Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-5c6d5d7d68-pkt8n Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-09-01T07:23:12.028Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Bibliography

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

H. R. Ekbia
Affiliation:
Indiana University
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Artificial Dreams
The Quest for Non-Biological Intelligence
, pp. 373 - 392
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Agre, P. E. (1995). “The Soul Gained and Lost: Artificial Intelligence as A Philosophical Project.” Stanford Humanity Review 4(2).Google Scholar
Agre, P. E. (1997a). Computation and Human Experience. Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Agre, P. E. (1997b). Toward a Critical Technical Practice: Lessons Learned in Trying to Reform AI. Bridging the Great Divide: Social Science, Technical Systems, and Cooperative Work. G. Bowker, L. S. Star, B. Turner and L. Gasser. New York, NY, Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Agre, P. E. (2002). The Practical Logic of Computer Work. Computationalism: New Directions. M. Scheutz. Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Agre, P. E. and , D. Chapman (1987). Pengi: An implementation of a theory of activity. Sixth National Conference on Artificial Intelligence, San Mateo, CA., Morgan Kaufmann.Google Scholar
Agre, P. E. and , I. D. Horswill (1992). Cultural Support for Improvisation. Tenth National Conference on Artificial Intelligence, Los Altos, CA., Morgan Kaufmann.Google Scholar
Agre, P. E. and , D. Schuler (1997). Reinventing Technology, Rediscovering Community: Critical Explorations of Computing as a Social Practice. New York, NY, Ablex.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. A (1995). An Introduction to Neural Networks. Cambridge, MA., A Bradford Book/ MIT Press.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R (1993). Rules of the Mind. Hillsdale: NJ, Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Athanasiou, T. (1985). Artificial Intelligence: cleverly disguised politics. Compulsive Technology. T. Solomonides and L. Levidow. London, UK, Free Association Books: 13–35.Google Scholar
Axelrod, R. and , L. Tesfatsion (2007). A Guide for Newcomers to Agent-Based Modeling in the Social Sciences. Handbook of Computational Economics, Vol. 2: Agent-Based Computational Economics. K. L. Judd and L. Tesfatsion. Amsterdam, North-Holland, Available at http://www.econ.iastate.edu/tesfatsi/abmread.htm.Google Scholar
Bajscy, R. and , E. W. Large (1999). “When and Where Will AI Meet Robotics.” AI Magazine 20(3): 57–65.Google Scholar
Ballard, D. H. (1997). An Introduction to Natural Computation. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Barad, K. (2003). “Posthumanist Performativity: Toward an Understanding of How Matter Comes to Matter.” Journal of Women in Culture and Society 28(3): 801–831.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A Study in Experimental and Social Psychology. New York, Macmillan.Google Scholar
Barwise, J. and , J. Etchemendy (1994). Hyperproof. New York, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bechtel, W. (1998). “Representations and Cognitive Explanations: Assessing the Dynamicist's Challenge in Cognitive Science.” Cognitive Science 22(3): 295–318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bechtel, W. and , R. C. Richardson (1992). Discovering Complexity: Decomposition and localization as strategies in scientific research. Princeton, NJ., Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Becker, H. (1982). The Art Worlds. Berkeley: CA, University of California Press.Google Scholar
Becker, H. S (forthcoming). What About Mozart? What About Murder? Accessed January 21, 2007 from: http://home.earthlink.net/∼hsbecker/mozart.htm.
Biagioli, M., Ed. (1999). Science Studies Reader. New York and London, Routledge.Google Scholar
Bijker, W. E., , T. E. Hughes, et al., Eds. (1997). The Social Construction of Technological Systems: new Directions in the Sociology and History of Technology. Cambridge, MA, The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bloomfield, B. P. (1987). The Culture of Artificial Intelligence. The Question of Artificial Intelligence. B. Bloomfield. London, UK, Croom Helm: 59–105.Google Scholar
Bobrow, D. G. (1964). Natural Language Input for a Computer Problem-Solving System. Report TR-1, Project MAC, MIT, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Boden, M. A. (1970). “Intentionality and Physical Systems.” Philosophy of Science 37: 200–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boden, M. A., Ed. (1990). The Creative Mind: Myths and Mechanisms. New York, Basic Books.Google Scholar
Boden, M. A (1994). Creativity and Computers. Artificial Intelligence and Creativity: An Interdisciplinary Approach. T. H. Dartnall. Dordrecht, Kluwer Academic Publishers.Google Scholar
Bolter, D. (1984). Turing's Man: Western Culture in the Computer Age. Chapel Hill, NC, University of North Carolina Press.Google Scholar
Born, R., Ed. (1987). Artificial Intelligence: The Case Against. London, Croom Helm.Google Scholar
Braitenberg, V. (1984). Vehicles: Experiments in Synthetic Psychology. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Breazeal, C. and , B. Scassellati (2000). Challenges in Building Robots that Imitate People. Imitation in Animals and Artifacts. K. Dautenhahn and C. Nehaniv. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Brick, T. and , M. Scheutz (2007). Incremental Natural Language Processing for HRI. HRI'07, Arlington, VA.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooks, R., , C. Breazeal, et al. (1998). The Cog Project: Building a Humanoid Robot. Computation for Metaphors, Analogy and Agents. C. Nehaniv, Springer-Verlag. LNCS1562.Google Scholar
Brooks, R. A (1986). “A Robust Layered Control System for a Mobile Robot.” IEEE Transactions on Robotics and Automation 2(1): 14–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooks, R. A (1991/1997). Intelligence without representation. Mind Design II. J. Haugeland. Cambridge, MA, The MIT Press. 395–420.
