Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-5c6d5d7d68-wtssw Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-08-24T19:21:05.993Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 July 2013

Patrick Bateson
Affiliation:
University of Cambridge
Paul Martin
Affiliation:
Wolfson College, Cambridge
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2013

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alves, J., Marques, M. J., Saur, I. & Marques, P. (2007). Creativity and innovation through multidisciplinary and multisectoral cooperation. Creativity and Innovation Management, 16, 27–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amabile, T., Conti, R., Coon, H., Lazenby, J. & Herron, M. (1996). Assessing the work environment for creativity. Academy of Management Journal, 39, 1154–84.Google Scholar
Amundson, R. (2005). The changing role of the embryo in evolutionary thought: roots of Evo-Devo. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anning, A. (2010). Play and legislated curriculum. In Moyles, J. (Ed.), The excellence of play. 3rd edition (pp. 19–33). Maidenhead, UK: Open University Press.Google Scholar
Artaud, A. (1976). The peyote dance. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.Google Scholar
Auersperg, A. M. I., von Bayern, A. M. P., Gajdon, G. K., Huber, L. & Kacelnik, A. (2011). Flexibility in problem solving and tool use of kea and New Caledonian crows in a multi-access box paradigm. PLoS ONE, 6, e20231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Auger, A. P. & Olesen, K. M. (2009). Brain sex differences and the organisation of juvenile social play behaviour. Journal of Neuroendocrinology, 21, 519–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Avital, E. & Jablonka, E. (2000). Animal traditions: behavioural inheritance in evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baas, M., De Dreu, C. K. W. & Nijstad, B. A. (2008). A meta-analysis of 25 years of mood–creativity research: hedonic tone, activation, or regulatory focus?Psychological Bulletin, 134, 779–806.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baird, B., Smallwood, J. & Schooler, J. W. (2011). Back to the future: autobiographical planning and the functionality of mind-wandering. Consciousness & Cognition, 20, 1604–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baldwin, J. D. & Baldwin, J. I. (1977). The role of learning phenomena in the ontogeny of exploration and play. In Chevalier-Skolnikoff, S. & Poirier, F. E. (Eds.), Primate bio-social development (pp. 343–406). New York: Garland.Google Scholar
Baldwin, J. M. (1896). A new factor in evolution. American Naturalist, 30, 441–51, 536–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baldwin, J. M. (1902). Development and evolution. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Barnett, L. A. & Kleiber, D. A. (1984). Playfulness and the early play environment. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 144, 153–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, D. (2001). The committee of sleep: how artists, scientists, and athletes use dreams for creative problem-solving – and how you can too. New York: Crown.Google Scholar
Barrett, L., Dunbar, R. M. & Dunbar, P. (1992). Environmental influences on play behaviour in immature gelada baboons. Animal Behaviour, 44, 111–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, P. & Bateson, P. (1978). The development of play in cats. Behaviour, 66, l06–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartholomew, G. (1982). Scientific innovation and creativity: a zoologist’s point of view. American Zoologist, 22, 227–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bateson, P. (1981). Discontinuities in development and changes in the organization of play in cats. In Immelmann, K., Barlow, G. W., Petrinovich, L. & Main, M. (Eds.), Behavioral development (pp. 28l–95). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bateson, P. (2000a). Behavioural development in the cat. In Turner, D. C. & Bateson, P. (Eds.), The domestic cat: the biology of its behaviour. 2nd edition (pp. 10–22). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bateson, P. (2000b). What must be known in order to understand imprinting? In Heyes, C. & Huber, L. (Eds.), The evolution of cognition (pp. 85–102). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bateson, P. (2004). The active role of behaviour in evolution. Biology and Philosophy, 19, 283–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bateson, P. (2006). The adaptability driver: links between behaviour and evolution. Biological Theory: Integrating Development, Evolution and Cognition, 1, 342–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bateson, P. (2011). Theories of play. In Pellegrini, A. D. (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of the development of play (pp. 41–47). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bateson, P., Barker, D., Clutton-Brock, T., Deb, D., D’Udine, B., Foley, R. A., et al. (2004). Developmental plasticity and human health. Nature, 430, 419–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bateson, P. & Gluckman, P. (2011). Plasticity, robustness, development and evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bateson, P. & Martin, P. (1999). Design for a life: how behaviour develops. London: Jonathan Cape.Google Scholar
Bateson, P., Martin, P. & Young, M. (1981). Effects of interrupting cat mothers’ lactation with bromocriptine on the subsequent play of their kittens. Physiology and Behavior, 27, 841–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bateson, P., Mendl, M. & Feaver, J. (1990). Play in the domestic cat is enhanced by rationing the mother during lactation. Animal Behaviour, 40, 514–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bateson, P. & Young, M. (1981). Separation from mother and the development of play in cats. Animal Behaviour, 29, 173–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Batey, M. & Furnham, A. (2006). Creativity, intelligence, and personality: a critical review of the scattered literature. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 132, 355–429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Batey, M. & Furnham, A. (2008). The relationship between measures of creativity and schizotypy. Personality and Individual Differences, 45, 816–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, E. B. & Smuts, B. K. (2007). Cooperation and competition during dyadic play in domestic dogs, Canis familiaris. Animal Behaviour, 73, 489–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bekoff, M. (1976). Animal play: problems and perspectives. Perspectives in Ethology, 2, 165–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bekoff, M. (2010). The animal manifesto: six reasons for expanding our compassion footprint. Novato, CA: New World Library.Google Scholar
