Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-pfhbr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-12T08:36:32.711Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Appendix – where the comparative method discovery procedure fails

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

R. M. W. Dixon
Affiliation:
La Trobe University, Victoria
Get access

Summary

The term ‘comparative method’ has been used in two distinct ways. In its original and widest sense it refers to a method of proving genetic relationships between languages. This involves the systematic comparison of grammatical forms and lexemes, establishing correspondence sets between phonemes, putting forward hypotheses concerning a putative proto-system and the changes through which the systems of modern languages developed (see, for example, Meillet 1925/1967).

‘Comparative method’ has also been used, in a narrower sense, to describe a set of discovery procedures which would automatically reconstruct the phonemes for a proto-language. This procedure was first put forth by Hoenigswald (1950), being directly analogised from the discovery procedures then in fashion for working out phonemes in synchronic linguistic analysis. Hoenigswald suggested that just as phonetically related phones that are in complementary distribution can be grouped together as allophones of one phoneme (as was believed at the time), so phonetically similar correspondence sets that are in complementary distribution should be grouped together as reflexes of a single proto-phoneme.

Hoenigswald's discovery procedure for comparative reconstruction rapidly gained acceptance. His own textbook (Hoenigswald 1960) stated the environment for a correspondence set in terms of other correspondence sets and looked for the grouping into proto-phonemes that gives the simplest overall phonemic system for the proto-language; there is an analogy to phonemic procedures but Hoenigswald did now imply that it is a problem of a different type from synchronic analysis. However, Hockett, in his textbook (1958: 485–505), went one step further and explained reconstruction by ‘the comparative method’ as if it were identical to an exercise in synchronic phonemic analysis; each distinct correspondence set relates to a phone in the proto-language and these are then grouped into phonemes in the familiar way.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 1997

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×