Brooks, R. A (1999). Cambrian Intelligence: The Early History of the New AI. Cambridge, MA., A Bradford Book/MIT Press.Google Scholar
Brooks, R. A (2002a). Flesh and Machines: How Robots Will Change Us. New York, Pantheon Books.Google Scholar
Brooks, R. A (2002b). “Lord of the Robots.” Technology Review 105(3): 78–83.Google Scholar
Brooks, R. A and , L. A. Stein (1994). “Building Brains for Bodies.” Autonomous Robots 1(1): 7–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruce, B. (1999) Educational Reform: How Does Technology Affect Educational Change? Journal of Adolescent and Adult Education. Accessed Nov. 14, 2006 from: http://www.readingonline.org/electronic/jaal/9-99_Column.html.Google Scholar
Brucell, V., , M. Burton, et al. (1993). “Sex Discrimination: how do we tell the difference between male and female faces?Perception 22: 131–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buchanan, B. G., Sutherland, G. L., et al. (1969). Heurstic DENDRAL: A program for generating explanatory hypothesis in organic chemistry. Machine Intelligence. B. Meltzer, Michie, D., and Swann, M. Edinburgh, Scotland, Edinburgh University Press. 4: 209–254.Google Scholar
Castañeda, C. and , L. Suchman (2005). Robots Visions. Accessed April 25, 2007 from:
http://www.comp.lancs.ac.uk/sociology/papers/***.pdf
Castells, M. (1996). The Rise of The Network Society. Malden: MA, Blackwell Publishers.Google Scholar
Castells, M. (2001). The Internet Galaxy: Reflections on the Internet, Business, and Society. Oxford: UK, Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chalmers, D. J., R. M. French, et al. (1992). “High-level Perception, Representation, and Analogy.” Journal of Experimental and Theoretical Artificial Intelligence 4(3): 185–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chemero, A. (2000). “Anti-Representationalism and the Dynamical Stance.” Philosophy of Science 67: 626–647.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic Structures. The Hague, Mouton.Google Scholar
Chrisley (2002). Artificial Intelligence. The Oxford Companion to the Mind. R. Gregory. Oxford, Oxford University Press.
Christiansen, M. H and , N. Chater (1999). “Connectionist Natural Language Processing: The State of the Art.” Cognitive Science 23(4): 417–437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Churchland (1997). On the Nature of Theories: A Neurocomputational Perspective. Mind Design II. J. Haugeland. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press: 251–292.
Churchland, P. M (1989). On the Nature of Theories: A Neurocomputatoinal Perspective. Mind Design II. J. Haugeland. Cambridge, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clancey, W. J (1997). Situated Cognition: On Human Knowledge and Computer Representations. Cambridge, UK, Univeristy of Cambridge.Google Scholar
Clapin, H., Ed. (2002). Philosophy of Mental Representation. Oxford, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (1997). Being There: Putting Brain, Body, and World Together Again. Cambridge, MA, A Bradford Book/MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2001). Mindware. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2003). Natural-Born Cyborgs: Minds, Technologies, and the Future of Human Intelligence. Oxford, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V (1993). The Lexicon in Acquisition. Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clynes, M. E and N. S. Kline (1960). “Cyborgs and Space.” Astronautics (Sept. 1960): 33–42.Google Scholar
Cohen, D. I. A. (1997). Theory of Computer Theory. New York, John Wiley and Sons.Google Scholar
Cohen, H. (1995). “The Further Exploits of AARON, Painter.” Stanford Humanities Review 4(2).Google Scholar
Cohen, P. R and , A. E. Howe (1988). “How Evaluation Guides AI Research.” AI Magazine Winter 1988: 35–43.Google Scholar
Collins, G. (1989). Coach. Inside Case-based Reasoning. C. Riesbeck and R. C. Schank. Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Collins, H. (1990). Artificial Experts: Social Knowledge and Intelligent Machines. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Collins, H. (1992). “Hubert L. Dreyfus, Forms of Life, and a Simple Test for Machine Intelligence.” Social Studies of Science 22: 726–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collins, H. and , M. Kusch (1998). The Shape of Actions: What Humans and Machines Can Do. Cambridge, MA., MIT press.Google Scholar
Collins, H. and , T. Pinch (1998). The Golem: What everyone should know about science. Cambirdge, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cope, D. (1991). Computers and Musical Style. Madison: WI, A-R Editions, Inc.Google Scholar
Cope, D. (2001). Virtual Music: Computer Synthesis of Musical Style. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Copeland, J. B (1997). “CYC: A case Study in Ontological Engineering.” Retrieved April 2003, from http://phil.indiana.edu/ejap/1997.Spring/copeland976.2.html.
Cottrell, G. W., C. Padgett, et al. (2000). Is all face processing holistic? The View from UCSD, ScientificCommons.
Crevier, D. (1993). The Tumultuous History of The Search for Artificial Intelligence. New York, NY, Basic Books.Google Scholar
Cussins, A. (1998). Nonconceptual Content of Experience and Conceptual Content of Thought. Unpublished manuscript.
Cussins, A. (in press). “Content, Embodiment and Objectivity – The Theory of Cognitive Trails.” Unpublished manuscript.
