Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-21T09:13:14.594Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

9 - Institutional Design and the Psychology of the Trial Judge

from II - Ontology and Epistemology

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 April 2021

Bartosz Brożek
Affiliation:
Jagiellonian University, Krakow
Jaap Hage
Affiliation:
Universiteit Maastricht, Netherlands
Nicole Vincent
Affiliation:
Macquarie University, Sydney
Get access

Summary

This chapter discusses the ways in which the psychology of judicial decision making may be intertwined with wider institutional design aspects. It is commonly recognized that decision-making processes are susceptible to various cognitive biases, and judicial decisions are no exception. Extending the focus from the individual trial judge to the characteristics of the court in which a judge operates, this chapter’s aim is to highlight the possible role of procedures and institutions in perpetuating, exacerbating or creating cognitive biases in judicial decisions.

These cognitive biases and heuristics are of particular interest as they can not only lead to transient effects on decisions but can also affect judgements in more systematic and prolonged ways. The expertise and repeat experience that characterize judges can lead to the creation of schemas and baselines which judges employ in individual cases. The institutional capacity of the courts and the applicable rules of procedure and evidence can foster different decision-making environments, thereby changing the schemas created and pushing judicial decisions in a particular direction.

This chapter reviews this connection between institutional design and the psychology of judging through three prominent examples. The first section discusses how due to context-dependent decision making, case assignment across courts can change the relative within-docket evaluation of particular cases, thus altering the substantive outcomes reached by the courts. The second section draws attention to the tension between the human tendency to retributivism and the rehabilitative goals set by problem-solving or treatment-oriented courts, and how this tendency may be responsible for some unintended consequences of specialized courts. The third section reviews the accumulating findings regarding the difficulty of disregarding irrelevant or inadmissible evidence, and discusses how these could inform the applicable rules of evidence governing trials.

Type
Chapter
Information
Law and Mind
A Survey of Law and the Cognitive Sciences
, pp. 193 - 206
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2021

