesch, C. (2001). Paternity and relatedness in wild chimpanzee communities. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98, 12,89012,895.Google Scholar
Vigilant, L., Pennington, R., Harpending, H., Kocher, T. & Wilson, A. (1989). Mitochondrial DNA sequences in single hairs from a southern African population. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 86, 93509354.Google Scholar
Watts, D. & Mitani, J. (2002). Hunting behavior of chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda. International Journal of Primatology, 23, 128.Google Scholar
Watts, D. & Mitani, J. (2015). Hunting and prey switching by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) at Ngogo. International Journal of Primatology, 36, 728748.Google Scholar
Wrangham, R. (2006). Chimpanzees: The culture-zone concept becomes untidy. Current Biology, 16(16), R634.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×