Bekoff, M. & Byers, J. A. (1998). Animal play: evolutionary, comparative, and ecological perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bell, H. C., McCaffrey, D. R., Forgie, M. L., Kolb, B. & Pellis, S. M. (2009). The role of the medial prefrontal cortex in the play fighting of rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 123, 1158–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bell, H. C., Pellis, S. M. & Kolb, B. (2010). Juvenile peer play experience and the development of the orbitofrontal and medial prefrontal cortices. Behavioural Brain Research, 207, 7–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belton, T. (2001). Television and imagination: an investigation of the medium’s influence on children’s story-making. Media Culture & Society, 23, 799–820.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benfey, O. T. (1958). August Kekulé and the birth of the structural theory of organic chemistry in 1858. Journal of Chemical Education, 35, 21–3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bennett, M. P., Zeller, J. M., Rosenberg, L. & McCann, J. (2003). The effect of mirthful laughter on stress and natural killer cell activity. Alternative Therapies, Health & Medicine, 9, 38–45.Google ScholarPubMed
Bergen, D. (2009). Play as the learning medium for future scientists, mathematicians and engineers. American Journal of Play, 1, 413–28.Google Scholar
Beveridge, A. & Yorston, G. (1999). I drink, therefore I am: alcohol and creativity. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 92, 646–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Birch, H. G. (1945). The relation of previous experience to insightful problem-solving. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 38, 367–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bird, C. D. & Emery, N. J. (2009). Rooks use stones to raise the water level to reach a floating worm. Current Biology, 19, 1410–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blagrove, M. & Hartnell, S. J. (2000). Lucid dreaming: associations with internal locus of control, need for cognition and creativity. Personality & Individual Differences, 28, 41–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blatchford, P. & Baines, E. (2006). A follow up national survey of breaktimes in primary and secondary schools. London: The Nuffield Foundation.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. & O’Reilly, A. W. (1993). The role of play in the development of thought. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Brodzinsky, D. M. & Rubien, J. (1976). Humor production as a function of sex of subject, creativity, and cartoon content. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 44, 597–600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bronson, P. & Merryman, A. (2010). The creativity crisis. Newsweek, 10 July 2010.
Brown, S. (2009). Play. London: Penguin.Google ScholarPubMed
Brownlee, A. (1954). Play in domestic cattle: an analysis of its nature. British Veterinary Journal, 110, 48–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. S., Jolly, A. & Sylva, K. (1976). Play: its role in development and evolution. Harmondsworth, UK: Penguin.Google Scholar
Burdette, H. L. & Whitaker, R. C. (2005). Resurrecting free play in young children. Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine, 159, 46–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burghardt, G. M. (2005). The genesis of animal play: testing the limits. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Burghardt, G. M. (in press). The origins, evolution, and interconnections of play and ritual: setting the stage. In Renfrew, C., Boyd, M. & Morley, I. (Eds.), Play, ritual and belief, in animals and early human societies (pp. 000–000). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bus, R. R., Sun, W. & Oppenheim, R. W. (2006). Adaptive roles of programmed cell death during nervous system development. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 29, 1–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Busch, H. & Silver, B. (1994). Why cats paint: a theory of feline aesthetics. London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson.Google Scholar
Cameron, E. Z., Linklater, W. L., Stafford, K. J. & Minot, E. O. (2008). Maternal investment results in better foal condition through increased play behaviour in horses. Animal Behaviour, 76, 1511–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, D. T. (1960). Blind variation and selective retention in creative thought as in other knowledge processes. Psychological Review, 67, 380–400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caro, T. M. (1980). Predatory behaviour in domestic cat mothers. Behaviour, 74, 128–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caro, T. M. (1995). Short-term costs and correlates of play in cheetahs. Animal Behaviour, 49, 333–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caro, T. M. & Bateson, P. (1986). Organisation and ontogeny of alternative tactics. Animal Behaviour, 34, 1483–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caro, T. M., Roper, R., Young, M. & Dank, G. R. (1979). Inter-observer reliability. Behaviour, 69, 303–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carson, S. H., Peterson, J. B. & Higgins, D. M. (2003). Decreased latent inhibition is associated with increased creative achievement in high-functioning individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 499–506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chalmers, N. R. (1980). The ontogeny of play in feral olive baboons (Papio anubis). Animal Behaviour, 28, 570–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheke, L. D., Bird, C. D. & Clayton, N. S. (2011). Tool-use and instrumental learning in the Eurasian jay (Garrulus glandarius). Animal Cognition, 14, 441–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clements, R. (2004). An investigation of the status of outdoor play. Contemporary Issues in Early Childhood, 5, 68–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colby, R. S. & Colby, R. (2008). A pedagogy of play: integrating computer games into the writing classroom. Computers and Composition, 25, 300–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Copeland, K. A.Sherman, S. N., Kendeigh, C. K., Kalkwarf, H. J. & Saelens, B. E. (2012). Societal values and policies may curtail preschool children’s physical activity in child care centers. Pediatrics, 129, 265–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). Creativity: flow and the psychology of discovery and invention. New York: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1877). A biographical sketch of an infant. Mind, 2, 285–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, M. A. (2009). Understanding the relationship between mood and creativity. