Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-m9pkr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-11T17:25:30.394Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - Linguistic areas and diffusion

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

R. M. W. Dixon
Affiliation:
La Trobe University, Victoria
Get access

Summary

If two languages are in contact – some of the speakers of each having a degree of competence in the other – they are likely to borrow lexemes, grammatical categories and techniques, and some grammatical forms (in at least one direction, often in both directions) and gradually become more similar.

If a number of languages are spoken in a geographically continuous area – which contains no physical or social impediments to cross-cultural communication – there will in each language community be a degree of multilingualism. A number of linguistic traits will diffuse from language to language until each applies across a considerable region within the geographical area, sometimes across the whole area. Each language has two possible kinds of similarities to other languages – genetic similarities, which are shared inheritances from a common proto-language; and areal similarities, which are due to borrowing from geographical neighbours. Emeneau (1956:16) defined ‘linguistic area’ as ‘an area which includes languages belonging to more than one family but showing traits in common which are found not to belong to other members of (at least) one of the families’. This is a readily recognisable area. We could equally well have an area which includes all the languages of each of several families (no language from any of the families being located outside the area). If the languages from each family were scattered over the area, rather than each being in a solid block, then it might be possible to distinguish genetic from areal similarities. (Lowland Amazonia is a linguistic area essentially of this type.)

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 1997

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×