Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-09T03:29:49.207Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 20 - An Introduction to Psychologically Plausible Sampling Schemes for Approximating Bayesian Inference

from Part VI - Computational Approaches

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 June 2023

Klaus Fiedler
Affiliation:
Universität Heidelberg
Peter Juslin
Affiliation:
Uppsala Universitet, Sweden
Jerker Denrell
Affiliation:
University of Warwick
Get access

Summary

The brain must make inferences about, and decisions concerning, a highly complex and unpredictable world, based on sparse evidence. An “ideal” normative approach to such challenges is often modeled in terms of Bayesian probabilistic inference. But for real-world problems of perception, motor control, categorization, language understanding, or commonsense reasoning, exact probabilistic calculations are computationally intractable. Instead, we suggest that the brain solves these hard probability problems approximately, by considering one, or a few, samples from the relevant distributions. Here we provide a gentle introduction to the various sampling algorithms that have been considered as the approximation used by the brain. We broadly summarize these algorithms according to their level of knowledge and their assumptions regarding the target distribution, noting their strengths and weaknesses, their previous applications to behavioural phenomena, as well as their psychological plausibility.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abbott, J. T., & Griffiths, T. L. (2011). Exploring the influence of particle filter parameters on order effects in causal learning. Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 33, 29502955.Google Scholar
Arulampalam, M. S., Maskell, S., Gordon, N., & Clapp, T. (2002). A tutorial on particle filters for online nonlinear/non-Gaussian Bayesian tracking. IEEE Transactions on Signal Processing, 50(2), 174188.Google Scholar
Battaglia, P. W., Hamrick, J. B., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2013). Simulation as an engine of physical scene understanding. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(45), 1832718332.Google Scholar
Blurton, S. P., Kyllingsbæk, S., Nielsen, C. S., & Bundesen, C. (2020). A Poisson random walk model of response times. Psychological Review, 127(3), 362.Google Scholar
Bonawitz, E., Denison, S., Griffiths, T. L., & Gopnik, A. (2014). Probabilistic models, learning algorithms, and response variability: sampling in cognitive development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(10), 497500.Google Scholar
Bowles, S., Kirman, A., & Sethi, R. (2017). Retrospectives: Friedrich Hayek and the market algorithm. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 31(3), 215–30.Google Scholar
Bramley, N. R., Dayan, P., Griffiths, T. L., & Lagnado, D. A. (2017). Formalizing Neurath’s ship: Approximate algorithms for online causal learning. Psychological Review, 124(3), 301.Google Scholar
Brown, G. D., Neath, I., & Chater, N. (2007). A temporal ratio model of memory. Psychological Review, 114(3), 539576.Google Scholar
Brown, S. D., & Steyvers, M. (2009). Detecting and predicting changes. Cognitive Psychology, 58(1), 4967.Google Scholar
Chater, N., & Manning, C. D. (2006). Probabilistic models of language processing and acquisition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(7), 335344.Google Scholar
Dasgupta, I., Schulz, E., & Gershman, S. J. (2017). Where do hypotheses come from? Cognitive Psychology, 96, 125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daw, N., & Courville, A. (2008). The pigeon as particle filter. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems, 20, 369376.Google Scholar
Dellaportas, P., & Roberts, G. O. (2003). An introduction to MCMC. In Spatial statistics and computational methods (pp. 141). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Doucet, A., De Freitas, N., & Gordon, N. (2001). An introduction to sequential Monte Carlo methods. In Sequential Monte Carlo methods in practice (pp. 3-14). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Earl, D. J., & Deem, M. W. (2005). Parallel tempering: Theory, applications, and new perspectives. Physical Chemistry Chemical Physics, 7(23), 39103916.Google Scholar
Fox, C. R., & Tversky, A. (1998). A belief-based account of decision under uncertainty. Management Science, 44, 879895.Google Scholar
Geman, S., & Geman, D. (1984). Stochastic relaxation, Gibbs distributions, and the Bayesian restoration of images. IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence (6), 721741.Google Scholar