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 108, 25–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Oliveira, C. R., Ruiz-Miranda, C. R., Kleiman, D. G. & Beck, B. B. (2003). Play behavior in juvenile golden lion tamarins (Callitrichidae: Primates): organization in relation to costs. Ethology, 109, 593–612.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deag, J. M., Lawrence, C. E. & Manning, A. (1987). The consequences of differences in litter size for the nursing cat and her kittens. Journal of Zoology, 213, 153–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deci, E. L., Koestner, R. & Ryan, R. M. (1999). A meta-analytic review of experiments examining the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 627–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, J. & Bond, A. B. (2003). A comparative analysis of social play in birds. Behaviour, 140, 899–924.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diehl, M. & Stroebe, W. (1991). Productivity loss in idea-generating groups: tracking down the blocking effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 392–403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dobkin de Rios, M. & Janiger, O. (2003). LSD, spirituality and the creative process. Rochester, VT: Park Street Press.Google Scholar
Domino, G. (1976). Primary process thinking in dream reports as related to creative achievement. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 44, 929–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dudink, S., Simonse, H., Marks, I., de Jonge, F. H. & Spruijt, B. M. (2006). Announcing the arrival of enrichment increases play behaviour and reduces weaning-stress-induced behaviours of piglets directly after weaning. Applied Animal Behaviour Sciences, 101, 86–101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Egan, J. (1976). Object-play in cats. In Bruner, J. S., Jolly, A. & Sylva, K. (Eds.), Play: its role in development and evolution (pp. 161–5). Harmondsworth, UK: Penguin.Google Scholar
Einon, D. & Morgan, M. (1976). Habituation of object contact in socially-reared and isolated rats (Rattus norvegicus). Animal Behaviour, 24, 415–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Einon, D. & Potegal, M. (1991). Enhanced defense in adult-rats deprived of playfighting experience as juveniles. Aggressive Behavior, 17, 27–40.3.0.CO;2-B>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elkind, D. (2008). The power of play: how spontaneous, imaginative activities lead to happier, healthier children. Cambridge, MA: De Capo Lifelong.Google Scholar
Epstein, R., Kirshnit, C. E., Lanza, R. P. & Rubin, L. C. (1984). ‘Insight’ in the pigeon: antecedents and determinants of an intelligent performance. Nature, 308, 61–2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erikson, E. H. (1963). Childhood and society. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Escher, M. C. (1989). Escher on Escher: exploring the infinite. New York: Abrams.Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (1995). Genius: the natural history of creativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fagen, R. (1981). Animal play behavior. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fagen, R. & Fagen, J. (2004). Juvenile survival and benefits of play behaviour in brown bears, Ursus arctos. Evolutionary Ecology Research, 6, 89–102.Google Scholar
Fagen, R. & Fagen, J. (2009). Play behaviour and multi-year juvenile survival in free-ranging brown bears, Ursus arctos. Evolutionary Ecology Research, 11, 1–15.Google Scholar
Fagen, R. M. (1974). Selective and evolutionary aspects of animal play. American Naturalist, 108, 850–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fagerberg, J. (2005). Innovation: a guide to the literature. In Fagerberg, J., Mowery, D. C. & Nelson, R. R. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of innovation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fagerberg, J., Mowery, D. C. & Nelson, R. R. (2005). The Oxford handbook of innovation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fedigan, L. (1972). Social and solitary play in a colony of vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops). Primates, 13, 347–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scientific and artistic creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 290–309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fenwick, P. B. C., Schatzman, M., Worsley, A., Adams, J., Stone, S. & Baker, A. (1984). Lucid dreaming: correspondence between dreamed and actual events in one subject during REM sleep. Biological Psychology, 18, 243–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernald, R. D. (2000). Evolution of eyes. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 10, 444–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fertl, D. & Wilson, B. (1997). Bubble use during prey capture by a lone bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus). Aquatic Mammals, 23, 113–14.Google Scholar
Feynman, R. (1985). Surely you’re joking, Mr. Feynman!New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Fisher, E. P. (1992). The impact of play on development: a metaanalysis. Play & Culture, 5, 159–81.Google Scholar
Flynn, J. R. (1987). Massive IQ gains in 14 nations: what IQ tests really measure. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 171–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forabosco, G. (1992). Cognitive aspects of the humor process: the concept of incongruity. Humor, 5, 45–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frith, C. B. & Frith, D. W. (2004). Bowerbirds. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Furedi, F. (2001). Paranoid parenting. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
Furnham, F. & Bachtiar, V. (2008). Personality and intelligence as predictors of creativity. Personality and Individual Differences, 45, 613–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garaigordobil, M. (2006). Intervention in creativity with children aged 10 and 11 years: impact of a play program on verbal and graphic–figural creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 18, 329–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geist, V. (1978). On weapons, combat, and ecology. In Krames, L., Pliner, P. & Alloway, T. (Eds.), Aggression, dominance and individual spacing (pp. 1–30). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