Dartnall, T., Ed. (2002). Creativity, Cognition, and Knowledge. Perspectives on Cognitive Science. Westport: CN, Praeger.Google Scholar
Davidson, A. (1990). Closing Up the Corpses. Meaning and Method. G. Boulos. Cambridge: MA, Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Davis, M. (1988). Mathematical Logic and the Origin of Modern Computers. The Universal Turing Machine: A Half-Century Survey. R. Herken. Oxford, UK, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Day, R. (2005). “Clearing up “implicit knowledge”: Implications for Knowledge Management, information science, psychology, and social epistemology.” Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology 56(6): 630–635.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dell, G. S (1986). “A spreading-activation theory of retreival in sentence production.” Psychological Review 93: 283–321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dell, G. S., , M. F. Schwartz, , N. Martin, , E. M Saffran, , D. A Gagnon (1997). “Lexical access in aphasic and nonaphasic speakers.” Psychological Review 104(801–939).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dell, G. S., F. Chang, Z. M. Griffin (1999). “Connectionist models of language production: Lexical access and grammatical encoding.” Cognitive Science 23: 517–542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dell, G. S., C. Juliano, et al. (1993). “Structure and content in language production: A theory of frame constraints in phonological speech errors.” Cognitive Science 17: 149–195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeLong, H. (1970). A Profile of Mathematical Logic. Reading, MA, Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. (1998). “Revolution, no! Reform.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21(5): 636–637.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennett, D. C (1990). Cognitive Wheels: The Frame Problem of AI. The Philosophy of Artificial Intelligence. M. A. Boden. Oxford, UK, Oxford University Press: 147–170.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C (1991). “Real Patterns.” Journal of Philosophy 88(1): 27–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennett, D. C (1995). Darwin's Dangerous Idea. New York, Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C (1997). True Believers: Intentional Strategy and Why It Works. Mind Design II. J. Haugeland. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press: 57–79.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C (1977). Brainstorms. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Descartes, R. (1979). Meditations on First Philosophy. Indianapolis, IN, Hackett Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (1920/1948). Reconstruction of Philosophy. Boston, MA., Beacon Press.CrossRef
Dewey, J. (1938). Logic: The Theory of Inquiry. Carbondale, IL., Southern Illinois University Press.Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (1950). Common Sense and Science. Carbondale, IL, Southern Illinois University Press.Google Scholar
Dietrich, E. and , A. B. Markman (1998). “All information processing entails computation.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21(5): 637–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dretske, F. I (1994). If You Can't Make One, You Don't Know How It Works. Midwest Studies in Philosophy. P. French and T. Uehling. Notre Dame, IN., University of Notre Dame Press: 468–482.Google Scholar
Dreyfus, H. L (1972). What Computers Can't Do?New York, Harper Collins.Google Scholar
Dreyfus, H. L (1991). “Response to Collins, Artificial Experts.” Social Studies of Science 22: 717–726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dreyfus, H. L (1992). What Computers Still Can't Do? A critique of Artificial Reason. Cambridge, MA, The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dyer, F. C and , T. D. Seeley (1989). “On the evolution of the dance language.” The American Naturalist 133(4): 580–590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edelman, G. M (1987). Neural Darwinism: The Theory of Neural Group Selection. New York, Basic Books.Google Scholar
Edwards, P. N (1996). The Closed World: Computers and The Politics of Discourse in Cold War America. Cambridge, MA, The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ekbia, H. R (2001). Once Again, Artificial Intelligence at A Crossroads. Annual Meeting of the Society for Social Studies of Science, Boston, MA.Google Scholar
Ekbia, H. R (2002a). The Two Senses of “Common Sense.” 13th Midwest Artificial Intelligence and Cognitive Science Conference, Chicago, IL.Google Scholar
Ekbia, H. R (2002b). AI's Disparity Syndrome. Annual Meeting of the Society for Social Studies of Science, Milwaukee, WI.Google Scholar
Ekbia, H. R (2006). Taking Decisions into the Wild: An AI Perspective on the Design of iDSS. Intelligent Decision-Making Support Systems (I-DMSS): Foundations, Applications and Challenges J. Gupta, G. Forgionne and M. More. Berlin, Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Ekbia, H. R (forthcoming). “Informational Artifacts as Quasi-Objects.”
Ekbia, H. R and , L. Gasser (forthcoming). “Common Ground: For a Sociology of Code.”
Ekbia, H. R., , J Goldberg, , D Landy (2003). Starting Large or Small? An Unresolved Dilemma for Connectionist Models of Language Learning. Bootstrapping in Language Acquisition: Psychological, Linguistic and Computational Aspects. Bloomington, IN.Google Scholar
Ekbia, H. R and , N. Hara (2005). Incentive Structures in Knowledge Management. Encyclopedia of Knowledge Management. D. Schwartz. Hershey, PA, Idea Group.Google Scholar
Ekbia, H. R and , R. Kling (2003). Power in Knowledge Management in Late Modern Times. Academy of Management, Seattle, WA.Google Scholar
Ekbia, H. R and , A. Maguitman (2001). Context and Relevance: A Pragmatic Approach. 3rd International Conference on Modeling and Using Context, Dundee, UK, Springer Verlag.Google Scholar
Elman, J. L (1990). “Finding structure in time.” Cognitive Science 14: 179–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elman, J. L (1993). “Learning and development in neural networks: The importance of starting small.” Cognition 48: 71–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enfield, N. J and , S. C. Levinson (2006). Roots of Human Sociality: Culture, Cognition and Interaction. Oxford, Berg.Google Scholar
Epstein, J. M (2005). Remarks on the Foundations of Agent-Based Generative Social Science. The Brookings Institution: CSED Working Paper. Washington DC.Google Scholar