Gershman, S. J. (2019). Uncertainty and exploration. Decision, 6(3), 277.Google Scholar
Gershman, S. J., Blei, D. M., & Niv, Y. (2010). Context, learning, and extinction. Psychological Review, 117(1), 197.Google Scholar
Gershman, S. J., Vul, E., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2012). Multistability and perceptual inference. Neural Computation, 24(1), 124.Google Scholar
Geweke, J. (1989). Bayesian inference in econometric models using Monte Carlo integration. Econometrica, 57(6), 13171339.Google Scholar
Geyer, C. J. (1991). Markov chain Monte Carlo maximum likelihood. In Keramidas, (Ed.), Computing Science and Statistics: Proceedings of the 23rd Symposium on the Interface. Interface Foundation, Fairfax Station, pp. 156–163.Google Scholar
Gilden, D. L. (2001). Cognitive emissions of 1/f noise. Psychological Review, 108(1), 33.Google Scholar
Gilden, D. L., Thornton, T., & Mallon, M. W. (1995). 1/f noise in human cognition. Science, 267(5205), 18371839.Google Scholar
Gittins, J., & Jones, D. (1979). A dynamic allocation index for the discounted multiarmed bandit problem. Biometrika. 66(3), 561565.Google Scholar
Griffiths, T. L., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2006). Optimal predictions in everyday cognition. Psychological Science, 17(9), 767773.Google Scholar
Griffiths, T. L., Vul, E., & Sanborn, A. N. (2012). Bridging levels of analysis for probabilistic models of cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(4), 263268.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1972). Subjective probability: A judgment of representativeness. Cognitive Psychology, 3(3), 430454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koller, D., & Friedman, N. (2009). Probabilistic graphical models: principles and techniques. Cambridge; MA: MIT.Google Scholar
Krafft, P. M., Shmueli, E., Griffiths, T. L., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2021). Bayesian collective learning emerges from heuristic social learning. Cognition, 212, 104469.Google Scholar
Kuhn, T. (1962). The structure of scientific revolutions, Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Levy, R. P., Reali, F., & Griffiths, T. L. (2008). Modeling the effects of memory on human online sentence processing with particle filters. In Koller, D., Schuurmans, D., Bengio, Y., & Bottou, L. (Eds.), Advances in neural information processing systems 21 (pp. 937944).Google Scholar
Lieder, F., Griffiths, T. L., & Hsu, M. (2018). Overrepresentation of extreme events in decision making reflects rational use of cognitive resources. Psychological Review, 125(1), 1.Google Scholar
Lloyd, K., Sanborn, A., Leslie, D., & Lewandowsky, S. (2019). Why higher working memory capacity may help you learn: Sampling, search, and degrees of approximation. Cognitive Science, 43(12), e12805.Google Scholar
Metropolis, N., Rosenbluth, A. W., Rosenbluth, M. N., Teller, A. H., & Teller, E. (1953). Equation of state calculations by fast computing machines. Journal of Chemical Physics, 21(6), 10871092.Google Scholar
Mosteller, F., & Nogee, P. (1951). An experimental measurement of utility. Journal of Political Economy, 59(5), 371404.Google Scholar
Murphy, K. P. (2012). Machine learning: a probabilistic perspective. Cambridge, MA: MIT.Google Scholar
Nobandegani, A. S., Castanheira, K. D. S., Otto, A. R., & Shultz, T. R. (2018). Over-representation of extreme events in decision-making: A rational metacognitive account. arXiv preprint arXiv:1801.09848.Google Scholar
Osofsky, R. M. (1984). Choice, similarity, and the context theory of classification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10(1), 104114.Google Scholar
Nosofsky, R. M. (2011). The generalized context model: An exemplar model of classification. In Pothos, E. M. & Wills, A. J. (Eds.), Formal approaches in categorization (pp. 1839). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
O’Hagan, A., & Forster, J. J. (2004). Kendall’s advanced theory of statistics: Bayesian inference (Vol. 2B). London: Arnold.Google Scholar
Pleskac, T. J., & Busemeyer, J. R. (2010). Two-stage dynamic signal detection: a theory of choice, decision time, and confidence. Psychological Review, 117(3), 864.Google Scholar
Prat-Carrabin, A., Wilson, R. C., Cohen, J. D., & Azeredo da Silveira, R. (2021). Human inference in changing environments with temporal structure. Psychological Review, 128(5), 879912.Google Scholar
Raaijmakers, J. G., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1981). Search of associative memory. Psychological Review, 88(2), 93134.Google Scholar