George, J. M. & Zhou, J. (2002). Understanding when bad moods foster creativity and good ones don’t: the role of context and clarity of feelings. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 687–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gervais, M. & Wilson, D. S. (2005). The evolution and functions of laughter and humor: a synthetic approach. Quarterly Review of Biology, 80, 395–430.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilbert, S. F. (2005). Mechanisms for the environmental regulation of gene expression: ecological aspects of animal development. Journal of Biosciences, 30, 65–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gleave, J. (2009). Children’s time to play: a literature review. London: Play England.Google Scholar
Gluckman, P. & Hanson, M. (2006). Mismatch: why our world no longer fits our bodies. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gomendio, M. (1988). The development of different types of play in gazelles: implications for the nature and functions of play. Animal Behaviour, 36, 825–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gomendio, M., Cassinello, J., Smith, M. W. & Bateson, P. (1995). Maternal state affects intestinal changes of rat pups at weaning. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 37, 71–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graves, P. L. (1976). Nutrition, infant behavior, and maternal characteristics: a pilot study in West Bengal, India. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 29, 305–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, P. (2009). Play as a foundation for hunter-gatherer social existence. American Journal of Play, 1, 476–522.Google Scholar
Gray, P. (2011). The decline of play and the rise of psychopathology in children and adolescents. American Journal of Play, 3, 443–63.Google Scholar
Green, R. E., Krause, J., Briggs, A. W., Maricic, T., Stenzel, U., Kircher, M., et al. (2010). A draft sequence of the Neandertal genome. Science, 328, 710–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Groos, K. (1898). The play of animals. New York: Appleton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guilford, J. P. (1956). Structure of intellect. Psychological Bulletin, 53, 267–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gustafson, R. & Kallmen, H. (1989). The effect of alcohol intoxication on primary and secondary processes in male social drinkers. British Journal of Addiction, 84, 1507–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harcourt, R. (1991a). The development of play in the South American fur seal. Ethology, 88, 191–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harcourt, R. (1991b). Survivorship costs of play in the South American fur seal. Animal Behaviour, 42, 509–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hargadon, A. & Sutton, R. I. (2000). Building an innovation factory. Harvard Business Review, 78, 157–66.Google ScholarPubMed
Harman, W. W., McKim, R. H. & Mogar, R. E. (1966). Psychedelic agents in creative problem-solving: a pilot study. Psychological Reports, 19, 211–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hart, B. L. & Miller, M. F. (1985). Behavioral profiles of dog breeds. Journal of the Veterinary Medicine Association, 186, 1175–80.Google ScholarPubMed
Hausberger, M., Fureix, C., Bourjade, M., Wessel-Robert, S. & Richard-Yris, M.-A. (2012). On the significance of adult play: what does social play tell us about adult horse welfare?Naturwissenschaften, 99, 291–302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hearne, K. M. T. (1990). The dream machine. Wellingborough, UK: Aquarian Press.Google Scholar
Heilman, K. M., Nadeau, S. E. & Beversdorf, D. O. (2003). Creative innovation: possible brain mechanisms. Neurocase, 9, 369–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Held, S. D. E., & Spinka, M. (2011). Animal play and animal welfare. Animal Behaviour, 81, 891–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henig, R. M. (2008). Taking play seriously. New York Times Magazine, February 27, 2008.
Herron, R. E. & Sutton-Smith, B. E. (1971). Child’s play. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Hinde, R. A. & Fisher, J. (1951). Further observations on the opening of milk bottles by birds. British Birds, 44, 393–6.Google Scholar
Hinton, G. E. & Nowlan, S. J. (1987). How learning can guide evolution. Complex Systems 1, 495–502.Google Scholar
Holzhaider, J. C., Hunt, G. R. & Gray, R. D. (2010). The development of pandanus tool manufacture in wild New Caledonian crows. Behaviour, 147, 553–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoof, J. A. R. A. M. (1973). A structural analysis of the social behaviour of a semi-captive group of chimpanzees. In von Cranach, M. & Vine, I. (Eds.), Social communication and movement: studies of interaction and expression in man and chimpanzee (pp. 75–162). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Huber, L., Rechberger, S. & Taborsky, M. (2001). Social learning affects object exploration and manipulation in keas, Nestor notabilis. Animal Behaviour, 62, 945–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huizinga, J. (1955). Homo ludens. Boston, MA: Beacon Press.Google Scholar
Humke, C. & Schaefer, C. E. (1996). Sense of humor and creativity. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 82, 544–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphrey, N. (1983). Consciousness regained: chapters in the development of mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Humphrey, N. (1986). The inner eye. London: Faber & Faber.Google Scholar
Humphrey, N. (1992). A history of the mind. London: Chatto & Windus.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphrey, N. (2000). Dreaming as play. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 23, 953.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphrey, N. (2007). Dreaming to learn. In Margulis, L. & Punset, E. (Eds.), Mind, life and universe: conversations with great scientists of our time (pp. 140–8). White River Junction, VT: Chelsea Green Publishing.Google Scholar
Humphreys, A. P. & Einon, D. F. (1981). Play as a reinforcer for maze-learning in juvenile rats. Animal Behaviour, 29, 259–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunt, G. R. (1996). Manufacture and use of hook-tools by New Caledonian crows. Nature 379, 249–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hurley, M. M., Dennett, D. C. & Adams, R. B. (2011). Inside jokes: using humor to reverse-engineer the mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Isen, A. M. & Reeve, J. (2005). The influence of positive affect on intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: facilitating enjoyment of play, responsible work behavior, and self-control. Motivation and Emotion, 29, 297–325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jablonka, E. & Raz, G. (2009). Transgenerational epigenetic inheritance: prevalence, mechanisms, and implications for the study of heredity and evolution. Quarterly Review of Biology, 84, 131–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, W. (1902). The varieties of religious experience. London: Longmans, Green.Google Scholar