Falkenheiner, B., K. D. Forbus, et al. (1990). “The structure-mapping engine.” Artificial Intelligence 41(1): 1–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farah, M. J., K. D. Wiilson, et al. (1998). “What Is “Special” About Face Perception?Psychological Review 105(3): 42–498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fauconnier, G. (1994). Mental Spaces: Aspects of Meaning Construction in Natural Language. Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feigenbaum, E. A and A. Barr, Eds. (1982). The Handbook of Artificial Intelligence. Reading, MA., Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Fikes, R. E., P. E. Hart, et al. (1972). “Learning and Executing Generalized Robot Plans.” Artificial Intelligence 3(4): 251–288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. and , Z. Pylyshyn (1997). Connectionism and Cognitive Architecture: A Critical Analysis. Mind Design II: Philosophy, Psychology, Artificial Intelligence. J. Haugeland. Cambridge, MA, The MIT Press: 309–350.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A (1980). Methodological Solipsism Considered as a Research Strategy in Cognitive Psychology. Representations: Philosophical Essays on the Foundations of Cognitive Science, The Harvester Press.Google Scholar
Forbus, K. D., , D Gentner, et al. (1998). “Analogy just looks like high level perception: why a domain-general approach to analogical mapping is rightJ. EXPT. THFOR. ARTIF. INTELL 10: 231–257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forrest, S., Ed. (1991). Emergent Computation. Cambridge: MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Forsythe, D. E (2001). Studying Those Who Study Us: An Anthropologist in the World of Artificial Intelligence. Stanford, CA., Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Foucault, M. (1977). Knowledge and Power: Selected Interviews and Other Writings. New York, Pantheon Books.Google Scholar
Foucault, M. (1994). The Order of Things: An Archaelogy of the Human Sciences. New York: NY, Random House/Vintage Books.Google Scholar
Foundalis, H. (2006). Phaeaco: A Cognitive Architecture Inspired by Bongard's Problems. Computer Science Dept. Bloomington, Indiana University.Google Scholar
Friedman, M. (1999). Reconsidering Logical Positivism. Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Funt, B. V (1980). “Problem-Solving with Diagrammatic Representations.” Artificial Intelligence 13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gadamer, H.-G. (1976). Philosophical Hermeneutics. Berkeley, CA, University of California Press.Google Scholar
Gallistel, C. R (1998). Symbolic Processes in the Brain: The Case of Insect Navigation. An Invitation to Cognitive Science: Methods, Models, and Conceptual Issues. D. Scarborough and S. Sternberg. Cambridge, Mass., MIT Press/A Bradford Book.Google Scholar
Gasser, L. (1991). “Social conceptions of knowledge and action.” Artificial Intelligence 47(1): 107–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gasser, M. and , E. Colunga (2000). Babies, Variables, and Relational Correlations. 22nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Philadelphia, Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S., , R Ivry, et al. (1998). Fundamentals of Cognitive Neuroscience. New York, W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
Gee, J. P (1999). An Introduction to Discourse Analysis. UK, Routledge.Google Scholar
Gell, A. (1998). Art and Agency: An Anthropological Theory. Oxford, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Genter, D. (1983). “Structure-Mapping: A Theoretical Framework for Analogy.” Cognitive Science 7(2): 155–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerschenfeld, N. (1999). When Things Start to Think. New York, Henry Holt.Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J (1978). “The Ecological Approach to the Visual Perception of Pictures.” Leonardo 11(3): 227–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giere, R. N (1999). Science without Laws. Chicago, The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Gieryn, T. F (1999). Cultural Boundaries of Science: Credibility on the line. Chicago: IL, Univeristy of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Gilbert, N. (2004). Agent-Based Social Simulation. Center for Research on Social Simulation. Guilford: UK.Google Scholar
Gilbert, N. and , K. G. Troitzsch (2005). Simulation for the Social Scientist. Berkshire: UK, Open University Press.Google Scholar
Goodman, N. (1976). Languages of Art. Indianapolis, IN, Hacket Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Govindarajan, K. K., , S Grossberg, et al. (1994). A neural network model of auditory scene analysis and source segregation. Technical report. Boston, MA.Google Scholar
Graubard, S. R (1988). The Artificial Intelligence Debate: False Starts, Real Foundations. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Greenberg, K., Ed. (2000). The Robot in the Garden. Cambridge: MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1976). “Adaptive pattern classification and universal recoding: Part I. Parallel development and coding of neural feature detectors.” Biological Cybernetic 21: 121–134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1987). “Competetive Learning: From Interactive Activation to Adaptive Resonance.” Cognitive Science 11(1): 23–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1995). “The Attentive Brain.” American Scientist 83: 438–449.Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. and , C. W. Myers (1999). The Resonant Dynamics of Conscious Speech. Technical Report. Boston, MA.Google Scholar
Grover, L. (1996). A Fast Quantum Mechanical Algorithm for Data base Search. 28th Annual ACM Symposium on the Theory of Computing.Google Scholar
Hacking, I. (1979). “What is logic?The Journal of Philosophy LXXVI(6): 285–319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hacking, I. (1983). Representing and Intervening: Introductory Topics in the Philosophy of Natural Science. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hacking, I. (1999). Making Up People. Science Studies Reader. M. Biagioli. New York and London, Routledge: 161–171.Google Scholar
Hammond, K. J (1989). Case-Based Planning: Viewing Planning as a Memory Task. San Diego, CA., Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hammond, K. J., T. M. Converse, et al. (1995). “The Stabilization of Environments.” Artificial Intelligence 72(1–2): 305–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haraway, D. (1991). Simians, Cyborgs, and Women: The Reinvention of Nature. New York, Routledge.Google Scholar
Haraway, D. (1997). Modest_Witness@Second_Millenium.FemaleMan_Meets_OncoMouseTM. New York, Routledge.Google Scholar
Haraway, D. (2003). The Companion Species Manifesto: Dogs, People and Significant Others. Chicago, Prickly Paradigm Press.Google Scholar
Haraway, D. J (1989). Primate Visions: Gender, Race, and Nature in the World of Modern Science. New York and London, Routledge.Google Scholar
Harnad, S. (1990). “The symbol grounding problem.” Physica D 42: 335–346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haugeland, J. (1978/98). The Plausibility of Cognitivism. Having Thought. J. Haugeland. Cambridge, MA., Harvard University Press.
