Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-18T10:53:00.356Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

17 - Early Childhood Teachers' Awareness, Beliefs, and Practices toward Children's Private Speech

from Part IV - Motivational and Educational Applications

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 July 2009

Adam Winsler
Affiliation:
George Mason University, Virginia
Charles Fernyhough
Affiliation:
University of Durham
Ignacio Montero
Affiliation:
Universidad Autónoma de Madrid
Get access

Summary

Private speech is speech spoken to oneself for communication, self-guidance, and self-regulation of behavior (Manning, 1991; Vygotsky, 1934/1986; Winsler, Díaz, & Montero, 1997). Although it is audible, it is neither intended for, nor directed at, others (Manning, 1991; Piaget, 1959; White & Manning, 1994). Overall, there is evidence that task-related private speech might be an important factor in task performance and future achievement. For example, Bivens and Berk (1990) compared children's frequency of private speech used in first grade to their later performance in second grade. They found that the occurrence of task-relevant private speech was positively correlated with achievement 1 year later. In a similar study, Behrend, Rosengren, and Perlmutter (1992) compared the frequency of children's private speech during puzzle-solving activities (Time 1) with their later performance on puzzle solving tasks 1 week later (Time 2). They found that children who had frequent private speech utterances during Time 1 improved their performance on the puzzle-solving task during Time 2.

The awareness, beliefs, and practices of teachers in relation to children's private speech in a natural classroom environment are still largely unknown. Yet, the influence of teachers and teacher-child relationships on child outcomes is well documented (Pianta, 1997; Pianta, Nimetz, & Bennett, 1997). In a study by Winsler et al. (1997), 40 children were videotaped during adult-scaffolded task completion.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Azmitia, M. (1992). Expertise, private speech, and self-regulation. In Díaz, R. M. & Berk, L. E. (Eds.), Private speech: From social interaction to self-regulation (pp. 101–122). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Behrend, D. A., Rosengren, K. S., & Perlmutter, M. (1992). The relation between private speech and parental interactive style. In Díaz, R. M. & Berk, L. E. (Eds.), Private speech: From social interaction to self-regulation (pp. 85–100). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Berger, P. L., & Luckmann, T. (1967). The social construction of reality: A treatise in the sociology of knowledge. Garden City, NY: Anchor Books.Google Scholar
Berk, L. E. (1986). Relationship of elementary school children's private speech to behavioral accompaniment to task attention and task performance. Developmental Psychology, 22, 671–680.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berk, L. E., & Garvin, R. A. (1984). Development of private speech among low-income Appalachian children. Developmental Psychology, 20, 271–286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berk, L. E., & Potts, M. K. (1991). Development and functional significance of private speech among attention-deficit hyperactivity disordered and normal boys. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 19, 357–377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bivens, J. A., & Berk, L. E. (1990). A longitudinal study of the development of elementary school children's private speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 36, 443–463.Google Scholar
Copeland, A. P. (1979). Types of private speech produced by hyperactive and nonhyperactive boys. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 7, 169–177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deniz, C. B. (2001). Teachers' perceptions of children's private speech: A pilot study. Unpublished manuscript, George Mason University.Google Scholar
Elicker, J., Huang, H., & Wen, X. (2003, April). Early childhood teachers' curriculum beliefs: Are they consistent with classroom practices. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Tampa, FL.Google Scholar
Fang, Z. (1996). A review of research on teacher beliefs and practices. Educational Research, 38, 47–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fisher, C. (1990). The effects of teacher-perceived academic achievement ability, socioeconomic status, gender, and sociability on the private speech of kindergarten students during the performance of a task. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 324 105)
Frauenglass, M. H., & Díaz, R. M. (1985). Self-regulatory functions of children's private speech: A critical analysis of recent challenges to Vygotsky's theory. Developmental Psychology, 21, 357–364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kagan, D. (1992). Implications of research on teacher belief. Educational Psychologist, 27, 65–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kohlberg, L., Yaeger, J., & Hjertholm, E. (1968). Private speech: Four studies and a review of theories. Child Development, 39, 691–736.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, J. (1999). The effects of 5-year-old preschoolers' use of private speech on performance and attention for two kinds of problem solving tasks. Dissertation Abstracts International, 60, 06A. (UMI No. AAG99–32671)Google Scholar
Manning, B. H. (1991). Cognitive self-instruction for classroom processes. Albany: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Manning, B. H., White, C. S., & Daugherty, M. (1994). Young children's private speech as a precursor to metacognitive strategy use during task engagement. Discourse Processes, 17, 191–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McMullen, M. B. (2003, April). Acquiring and supporting developmentally appropriate beliefs and practices in early care and education professionals. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Tampa, FL.Google Scholar
Miles, M., & Huberman, A. (1994). Qualitative data analysis: An expanded sourcebook (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Oliver, J. A., Edmiaston, R., & Fitzgerald, L. M. (2003, April). Regular and special education teachers' beliefs regarding the role of private speech in children's learning. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Tampa, FL.Google Scholar
Pajares, M. (1992). Teachers' beliefs and educational research: Cleaning up a messy construct. Review of Educational Research, 62, 307–332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1959). The language and thought of the child. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Pianta, R. (1997). Adult-child relationship processes and early schooling. Early Education & Development, 8, 11–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pianta, R. C., Nimetz, S. L., & Bennett, E. (1997). Mother-child relationships, teacher-child relationships, and school outcomes in preschool and kindergarten. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 12, 263–280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language (Kozulin, A., Trans.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Original work published 1934)Google Scholar
Weiss, R. (1994). Learning from strangers: The art and method of qualitative interview studies. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
White, C., Deal, D., & Deniz, C. (2004). Teachers' knowledge, beliefs, and practices and mathematical and analogical reasoning. In English, L. (Ed.), Mathematical and analogical reasoning of young children (pp. 127–152). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
White, C. S., & Manning, B. H. (1994). The effects of verbal scaffolding instruction on young children's private speech and problem-solving capabilities. Instructional Science, 22, 39–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winsler, A. (1998). Parent-child interaction and private speech in boys with ADHD. Applied Developmental Science, 2, 17–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winsler, A., Carlton, M. P., & Barry, M. J. (2000). Age-related changes in preschool children's systematic use of private speech in a natural setting. Journal of Child Language, 27, 665–687.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winsler, A., & Díaz, R. M. (1995). Private speech in the classroom: The effects of activity type, presence of others, classroom context, and mixed-age grouping. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 18, 463–487.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winsler, A., Díaz, R. M., & Montero, I. (1997). The role of private speech in the transition from collaborative to independent task performance in young children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 12, 59–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winsler, A., Manfra, L., & Díaz, R. M. (2007). “Should I let them talk?”: Private speech and task performance among preschool children with and without behavior problems. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 22, 215–231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winsler, A., & Naglieri, J. (2003). Overt and covert verbal problem-solving strategies: Developmental trends in use, awareness, and relations with task performance in children age 5 to 17. Child Development, 74, 659–678.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×