Jarosz, A. F., Colflesh, G. J. H. & Wiley, J. (2012). Uncorking the muse: alcohol intoxication facilitates creative problem solving. Consciousness and Cognition, 31, 487–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jensen, M. B. (1999). Effects of confinement on rebounds of locomotor behaviour of calves and heifers, and the spatial preferences of calves. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 62, 43–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, H. (1989). The story of wine. London: Mitchell Beazley.Google Scholar
Johnson, S. (2010). Where good ideas come from. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
Jones, D. (2012). The aha! moment: a scientist’s take on creativity. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Judson, H. F. (1980). The search for solutions. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Kahlenberg, S. M. & Wrangham, R. W. (2010). Sex differences in chimpanzees’ use of sticks as play objects resemble those of children. Current Biology, 20, R1067–R1068.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. London: Allen Lane.Google Scholar
Kangas, M. (2010). Creative and playful learning: learning through game co-creation and games in a playful learning environment. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 5, 1–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kanter, R. M. (2006). Innovation: the classic traps. Harvard Business Review, 84, 73–83.Google ScholarPubMed
Kanter, R. M. (2009). SuperCorp. London: Profile.Google Scholar
Kauffman, S. A. (2000). Investigations. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kawecki, T. J. (2010). Evolutionary ecology of learning: insights from fruit flies. Population Ecology, 52, 15–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelley, T. (2006). The ten faces of innovation: IDEO’s strategies for beating the Devil’s advocate & driving creativity throughout your organization. London: Profile Books.Google Scholar
Kendall, R. L., Coe, R. L. & Laland, K. N. (2005). Age differences in neophilia, exploration, and innovation in family groups of callitrichid monkeys. American Journal of Primatology, 66, 167–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenward, B., Rutz, C., Weir, A. A. S. & Kacelnik, A. (2006). Development of tool use in New Caledonian crows: inherited action patterns and social influences. Animal Behaviour, 72, 1329–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, K. H. (2005). Learning from each other: creativity in East Asian and American education. Creativity Research Journal, 17, 337–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, K. H. (2011). The creativity crisis: the decrease in creative thinking scores on the Torrance tests of creative thinking. Creativity Research Journal, 23, 285–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, Z. (2012). The Goldilocks network. New Scientist, 26 May 2012, 37–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kipper, S. & Todt, D. (2005). The sound of laughter: recent concepts and findings in research into laughter vocalizations. In Garfitt, T., McMorran, E. & Taylor, J. (Eds.), The anatomy of laughter (pp. 24–33). London: Legenda, Modern Humanities Research Association and Maney Publishing.Google Scholar
Koestler, A. (1964). The act of creation. London: Hutchison.Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1925). The mentality of apes. London: Paul, Trench & Trubner.Google Scholar
Koski-Jännes, A. (1985). Alcohol and literary creativity: the Finnish experience. Journal of Creative Behavior, 19, 120–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuczaj, S. A., Makecha, R., Trone, M., Paulos, R. D. & Ramos, J. A. A. (2006). Role of peers in cultural innovation and cultural transmission: evidence from the play of dolphin calves. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 19, 223–40.Google Scholar
Kuhn, T. S. (1962). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kummer, H. & Goodall, J. (1985). Conditions of innovative behaviour in primates. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 308, 203–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kyaga, S., Landén, M., Boman, M., Hultman, C. M., Långström, N. & Lichtenstein, P. (2012). Mental illness, suicide and creativity: 40-year prospective total population study. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 47, 83–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LaBerge, S. P., Nagel, L. E., Dement, W. C. & Zarcone, V. P. (1981). Lucid dreaming verified by volitional communication during REM-sleep. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 52, 727–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laland, K. N. & Hoppitt, W. (2003). Do animals have culture?Evolutionary Anthropology, 12, 150–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, P. C. (1984). Ecological constraints on the social development of vervet monkeys. Behaviour, 91, 245–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lefebvre, L., Whittle, P., Lascaris, E. & Finkelstein, A. (1997). Feeding, innovation and forebrain size in birds. Animal Behaviour, 53, 549–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lehrer, J. (2012). Imagine: how creativity works. Edinburgh: Canongate.Google Scholar
Lieberman, J. N. (1977). Playfulness: its relationship to imagination and creativity. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Lillard, A. S., Lerner, M. D., Hopkins, E. J., Dore, R. A., Smith, E. D. & Palmquist, C. M. (2013). The impact of pretend play on children’s development: a review of the evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 1–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Livingston, G. & Levin, R. (1991). The effects of dream length on the relationship between primary process in dreams and creativity. Dreaming, 1, 301–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lloyd Morgan, C. (1896). On modification and variation. Science, 4, 733–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1935). Der Kumpan in der Umwelt des Vogels. Journal für Ornithologie, 83, 137–213, 289–413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ludwig, A. M. (1990). Alcohol input and creative output. British Journal of Addiction, 85, 953–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyubomirsky, S., King, L. & Diener, E. (2005). The benefits of frequent positive affect: does happiness lead to success?Psychological Bulletin, 131, 803–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Madden, J. R. (2007). Innovation in sexual display. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 417–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Magin, C. M. (1988). Behavioural development in two species of hyrax living in the Serengeti National Park, Tanzania. PhD dissertation, University of Cambridge.