Haugeland, J., Ed. (1981). Mind Design I. Cambridge, MA, A Bradford Book/MIT Press.Google Scholar
Haugeland, J. (1985). Artificial Intelligence: The Very Idea. Cambridge, MA, Bradford/MIT Press.Google Scholar
Haugeland, J., Ed. (1997). Mind Design II: Philosophy, Psychology, Artificial Intelligence. Cambridge, Mass., A Bradford Book/MIT Press.Google Scholar
Haugeland, J. (1998). Having Thought: Essays in the Metaphysics of Mind. Cambridge, Mass., Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Hayes, P. J (1973). Computation and Deduction. 2nd Mathematical Fooundations of Computer Science Symposium, Czechoslovakian Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
Hayes, P. J (1990). The Naive Physics Manifesto. The Philosophy of Artificial Intelligence. M. A. Boden. Oxford, UK, Oxford University Press: 171–205.Google Scholar
Hayles, N. K (1999). How we became posthuman: Virtual bodies in cybernetics, literature, and informatics. Chicago, Chicago University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayles, N. K (2005). “Computing the Human.” Theory, Culture & Society 22(1): 131–151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hebb, D. (1949). Organization of Behavior. New York, Wiley.Google Scholar
Hempel, C. G and , P. Oppenheim (1948). “Studies in the Logic of Explanation.” Philosophy of Science 15: 135–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hempel, C. G and , P. Oppenheim (1949). Studies in the Logic of Explanation. Aspects of Scientific Explanation. C. G. Hempel. New York, Macmillan.Google Scholar
Hennessy, J. L and , D. A. Patterson (1996). Computer Architecture: A Quantitative Approach. San Francisco, CA., Morgan Kaufmann Publishers.Google Scholar
Hertz, J., , A Krogh, et al. (1991). Introduction to the Theory of Neural Computation. Redwood City, CA, Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Hinton, G. and , T. Sejnowski (1986). Learning and relearning in Boltzmann machines. Parallel Distributed Processing. D. Rumelhart and J. McClelland. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press. 1: 282–317.Google Scholar
Hirsh-Pasek, K. and , R. M. Golinkoff (1996). The Origins of Grammar: Evidence from Early Language Comprehension. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hofstadter, D. R (1979). Gödel, Escher, Bach: an Eternal Golden Braid. New York, N.Y., Basic Books.Google Scholar
Hofstadter, D. R (1985). Metamagical Themas: Questing for the Essence of Mind and Pattern. New York, N.Y., Basic Books.Google Scholar
Hofstadter, D. R (1985b). Waking Up from the Boolean Dream, Or, Subcognition as Computation. Metamagical Themas: Questing for the Essence of Mind and Pattern. New York, Basic Books.Google Scholar
Hofstadter, D. R (1995). Fluid Concepts and Creative Analogies. New York, N.Y., Basic Books.Google Scholar
Hofstadter, D. R (1997). Le Ton beau de Marot: In Praise of the Music of Language. New York, Basic Books.Google Scholar
Hofstadter, D. R (1999). Moore's Law, Artificial Evolution, and the Fate of Humanity. Festschrift in Honor of John Holland's 70th Birthday. M. Mitchell.Google Scholar
Hofstadter, D. R (2001). Analogy as the Core of Cognition. Integration of Theory and Data from the Cognitive, Computational, and Neural Sciences. K. J. Holyoak, Gentner, D., and Kokinov, B. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hofstadter, D. R (2002). Staring Emmy Straight in the Eye – and Doing My Best Not to Flinch. Creativity, Cognition, and Knowledge. T. Dartnall. Westport: CN, Praeger: 67–100.Google Scholar
Hofstadter, D. R and , D. C. Dennett (1982). The Mind's I: Fantasies and Reflections on Self and Soul. New York, Basic Books.Google Scholar
Hofstadter, D. R and , D. Moser (1989). “To Err is Human: To Study Error-Making in Cognitive Science.” Michigan Quarterly Review 28(2): 185–215.Google Scholar
Holland, J. (1975). Adaptation in Natural and Artificial Systems. Ann Arbor, Michigan, University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Holland, J. (1995). Hidden Order: How Adaptation Builds Complexity. Reading, MA., Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Holland, J. H (1986). Escaping Brittleness: The Possibilities of General-purpose Learning Algorithms Applied to Parallel Rule-based Systems. Machine Learning: An Artificial Intelligence Approach. R. S. Michalski, J. G. Carbonell and T. M. Mitchell. Los Altos, CA., Morgan Kaufmann. II: 593–623.Google Scholar
Hölldobler, B. and , E. O. Wilson (1990). Ants. Cambridge, MA, Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holyoak, K. J and , P. Thagard (1995). Mental Leaps: Analogy in Creative Thought. Cambridge: MA, MIT Press/Bradford Books.Google Scholar
Hopfield, J. (1982). Neural Networks and Physical Systems with Emergent Collective Computational Abilities. National Academy of Sciences, USA79.Google ScholarPubMed
Hume, D. (1739). A Treatise of Human Nature. London, Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Hummel, J. E and , K. J. Holyoak (1997). “Distributed Representations of Structure: A Theory of analogical access and mapping.” Psychological Review 104: 427–466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hurlbert, A. and , T. Poggio (1988). “Making machines (and artificial intelligence).” Daedalus 117(1): 213–239.Google Scholar
Huskey, V. R and , H. D. Huskey (1980). “Lady Lovelace and Charles Babbage.” Annals of History of Computing 2: 299–329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in The Wild. Cambridge, Mass., MIT Press.Google Scholar
James, W. (1907/1998). Pragmatism and The Meaning of Truth. Cambridge, MA., Harvard University Press.