Ratcliff, R., & Rouder, J. N. (1998). Modeling response times for two-choice decisions. Psychological Science, 9(5), 347356.Google Scholar
Rhodes, T., & Turvey, M. T. (2007). Human memory retrieval as Lévy foraging. Physica A: Statistical Mechanics and its Applications, 385(1), 255260.Google Scholar
Roth, D. (1996). On the hardness of approximate reasoning. Artificial Intelligence, 82(1–2), 273302.Google Scholar
Russo, D. J., Van Roy, B., Kazerouni, A., Osband, I., & Wen, Z. (2018). A tutorial on Thompson sampling. Foundations and Trends® in Machine Learning, 11(1), 196.Google Scholar
Sanborn, A. N., & Chater, N. (2016). Bayesian brains without probabilities. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(12), 883893.Google Scholar
Sanborn, A. N., Griffiths, T. L., & Navarro, D. J. (2010). Rational approximations to rational models: alternative algorithms for category learning. Psychological Review, 117(4), 1144.Google Scholar
Sanborn, A. N., Mansinghka, V. K., & Griffiths, T. L. (2013). Reconciling intuitive physics and Newtonian mechanics for colliding objects. Psychological Review, 120(2), 411.Google Scholar
Shadlen, M. N., & Shohamy, D. (2016). Decision making and sequential sampling from memory. Neuron, 90(5), 927939.Google Scholar
Shi, L., Griffiths, T. L., Feldman, N. H., & Sanborn, A. N. (2010). Exemplar models as a mechanism for performing Bayesian inference. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17(4), 443464.Google Scholar
Sloman, S., Rottenstreich, Y., Wisniewski, E., Hadjichristidis, C., & Fox, C. R. (2004). Typical versus atypical unpacking and superadditive probability judgment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 573582.Google Scholar
Speekenbrink, M. (2016). A tutorial on particle filters. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 73, 140152.Google Scholar
Speekenbrink, M., & Konstantinidis, E. (2015). Uncertainty and exploration in a restless bandit problem. Topics in Cognitive Science, 7(2), 351367.Google Scholar
Spicer, J., Mullett, T. L., & Sanborn, A. N. (2022, December 2). Repeated Risky Choices Become More Consistent with Themselves but not Expected Value, with No Effect of Trial Order. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/jgefrGoogle Scholar
Stigler, S. M. (1986). The history of statistics: The measurement of uncertainty before 1900. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Thompson, W. R. (1933). On the likelihood that one unknown probability exceeds another in view of the evidence of two samples. Biometrika, 25(3–4), 285294.Google Scholar
Tierney, L. (1994). Markov chains for exploring posterior distributions. Annals of Statistics, 1701–1728.Google Scholar
Todd, P. M., & Hills, T. T. (2020). Foraging in mind. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 29(3), 309315.Google Scholar
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5(2), 207232.Google Scholar
Usher, M., & McClelland, J. L. (2001). The time course of perceptual choice: The leaky, competing accumulator model. Psychological Review, 108(3), 550.Google Scholar
Van Rooij, I. (2008). The tractable cognition thesis. Cognitive Science, 32(6), 939984.Google Scholar
Von Neumann, J., & Morgenstern, O. (2007). Theory of games and economic behavior (commemorative edition). Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Vul, E., Goodman, N., Griffiths, T. L., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2014). One and done? Optimal decisions from very few samples. Cognitive Science, 38(4), 599637.Google Scholar
Vul, E., & Pashler, H. (2008). Measuring the crowd within: Probabilistic representations within individuals. Psychological Science, 19(7), 645647.Google Scholar
Vulkan, N. (2000). An economist’s perspective on probability matching. Journal of Economic Surveys, 14(1), 101118.Google Scholar
Wagenaar, W. A. (1972). Generation of random sequences by human subjects: A critical survey of literature. Psychological Bulletin, 77(1), 6572.Google Scholar
Wolpert, D. M. (2007). Probabilistic models in human sensorimotor control. Human Movement Science, 26(4), 511524.Google Scholar
Yi, M. S., Steyvers, M., & Lee, M. (2009). Modeling human performance in restless bandits with particle filters. Journal of Problem Solving, 2(2), 5.Google Scholar
Zhu, J.-Q., Sanborn, A., & Chater, N. (2018). Mental sampling in multimodal representations. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems, 31, 57485759.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×