Mann, J., Stanton, M. A., Patterson, E. M., Bienenstock, E. J. & Singh, L. O. (2012). Social networks reveal cultural behaviour in tool-using dolphins. Nature Communications, 3, 980.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manning, A. & Dawkins, M. S. (2012). An introduction to animal behaviour. Sixth edition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marler, P. & Slabberkoorn, H. (2004). Nature’s music: the science of birdsong. San Diego, CA: Elsevier/Academic Press.Google Scholar
Marten, K., Shariff, K., Psarokos, S. & White, D. J. (1996). Ring bubbles of dolphins. Scientific American, August 1996, 83–7.Google ScholarPubMed
Martin, P. (1984a). The (four) whys and wherefores of play in cats: a review of functional, evolutionary, developmental and causal issues. In Smith, P. K. (Ed.), Play in animals and humans (pp. 71–94). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Martin, P. (1984b). The time and energy costs of play behaviour in the cat. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 64, 298–312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, P. (1997). The sickening mind: brain, behaviour, immunity and disease. London: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Martin, P. (2002). Counting sheep: the science and pleasures of sleep and dreams. London: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Martin, P. (2005). Making happy people: the nature of happiness and its origins in childhood. London: Fourth Estate.Google Scholar
Martin, P. (2008). Sex, drugs & chocolate: the science of pleasure. London: Fourth Estate.Google Scholar
Martin, P. & Bateson, P. (1985). The influence of experimentally manipulating a component of weaning on the development of play in domestic cats. Animal Behaviour, 33, 511–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, P. & Bateson, P. (2007). Measuring behaviour: an introductory guide. 3rd edition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, P. & Caro, T. M. (1985). On the functions of play and its role in behavioral development. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 15, 59–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mason, M. F., Norton, M. I., Van Horn, J. D., Wegner, D. M., Grafton, S. T. & Macrae, C. N. (2007). Wandering minds: the default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science, 315, 393–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maurois, A. (1959). The life of Alexander Fleming, discoverer of penicillin. London: Jonathan Cape.Google Scholar
McClintock, B. (1950). The origin and behavior of mutable loci in maize. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 36, 344–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGee, P. E. (1979). Humor: its origin and development. San Francisco, CA: Freeman.Google Scholar
McGuire, M. T., Raleigh, M. J. & Pollack, D. B. (1994). Personality features in vervet monkeys: the effect of sex age, social status, and group composition. American Journal of Primatology, 33, 1–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mckeown, M. (2008). The truth about innovation. Harlow, UK: Pearson Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
McLean, L. D. (2005). Implications for human resource development organizational culture’s influence on creativity and innovation: a review of the literature and implications for human resource development. Advances in Developing Human Resources, 7, 226–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meaney, M. J. & Stewart, J. (1985). Sex differences in social play: the socialization of sex roles. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 15, 1–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mednick, S. A. (1968). The Remote Associates Test. Journal of Creative Behavior, 2, 213–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mehta, R. & Zhu, R. (2009). Blue or red? Exploring the effect of color on cognitive task performances. Science, 323, 1226–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mendl, M. (1988). The effects of litter-size variation on the development of play-behaviour in the domestic cat: litters of one and two. Animal Behaviour, 36, 20–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mendl, M. & Harcourt, R. (1988). Individuality in the domestic cat. In Turner, D. C. & Bateson, P. (Eds.), The domestic cat: the biology of its behaviour (pp. 41–54). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Milteer, R. M. & Ginsburg, K. R. (2012). The importance of play in promoting healthy child development and maintaining strong parent–child bond: focus on children in poverty. Pediatrics, 129, e204–e213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitchell, R. W. (2007). Pretense in animals: the continuing relevance of children’s pretense. In Göncü, A. & Gaskins, S. (Eds.), Play and development: evolutionary, sociocultural and functional perspectives (pp. 51–75). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Moore, M. & Rudd, S. W. (2008). Follow-up of a pretend play intervention: effects on play, creativity, and emotional processes in children. Creativity Research Journal, 20, 427–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, D. (1962). The biology of art: a study of the picture-making behaviour of the great apes and its relationship to human art. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Morrison, R. (2004). Informal relationships in the workplace: associations with job satisfaction, organisational commitment and turnover intentions. New Zealand Journal of Psychology, 33, 114–28.Google Scholar
Moultrie, J., Nilsson, M., Dissel, M., Haner, U.-E., Janssen, S. & van der Lugt, R. (2007). Innovation spaces: towards a framework for understanding the role of the physical environment in innovation. Creativity and Innovation Management, 16, 53–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moyles, J. (2010). Afterword. In Moyles, J. (Ed.), The excellence of play. 3rd edition (pp. 291–5). Maidenhead, UK: Open University Press.Google Scholar
Mullis, K. (1998). Dancing naked in the mind field. London: Vintage Books.Google Scholar
Murdock, M. C. & Ganim, R. M. (1993). Creativity and humor: integration and incongruity. Journal of Creative Behavior, 27, 57–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murrin, K. & Martin, P. (2004). What worries parents: the most common concerns of parents explored and explained. London: Vermilion.Google Scholar
Nemeth, C. J., Personnaz, B., Personnaz, M. & Goncalo, J. A. (2004). The liberating role of conflict in group creativity: a study in two countries. European Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 365–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nettle, D. (2002). Strong imagination: madness, creativity, and human nature. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nettle, D. (2006). Schizotypy and mental health amongst poets, visual artists, and mathematicians. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 876–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nettle, D. (2007). Personality: what makes you the way you are?Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nettle, D. & Clegg, H. (2006). Schizotypy, creativity and mating success in humans. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 273, 611–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norlander, T. & Gustafson, R. (1998). Effects of alcohol on a divergent figural fluency test during the illumination phase of the creative process. Creativity Research Journal, 11, 265–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osborn, H. F. (1896). Ontogenic and phylogenic variation. Science, 4, 786–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osborn, A. (1952). Your creative power: how to use your imagination. New York: Scribners.Google Scholar