Johnson-Laird, P. (1988). The Computer and the Mind: An Introduction to Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Jordan, M. I (1986). Attractor Dynamics and Parallelism in a Connectionist Sequential Machine. 8th Annual Conference of Cognitive Science Society, Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Joy, B. (2000). “Why the future doesn't need us?Wired 8(4).Google Scholar
Joyce, J. (1996). When/why/of what is less more? Center for Cogntive Science. Edinburgh, University of Edinburgh. M.Sc. Dissertation.Google Scholar
Kail, R. (1984). The development of memory. New York, W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Kaipainen, M. and , R. F. Port (unpublished). Speech Cycling.
Kanerva, P. (1988). Sparse Distributed Memory. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: a developmental perspcetive on cognitive science. Cambridge: MA, MIT Press/Bradford Books.Google Scholar
Keller, E. F (2007). Booting up baby. Genesis Redux Essays in the History and Philosophy of Artificial Life. J. Riskin. Chicago, University of Chicago Press: 334–345.Google Scholar
Kelso, J. A. S. (1995). Dynamic Patterns: The Self-Organization of Brain and Behavior. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kennedy, J. and , R. C. Eberhart (2001). Swarm Intelligence. New York, Morgan Kaufmann.Google Scholar
Kintsch, W. (2000). “Metaphor comprehension: A computational theory.” Psyhonomic Bulletin and Review 7: 257–266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirsh, D. (1991). “Today the earwig, tomorrow man?Artificial Intelligence 47: 161–184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kling, R. (1980). “Social analyses of computing: Theoretical perspectives in recent empirical research.” Computing Surveys 12(1): 61–110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kling, R. (1997). Hopes and Horrors: Technological Utopianism and Anti-Utopianism in Narratives of Computerization. Computerization and Controversy: Value Conflicts and Social Choices. R. Kling. San Diego, CA, Academic Press: 40–58.Google Scholar
Kling, R. (1999). What is Social Informatics and Why Does it Matter? D-Lib Magazine. 5.
Kling, R. and , S. Iacono (1995). Computerization movements and the mobilization of support for computerization. Ecologies of knowledge. L. S. Star. New York, SUNY: 119–153.Google Scholar
Knuth, D. (1983). The Art of Computer Programming. Reading, MA., Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Koestler, A. (1964). The Act of Creation. New York: NY, Macmillan.Google Scholar
Kohler, R. (1999). Moral Economy, Material Culture, and Community in Drosophilia Genetics. The Science Studies Reader. M. Biagioli. New York and London, Routledge: 243–257.Google Scholar
Kolodner, J. L (1996). Making the Implicit Explicit: Clarifying the Principles of Case-Based Reasoning. Case-Based Reasoning: Experiences, Lessons, and Future Directions. D. B. Leake. Menlo Park, CA., AAAI Press/MIT Press: 349–370.Google Scholar
Kolodner, J. L and , D. Leake (1996). A Tutorial Introduction to Case-Based Reasoning Case-Based Reasoning: Experiences, Lessons and Future Directions. D. Leake. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press: 31–65.Google Scholar
Kowalski, R. A (1977). Algorithm=Logic+Control. Memorandum, Imperial College. London, UK.Google Scholar
Kuhn, T. (1962). The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. Chicago, University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kurzweil, R. (1999). The Age of Spiritual Machines: When Computers Exceed Human Intelligence. New York, N.Y., Viking.Google Scholar
Laham, D. (1997). Latent Semantic Analysis approaches to categorization. 19th annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, Fire and Dangerous Things. Chicago, University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landauer, T. K., Foltz, P. W., & Laham, D. (1998). “Introduction to Latent Semantic Analysis.” Discourse Processes 25: 259–284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landauer, T. K., Laham, D., & Foltz, P. W. (1998). “Learning human-like knowledge by Singular Value Decomposition: A progress report. In M. I. Jordan, M. J. Kearns & S. A. Solla (Eds.), Advances.”
Landauer, T. K and , S. T. Dumais (1997). “A solution to Plato's problem: the latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge.” Psychological Review 104: 211–240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landauer, T. K., , D Laham, et al. (1997). How well can passage meaning be derived without using word order? A comparison of Latent Semantic Analysis and humans. 19th annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Landes, D. (2000). Revolution in Time: Clocks and the Making of the Modern World. London, Viking Press.Google Scholar
Langton, C. G (1989). Artificial Life. Redwood City, CA., Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Lanier, J. (2000) One Half of A Manifesto: Why stupid software will save the future from neo-Darwinian machines. Wired: 8(12).Google Scholar
Larkin, J. and , H. A. Simon (1987). “Why a Diagram is (Sometimes) Worth Ten Thousand Words.” Cognitive Science 11(1): 65–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Latash, M. L (1993). Control of Human Movement. Champaign, IL., Human Kinetics: 1–48.Google Scholar
Latour, B. (1987). Science in Action. Cambridge, MA., Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Latour, B. (1993). We Have Never Been Modern. Cambridge: MA, Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Latour, B. (1999). Pandora's Hope: Essays on the Reality of Science Studies. Cambridge, MA., Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Latour, B. (2005). Reassembling the Social: An Introduction to Actor-Network Theory. Oxford, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Leake, D. B., Ed. (1996). Case-Based Reasoning: Experiences, Lessons, and Future Directions. Menlo Park, CA., AAAI Press/MIT Press.Google Scholar
Leclerc, J. and , F. Gosselin (2004). Processes of artistic creativity: The case of Isabelle Hayeur. Twenty-Seventh Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Mahwah: NJ, Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Leclerc, J. and , F. Gosselin (2005). ACE: A Model of the Cognitive Strategies of a Contemporary Artist. Twenty-Seventh Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Mahwah: NJ, Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lenat, D. B (1997a). From 2001 to 2001: Common Sense and the Mind of HAL. Hal's Legacy: 2001's Computer As Dream & Reality. D. G. Stork. Cambridge, Mass., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lenat, D. B (1997b). “The Dimensions of Context Space.” Retrieved April, 2003, from www.cyc.com.