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baron, J., & Ritov, I. (1993). Intuitions About Penalties and Compensation in the Context of Tort Law. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 7, 1733.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron, J., & Ritov, I. (2009). The Role of Probability of Detection in Judgments of Punishment. Journal of Legal Analysis 1(2), 553–90.Google Scholar
Bowers, J. (2008). Contraindicated Drug Courts. UCLA Law Review 55, 783835.Google Scholar
Carlsmith, K. M., Darley, J. M., & Robinson, P. H. (2002). Why Do We Punish? Deterrence and Just Deserts as Motives for Punishment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 83(2), 284–99.Google Scholar
Chen, D. L., Moskowitz, T. J., & Shue, K. (2016). Decision Making Under the Gambler’s Fallacy: Evidence from Asylum Judges, Loan Officers, and Baseball Umpires. The Quarterly Journal of Economics 131(3), 11811242.Google Scholar
Darley, J. M., & Alter, A. L. (2013). Behavioral Issues of Punishment, Retribution, and Deterrence. In Shafir, E. (ed.), The Behavioral Foundations of Public Policy. Princeton: Princeton University Press, pp. 173205.Google Scholar
Darley, J. M., Carlsmith, K. M., & Robinson, P. H. (2000). Incapacitation and Just Deserts as Motives for Punishment. Law and Human Behavior 24(6), 659–83.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, T., Rachlinski, J. J., & Wells, M. T. (2002). Reconciling Experimental Incoherence with Real-World Coherence in Punitive Damages. Stanford Law Review 54(6), 1239–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Englich, B., Mussweiler, T., & Strack, F. (2006). Playing Dice with Criminal Sentences: the Influence of Irrelevant Anchors on Experts’ Judicial Decision Making. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 32(2), 188200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eren, O., & Mocan, N. (2018). Emotional Judges and Unlucky Juveniles. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics 10(3), 171205.Google Scholar
Guthrie, C. (2003). Panacea or Pandora’s Box? The Cost of Options in Negotiation. Iowa Law Review 88(3), 601–54.Google Scholar
Guthrie, C., Rachlinski, J. J., & Wistrich, A. J. (2001) Inside the Judicial Mind. Cornell Law Review 86(4), 777830.Google Scholar
Guthrie, C., Rachlinski, J. J., & Wistrich, A. J. (2007). Blinking on the Bench: How Judges Decide Cases. Cornell Law Review 93(1), 144.Google Scholar
Guthrie, C., Rachlinski, J. J., & Wistrich, A. J. (2009). ‘The Hidden Judiciary’: an Empirical Examination of Executive Branch Justice. Duke Law Journal 58(7), 14771530.Google Scholar
Guttel, E. (2004). Overcorrection. Georgetown Law Journal 93(1), 241–84.Google Scholar
Hans, V. P., & Ermann, M. D. (1989). Responses to Corporate Versus Individual Wrongdoing. Law and Human Behavior 13(2), 151–66.Google Scholar
Hatvany, N., & Strack, F. (1980). The Impact of a Discredited Key Witness. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 10(6), 490509.Google Scholar
Himelein, M. J., Nietzel, M. T., & Dillehay, R. C. (1991). Effects of Prior Juror Experience on Jury Sentencing. Behavioral Sciences & the Law 9(1), 97106.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., Schkade, D., & Sunstein, C. (1998). Shared Outrage and Erratic Awards: the Psychology of Punitive Damages. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 16(1), 4986.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelman, M., Rottenstreich, Y., & Tversky, A. (1996). Context-Dependence in Legal Decision Making. The Journal of Legal Studies 25(2), 287318.Google Scholar
Kerwin, J., & Shaffer, D. R. (1994). Mock Jurors Versus Mock Juries: the Role of Deliberations in Reactions to Inadmissible Testimony. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 20(2), 153–62.Google Scholar
Landsman, S., & Rakos, R. F. (1994). A Preliminary Inquiry Into the Effect of Potentially Biasing Information on Judges and Jurors in Civil Litigation. Behavioral Sciences and the Law 12, 113–26.Google Scholar
Leibovitch, A. (2016). Relative Judgments. The Journal of Legal Studies, 45(2), 281330.Google Scholar
Leibovitch, A. (2017). Punishing on a Curve. Northwestern University Law Review 111(5), 1205–80.Google Scholar
Liu, Z., Klöhn, L., & Spamann, H. (2019). Precedent and Chinese Judges: an Experiment. American Journal of Comparative Law.Google Scholar
London, K., & Nunez, N. (2000). The Effect of Jury Deliberations on Jurors’ Propensity to Disregard Inadmissible Evidence. Journal of Applied Psychology 85(6), 932–9.Google Scholar
McLeod, A. M. (2012). Decarceration Courts: Possibilities and Perils of Shifting Criminal Law. Georgetown Law Journal 100(5), 15871674.Google Scholar
Ouss, A., & Peysakhovich, A. (2015). When Punishment Doesn’t Pay: Cold Glow and Decisions to Punish. The Journal of Law and Economics 58(3), 625–55.Google Scholar