Otto, B. K. (2001). Fools are everywhere: the court jester around the world. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Paenke, I., Kawecki, T. J. & Sendhoff, B. (2009). The influence of learning on evolution: a mathematical framework. Artificial Life, 15, 227–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palmer, S. (2006). Toxic childhood: how the modern world is damaging our children and what we can do about it. London: Orion.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (2007). Neuroevolutionary sources of laughter and social joy: modeling primal human laughter in laboratory rats. Behavioural Brain Research, 182, 231–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Panksepp, J. (2011). Cross-species affective neuroscience decoding of the primal affective experiences of humans and related animals. PLoS ONE, 6, e21236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pellegrini, A. D. (1996). The future of play theory. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Pellegrini, A. D. (2009). The role of play in human development. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pellegrini, A. D. (2011). The Oxford handbook of the development of play. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pellegrini, A. D. & Smith, P. K. (2005). The nature of play: great apes and humans. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Pellis, S. & Pellis, V. (2009). The playful brain. Oxford: Oneworld.Google Scholar
Penrose, L. S. & Penrose, R. (1958). Impossible objects: a special type of visual illusion. British Journal of Psychology, 49, 31–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International Universities Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams, and imagination in children. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Pigliucci, M. & Müller, G. B. (2010). Evolution: the extended synthesis. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plowden, B. (1967). Children and their primary schools. London: Central Advisory Council for Education.Google Scholar
POST (2000). Early years learning. London: Parliamentary Office of Science and Technology.Google Scholar
Power, T. G. (2000). Play and exploration in children and animals. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Provine, R. R. (2000). Laughter: a scientific investigation. London: Faber & Faber.Google Scholar
Provine, R. R. (2012). Curious behavior: yawning, laughing, hiccupping, and beyond. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pruitt, J. N., Burghardt, G. M. & Riechert, S. E. (2012). Non-conceptive sexual behavior in spiders: a form of play associated with body condition, personality type, and male intrasexual selection. Ethology, 118, 33–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pruitt, J. N. & Riechert, S. E. (2011). Nonconceptive sexual experience diminishes individuals’ latency to mate and increases maternal investment. Animal Behaviour, 81, 789–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pryor, K., Haag, R. & O’Reilly, J. (1969). The creative porpoise: training for novel behavior. Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 653–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramachandran, V. S. (1998). The neurology and evolution of humor, laughter, and smiling. Medical Hypotheses, 51, 351–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramsey, G., Bastian, M. L. & van Schaik, C. (2007). Animal innovation defined and operationalized. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 393–437.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reader, S. M., Hager, Y. & Laland, K. N. (2011). The evolution of primate general and cultural intelligence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 1017–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reader, S. M. & Laland, K. N. (2002). Social intelligence, innovation and enhanced brain size in primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 99, 4436–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reader, S. M. & Laland, K. N. (2003a). Animal innovation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reader, S. M. & Laland, K. N. (2003b). Animal innovation: an introduction. In Reader, S. M. & Laland, K. N. (Eds.), Animal innovation (pp. 3–35). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reuter, M., Roth, S., Holve, K. & Hennig, J. (2006). Identification of first candidate genes for creativity: a pilot study. Brain Research, 1069, 190–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Revonsuo, A. (2000). The reinterpretation of dreams: an evolutionary hypothesis of the function of dreaming. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 904–1121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Root-Bernstein, M. & Root-Bernstein, R. (2006). Imaginary worldplay in childhood and maturity and its impact on adult creativity. Creativity Research Journal 18, 403–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Root-Bernstein, R. & Root-Bernstein, M. (2001). Sparks of genius. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Runco, M. A., Millar, G., Acar, S. & Cramond, B. (2010). Torrance tests of creative thinking as predictors of personal and public achievement: a fifty-year follow-up. Creativity Research Journal, 22, 361–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Runco, M. A., Noble, E. P., Reiter-Palmon, R., Acar, S., Ritchie, T. & Yurkovich, J. M. (2011). The genetic basis of creativity and ideational fluency. Creativity Research Journal, 23, 376–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russ, S. W. & Dillon, J. A. (2011). Changes in children’s pretend play over two decades. Creativity Research Journal, 23, 330–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schredl, M. (1995). Creativity and dream recall. Journal of Creative Behavior, 29, 16–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schredl, M. & Erlacher, D. (2007). Self-reported effects of dreams on waking-life creativity: an empirical study. Journal of Psychology, 141, 35–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schuldberg, D. (2000). Six subclinical spectrum traits in normal creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 13, 5–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott, G., Leritz, L. E. & Mumford, M. D. (2004). The effectiveness of creativity training: a quantitative review. Creativity Research Journal, 16, 361–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott, S. G. & Bruce, R. A. (1994). Determinants of innovative behavior: a path model of individual innovation in the workplace. Academy of Management Journal, 37, 580–607.Google Scholar
Sessa, B. (2008). Is it time to revisit the role of psychedelic drugs in enhancing human creativity?Journal of Psychopharmacology, 22, 821–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharpe, L. L. (2005a). Play fighting does not affect subsequent fighting success in wild meerkats. Animal Behaviour, 69, 1023–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sharpe, L. L. (2005b). Play does not enhance social cohesion in a cooperative mammal. Animal Behaviour, 70, 551–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sharpe, L. L. (2005c). Frequency of social play does not affect dispersal partnerships in wild meerkats. Animal Behaviour, 70, 559–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sharpe, L. L., Clutton-Brock, T. H., Brotherton, P. N. M., Cameron, E. Z. & Cherry, M. I. (2002). Experimental provisioning increases play in free-ranging meerkats. Animal Behaviour, 64, 113–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shettleworth, S. J. (2010). Cognition, evolution and behaviour. 2nd edition. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sigman, M., Neumann, C., Baksh, M., Bwibo, N. & McDonald, M. A. (1989). Relationship between nutrition and development in Kenyan toddlers. Journal of Pediatrics, 115, 357–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simonton, D. K. (1997). Creative productivity: a predictive and explanatory model of career trajectories and landmarks. Psychological Review, 104, 66–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (2000). Creativity: cognitive, personal, developmental, and social aspects. American Psychologist, 55, 151–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simpson, G. G. (1953). The Baldwin effect. Evolution, 7, 110–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siviy, S. M., Love, N. J., DeCiccio, B. M., Giordano, S. B. & Seifert, T. L. (2003). The relative playfulness of juvenile Lewis and Fischer-344 rats. Physiology & Behavior, 80, 385–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, E. F. S. (1991). Early social development in hooded rats (Rattus norvegicus): a link between weaning and play. Animal Behaviour, 41, 513–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, P. K. (1982). Does play matter? Functional and evolutionary aspects of animal and human play. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 5, 139–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, P. K. (2010). Children and play. Chichester, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Smolker, R. J., Richards, A., Connor, R., Mann, J. & Berggren, P. (1997). Sponge carrying by dolphins (Delphinidae, Tursiops sp.): a foraging specialization involving tool use?Ethology, 103, 454–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sol, D., Duncan, R. P., Blackburn, T. M., Cassey, P. & Lefebvre, L. (2005). Big brains, enhanced cognition, and response of birds to novel environments. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102, 5460–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spady, T. C. & Ostrander, E. A. (2008). Canine behavioural genetics: pointing out the phenotypes and herding up the genes. American Journal of Human Genetics, 82, 10–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spalding, D. A. (1873). Instinct with original observations on young animals. Macmillan’s Magazine, 27, 282–93.Google Scholar
Spencer, H. (1872). Principles of psychology. 2nd edition. New York: Appleton.Google Scholar
Spinka, M., Newberry, R. C. & Bekoff, M. (2001). Mammalian play: training for the unexpected. Quarterly Review of Biology, 76, 141–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stamps, J. (1995). Motor learning and the value of familiar space. American Naturalist, 146, 41–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., O’Hara, L. A. & Lubart, T. I. (1997). Creativity as investment. California Management Review, 40, 8–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suslov, I. (1992). Computer model of a ‘sense of humour’. I. General algorithm. Biofizika, 37, 242 [available at ].Google Scholar
Sutton-Smith, B. (1986). Toys as culture. New York: Gardner.Google Scholar
Sutton-Smith, B. (1997). The ambiguity of play. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Svartberg, K. & Forkman, B. (2002). Personality traits in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 79, 133–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sylva, K., Bruner, J. S. & Genova, P. (1976). The role of play in the problem-solving of children 3–5 years old. In Bruner, J. S., Jolly, A. & Sylva, K. (Eds.), Play: its role in development and evolution (pp. 244–57). Harmondsworth, UK: Penguin.Google Scholar
Tan, P. L. & Counsilman, J. J. (1985). The influence of weaning on prey-catching behaviour in kittens. Zeitschrift f ür Tierpsychologie, 70, 148–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teasdale, T. W. & Owen, D. R. (2008). Secular declines in cognitive test scores: a reversal of the Flynn effect. Intelligence, 36, 121–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tebbich, S., Sterelny, K. & Teschke, I. (2010). The tale of the finch: adaptive radiation and behavioural flexibility. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 365, 1099–109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tickell, C. (2012). The early years: foundations for life, health and learning. London: Her Majesty’s Government.Google Scholar
Tinbergen, N. (1963). On aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschrift f ür Tierpsychologie, 20, 410–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torrance, E. P. (1961). Priming creative thinking in the primary grades. Elementary School Journal, 62, 139–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torrance, E. P. (1972). Predictive validity of Torrance tests of creative thinking. Journal of Creative Behavior, 6, 236–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Treadwell, Y. (1970). Humor and creativity. Psychological Reports, 26, 55–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trezza, V., Baarendse, P. J. J. & Vanderschuren, L. J. M. J. (2010). The pleasures of play: pharmacological insights into social reward mechanisms. Trends in Pharmacological Sciences, 31, 463–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ulrich, R. S. (1984). View through a window may influence recovery from surgery. Science, 224, 420–1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ulrich, R. S., Berry, L. L., Quan, X. B. & Parish, J. T. (2010). Conceptual framework for the domain of evidence-based design. HERD: Health Environments Research & Design Journal, 4, 95–114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valentine, G. & McKendrick, J. (1997). Children’s outdoor play: exploring parental concerns about children’s safety and the changing nature of childhood. Geoforum, 28, 219–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van de Castle, R. L. (1994). Our dreaming mind. New York: Ballentine.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1967). Play and its role in the mental development of the child. Soviet Psychology, 5, 6–18.Google Scholar
Wallas, G. (1926). The art of thought. London: Watts.Google Scholar
Ward, H. (2012). All work and no play. TES Magazine, 2 November 2012, 5017, 26–30.Google Scholar
Watson, J. (1968). The double helix: a personal account of the discovery of the structure of DNA. New York: Scribner.Google Scholar
Wegener, A. (1912). Die Herausbildung der Grossformen der Erdrinde (Kontinente und Ozeane), auf geophysikalischer Grundlage. Petermanns Geographische Mitteilungen, 63, 185–95, 253–6, 305–9.Google Scholar
West, M. (1974). Social play in the cat. American Zoologist, 14, 427–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiten, A. & Byrne, R. W. (1988). Tactical deception in primates. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 11, 233–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiley, D., Ware, C., Bocconcelli, A., Cholewiak, D., Friedlaender, A., Thompson, M., et al. (2011). Underwater components of humpback whale bubble-net feeding behaviour. Behaviour, 148, 575–602.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood-Gush, D. G. M., Vestergaard, K. & Petersen, H. V. (1990). The significance of motivation and environment in the development of exploration in pigs. Biology of Behaviour, 15, 39–52.Google Scholar
Wyles, J. S., Kunkel, J. G. & Wilson, A. C. (1983). Birds, behavior, and anatomical evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 80, 4394–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ziv, A. (1989). Using humor to develop creative thinking. Journal of Children in Contemporary Society, 20, 99–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • Patrick Bateson, University of Cambridge, Paul Martin, Wolfson College, Cambridge
  • Book: Play, Playfulness, Creativity and Innovation
  • Online publication: 05 July 2013
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139057691.015
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • Patrick Bateson, University of Cambridge, Paul Martin, Wolfson College, Cambridge
  • Book: Play, Playfulness, Creativity and Innovation
  • Online publication: 05 July 2013
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139057691.015
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • Patrick Bateson, University of Cambridge, Paul Martin, Wolfson College, Cambridge
  • Book: Play, Playfulness, Creativity and Innovation
  • Online publication: 05 July 2013
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139057691.015
Available formats
×