Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T10:38:58.891Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

4 - System of tense–aspect marking 2: past time

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Lisa J. Green
Affiliation:
University of Massachusetts, Amherst
Get access

Summary

Nia: I BIN learning that. I been reading books at home before I come to school.

LJG: Oh!

Nia: At nighttime.

Introduction

On any given AAE feature list such as the one in Chapter 2, designed to capture prominent characteristics of child and adult AAE, there will be entries relating to ways of marking events in the past. The features on that list that refer to eventualities in the past are the following: Feature #3 completive done, Feature #8 preterite had, Feature #13 remote past been, and Feature #20 zero past tense. The focus of this chapter is on patterns and strategies that child AAE speakers use to talk about events in the past. The features pulled from the Craig and Washington list will be represented in the discussion about children’s interpretation and linguistic environments in which certain past markers are used.

The marker completive done, which I define as indicating that an eventuality is in its resultant state, that is, the state of having ended, is included in the feature list in Chapter 2, but it will only be briefly mentioned in this chapter because there is only one spontaneous resultant state done utterance in my corpus. In referring to the marker, I generally represent it as being unstressed by spelling it with a schwa (ə). That is my attempt to show that AAE speakers distinguish the pronunciation of the marker done from the past participle verb done. When speakers say I done (dən) done my homework to mean ‘I finished my homework,’ the first done is unstressed relative to the past participle done that follows it. Although dən was not used extensively by the developing AAE speakers in this study, comprehension tasks could help to determine whether they understand the marker as referring to events that have ended or are over. It is not clear whether the marker is acquired at an older age or whether the children did not have occasion to produce it in spontaneous speech. Terry (2005) explains that completive done is a perfect marker in AAE, so done left ‘already left’ has a perfect reading in AAE just as has left marks the present perfect in general English. It would be especially important to determine when children acquire the done construction because that information could provide insight into AAE-speaking children’s acquisition of the perfect. According to Craig and Washington (2006), completive done, “which indicates a recently completed action” (p. 120), is a feature of child AAE, but they do not give information about whether the marker is used by children in all age groups that the features in their list represent and whether its use also decreases as children go through school. However, the implication is that the marker occurs in the speech of even the youngest speakers (pre-kindergarten) represented in their data. They give a “recent past” definition for done, just one of its functions in adult AAE. It is not clear whether the description they give of done pertains specifically to the developing AAE speakers’ use of the marker.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×