Parducci, A. (1968). The Relativism of Absolute Judgments. Scientific American 219(6), 8493.Google Scholar
Pepitone, A., & DiNubile, M. (1976). Contrast Effects in Judgments of Crime Severity and the Punishment of Criminal Violators. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 33(4), 448–59.Google Scholar
Rachlinski, J. J., & Wistrich, A. J. (2017). Judging the Judiciary by the Numbers: Empirical Research on Judges. Annual Review of Law and Social Science, 13, 203–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rachlinski, J. J., Wistrich, A. J., & Guthrie, C. (2013). Altering Attention in Adjudication. UCLA Law Review, 60(6), 15861619.Google Scholar
Rachlinski, J. J., Wistrich, A. J., & Guthrie, C. (2015). Can Judges Make Reliable Numeric Judgments: Distorted Damages and Skewed Sentences. Indiana Law Journal 90(2), 695740.Google Scholar
Rachlinski, J. J., Guthrie, C., & Wistrich, A. J. (2006). Inside the Bankruptcy Judge’s Mind. Boston University Law Review 86(5), 1227–66.Google Scholar
Rachlinski, J. J., Guthrie, C., & Wistrich, A. J. (2007). Heuristics and Biases in Bankruptcy Judges. Journal of Institutional and Theoretical Economics 163(1), 167–86.Google Scholar
Rachlinski, J. J., Guthrie, C., & Wistrich, A. J. (2011). Probable Cause, Probability, and Hindsight. Journal of Empirical Legal Studies 8, 7298.Google Scholar
Rachlinski, J. J., & Jourden, F. (2003). The Cognitive Components of Punishment. Cornell Law Review 88(2), 457–85.Google Scholar
Robinson, P. H., & Darley, J. M. (1997). Utility of Desert. Northwestern University Law Review 91(2), 45399Google Scholar
Robinson, P. H., & Spellman, B. A. (2005). Sentencing Decisions: Matching the Decisionmaker to the Decision Nature. Columbia Law Review 105, 1124–61.Google Scholar
Rodríguez, G., & Blanco, S. (2016). Contrast Effect on the Perception of the Severity of a Criminal Offence. Anuario de Psicología Jurídica 26(1), 107–13.Google Scholar
Schauer, F. (2006). On the Supposed Jury–Dependence of Evidence Law. University of Pennsylvania Law Review 155(1), 165202.Google Scholar
Schauer, F., & Spellman, B. A. (2017). Analogy, Expertise, and Experience. University of Chicago Law Review 84(1), 249–68.Google Scholar
Schul, Y., & Goren, H. (1997). When Strong Evidence Has Less Impact than Weak Evidence: Bias, Adjustment, and Instructions to Ignore. Social Cognition 15(2), 133–55.Google Scholar
Sommers, S. R., & Kassin, S. M. (2001). On the Many Impacts of Inadmissible Testimony: Selective Compliance, Need for Cognition, and the Overcorrection Bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 27(10), 1368–77.Google Scholar
Spamann, H., & Klöhn, L. (2016). Justice Is Less Blind, and Less Legalistic, than We Thought: Evidence from an Experiment with Real Judges. The Journal of Legal Studies 45(2), 255–80.Google Scholar
Spellman, B. A. (2007). On the Supposed Expertise of Judges in Evaluating Evidence. University of Pennsylvania Law Review 156(1), 19.Google Scholar
Spellman, B. A., & Schauer, F. (2014). Law and Social Cognition. In Carlston, D. E. (ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Social Cognition. Oxford University Press, pp. 829–50.Google Scholar
Steblay, N., Hosch, H. M., Culhane, S. E., & McWethy, A. (2006). The Impact on Juror Verdicts of Judicial Instruction to Disregard Inadmissible Evidence: A Meta-Analysis. Law and Human Behavior 30(4), 469–92.Google Scholar
Sunstein, C. R., Kahneman, D., & Schkade, D. (1998). Assessing Punitive Damages (with Notes on Cognition and Valuation in Law). Yale Law Journal 107(7), 20712153.Google Scholar
Sunstein, C. R., Kahneman, D., Schkade, D., & Ritov, I. (2002). Predictably Incoherent Judgments. Stanford Law Review 54(6), 11531215.Google Scholar
Sunstein, C. R., Schkade, D., & Kahneman, D. (2000). Do People Want Optimal Deterrence? The Journal of Legal Studies 29(1), 237–53.Google Scholar
Tyler, T. R., & Darley, J. M. (2000). Building Law-Abiding Society: Taking Public Views About Morality and the Legitimacy of Legal Authorities into Account When Formulating Substantive Law. Hofstra Law Review 28(3), 707–40.Google Scholar
Wistrich, A. J., Guthrie, C., & Rachlinski, J. J. (2005). Can Judges Ignore Inadmissible Information? The Difficulty of Deliberately Disregarding. University of Pennsylvania Law Review 153(4), 12511345.Google Scholar
Zamir, E., & Medina, B. (2010). Law, Economics, and Morality. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Zamir, E., & Teichman, D. (2018). Behavioral Law and Economics. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Zimring, F. E. (1993). Drug Treatment as Criminal Sanction. University of Colorado Law Review 64(3), 809–26.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×