Lenat, D. B (2002). Cyc Workshop. Presentation.Google Scholar
Lenat, D. B and , E. A. Feigenbaum (1991). “On the Thresholds of Knowledge.” Artificial Intelligence 47(1–3): 185–250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lenat, D. B and , R. V. Guha (1990). Building Large Knowledge-Based Systems. Reading, Mass., Addison Wesley.Google Scholar
Lenat, D. B., , M Prakash, et al. (1986). “Using common-sense knowledge to overcome brittleness and knowledge acquisition bottlenecks.” AI Magazine 7: 65–85.Google Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lewis, D. (1971). “Analog and Digital.” Nous 5(3): 321–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewontin, R. C (1998). The Evolution of Cognition: Questions We Will Never Answer. An Invitation to Cognitive Science: Methods, Models, and Conceptual Issues. D. Scarborough and S. Sternberg. Cambridge, MA., A Bradford Book/MIT Press. 4: 107–132.Google Scholar
Lloyd, E. A (1994). The Structure and Confirmation of Evolutionary Theory. Princeton, N.J., Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Llyod, G. (1999). Science in Antiquity: The Greek and Chinese Cases and Their Relevance to the Problems of Culture and Cognition. Science Studies Reader. M. Biagioli. New York and London, Routledge: 302–316.Google Scholar
Loy, G. (1989). Composing with Computers: A Survey of Some Compositional Formalisms and Music Programming Languages. Current Directions in Computer Music Research. M. Mathews and J. R. Pierce. Cambridge: MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mackay, W. A., D. J. Crammond, et al. (1986). “Measurements of human forearm viscoelasticity.” Journal of Biomechanics 19: 231–238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manna, Z. and , R. Waldinger (1985). The Logical Basis for Computer Programming. Reading, Mass., Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Maris, M. and , R. T. Boekhorst (1996). Exploiting physical constraints: Heap formation through behavioral error in a group of robots. IEEE/RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems.Google Scholar
Marr, D. (1982). Vision. San Francisco, CA, Freeman.Google Scholar
Marshall, J. and , D. R. Hofstatder (1997). “The Metacat Project: a self-watching model of analogy-making.” Cognitive Studies: The Bulletin of Japanese Cogn.Sci.Soc. 4(4).Google Scholar
Marshall, J. B (1999). Metacat: A Self-Watching Cognitive Architecture for Analogy-Making and High-Level Perception. Computer Science Dept. Bloomington, Indiana University.Google Scholar
Mataric, M. J (1992). “Intergation of reprenstation into goal-driven behavior-based robots.” IEEE Transactions on Robotics and Automation 8(3): 304–312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mazlish, B. (1993). The Fourth Discontinuity: the co-evolution of humans and machines. New Haven, Yale University Press.Google Scholar
McCarthy, J. (1980). “Circumscription – A Form of Non-Monotonic Reasoning.” Artificial Intelligence 13: 27–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCarthy, J. (1986). “Applications of Circumscription to Formalizing Commonsense Knowledge.” Artificial Intelligence 28(1): 89–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCarthy, J. and , P. J. Hayes (1969). Some Philosophical Problems from the Standpoint of Artificial Intelligence. Machine Intelligence. B. M. D. Michie. Edinburgh, UK, Edinburgh University Press. 4.Google Scholar
McClelland, J. and , D. Rumelhart (1986). Parallel Distributed Processing. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
McCorduck, P. (1979). Machines who think. San Francisco, CA, W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
McCorduck, P. (1991). Aaron's Code: meta-art, artificial intelligence and the work of Harold Cohen. New York, Freeman.Google Scholar
McCulloch, W. S and , W. H. Pitts (1943). “A Logical Calculus of the Ideas Immanent in Nervous Activity.” Bulletin of Mathematical Biophysics 5: 115–133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDermott, D. (1976). “Artificial intelligence meets natural stupidity.” SIGART Newsletter 57: 4–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDermott, D. (1982). “A Temporal Logic for Reasoning about Processes and Plans.” Cognitive Science 6: 101–155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDermott, D. (1987). “A Critique of Pure Reason.” Computational Intelligence 3(1): 151–160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonough, R. (2002). Emergence and Creativity. Creativity, Cognition, and Knowledge. T. Dartnall. Westport: CN, Praeger: 282–301.Google Scholar
McGraw, G. and , D. R. Hofstadter (2002). Letter Spirit: Perception and Creation of Diverse Alphabetic Styles. Creativity, Cognition, and Knowledge. T. Dartnall. Westport: CN, Praeger: 251–266.Google Scholar
McLeod, P., , K Plunkett, et al. (1998). Introduction to Connectionist Modelling of Cognitive Processes. New York, Oxford Univeristy Press.Google Scholar
McNeal, C. (1998). The Medieval Art of Love. New York, N.Y., Gaimann & King Ltd.Google Scholar
Mead, G. H (1934). Mind, Self, and Society from the Standpoint of a Social Behaviorist. Chicago, Chicago Univeristy Press.Google Scholar
Meglicki, Z. (2000). “Introduction to Quantum Computing.” Retrieved May 1, 2003, from http://www.ovpit.indiana.edu/B679.
Menzel, P. and F. D'Aluisio (2000). Robo sapiens. Cambridge: MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Minsky, M. (1975). A framework for representing knowledge. The Psychology of Computer Vision. P. H. Winston. New York, McGraw-Hill: 211–277.Google Scholar
Minsky, M. (1985). The Society of Mind. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Minsky, M. (1995). “Future of AI Technology.” Toshiba Review 47(7).Google Scholar
Minsky, M. (2005). The Emotion Machine. Accessed November 28, 2006 from: web.media.mit.edu/∼minsky/.
Mitchell, M. (1993). Analogy-Making as Perception: A Computer Model. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mitchell, M. (1996). An Introduction to Genetic Algorithms. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mitchell, M. (1998). “Theories of structure versus theories of change.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21(5): 645–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moravec, H. (1998). Mind Children: The Future of Robot and Human Intelligence. Cambridge and New York, Cambirdge University Press.Google Scholar
Morris, W. C., G. W. Cottrell, et al. (1999). “A Connectionist Simulation of the Empirircal Acquisition of Grammatical Relations.” Cognitive Science.Google Scholar
Mortensen, C. (1989). Mental Images: Should Cognitive Science Learn From Neurophysiology. Computers, Brains, and Minds: Essays in Cognitive Scienc. P. Slezak and W. R. Albury. Dodrecht, Kluwer Academic Publishers.Google Scholar
Nagel, T. (1974). “What is it like to be a bat?Philosophical Review 4: 435–450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neumaier, O. (1987). A Wittgensteinian View of Artificial Intelligence. Aritificial Intelligence: The Case Against. R. Born. London, Croom Helm: 132–174.Google Scholar
Newborn, M. (1997). Kasparov versus Deep Blue: Computer Chess Comes of Age. New York, N.Y., Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newell, A. (1990). Unified Theories of Cognition. Cambridge, MA., Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Newell, A., J. C. Shaw, et al. (1960). Report on a General Problem-Solving Program. Proceedings of the International Conference on Information Processing, Paris.Google Scholar
Newell, A. and , H. A. Simon (1961). GPS, a program that simulates human thought. Lernende Automaten. H. Billing. Munich, Oldenbourg: 109–124.Google Scholar
Newell, A. and , H. A. Simon (1976). “Computer Science as Empirical Inquiry.” Communications of the ACM 19(3): 113–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newport, E. L (1990). “Maturational constraints on language learning.” Cognitive Science 14: 11–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noble, D. F (1977). America by Design: Science, Technology, and the Rise of Corporate capitalism. Oxford, UK, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Noble, D. F (1984). Forces of Production: A Social History of Industrial Automation. New York, NY, Alfred A. Knopf.Google Scholar
Norton, A. (1995). Dynamics: An Introduction. Mind as Motion. R. F. Port and T. v. Gelder. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press: 45–68.Google Scholar
Oyama, S. (2000). Evolution's Eye: A Systems View of the Biology-Culture Divide. Durham, NC, Duke University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peirce, C. S (1905/1940). Critical Common-Sensism. The Philosophy of Peirce: Selected Writings. J. Buchler. New York: Harcourt, Brace & Company: 290–301.
Petitot, J., F. J. Varela, et al. (1999). Naturalizing Phenomenology: Issues in Contemporary Phenomenology and Cogntive Science. Stanford, CA., Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Petroski, H. (1989). The Pencil: A History of Design and Circumstances. New York, Knopf.Google Scholar
Pfeifer, R. and , C. Scheier (1999). Understanding Intelligence. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1952). The Origins of Intelligence in Children. New York, International University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. (1984). Language Learnability and Language Development. Cambridge, MA., Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1991). “Rules of Language.” Science 253: 530–535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinker, S. and , A. Prince (1988). “On Language and Connectionism: Analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition.” Cognition 28: 73–193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plate, T. (1995). Holographic Reduced Representations. Department of Computer Science. Toronto, University of Toronto.Google Scholar
Plunkett, K. and , P. Juola (1999). “A Connectionist Model of English Past Tense and Plural Morphology.” Cognitive Science 23(4): 463–490.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plunkett, K. and , V. Marchman (1991). “U-shaped learning and frequency effects in a multi-layered perceptron: Implications for child language acquisition.” Cognition 38: 43–102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poincaré, H. (1952). Science and Hypothesis. New York, Dover Publications.Google Scholar
Poincaré, H. (1982). The Foundations of Science: Science and hypothesis, the value of science, science and method. Washington, DC, University Press of America.Google Scholar
Polanyi, M. (1958). Personal Knowledge: Towards a Postcritical Philosophy. Chicago, Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Popper, K. R (1972). Two Faces of Common Sense. Objective Knowledge: An Evolutionary Approach. Oxford, UK, Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Port, R. F and T. van Gelder (1995). Mind as Motion: Explorations in the Dynamics of Cognition. Cambridge, MA., A Bradford Book/MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pratt, V. (1994). “Visit to Cyc.” Retrieved Accessed July 24, 2002, from http://boole.stanford.edu/pratt.html.
Preskill, J. (1997). “Lecture Notes for Physics 229: Quantum Information and Computation.” Retrieved Accessed May 4 2000, from www.theory.caltech.edu/people/preskill/ph229.
Prinz, J. J and , L. W. Barsalou (2002). Acquisition and Productivity in Perceptual Symbol Systems: An Account of Mundane Creativity. Creativity, Cognition, and Knowledge. T. Dartnall. Westport: CN, Praeger: 105–126.Google Scholar
Pulvermüller, F. (1999). “Words in the Brain's Language.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22: 253–279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnam, H. (1975). Philosophical Papers: Mind, Language and Reality. Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnam, H. (1990). Realism with a Human Face. Cambridge, MA., Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Pylyshyn, Z. W (1984). Computation and Cognition: Toward a Foundation for Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA, Bradford/MIT Press.Google Scholar
Quartz, S. R and , T. J. Sejnowski (1997). “The Neural Basis of Cognitive Development: A Constructivist Manifesto.” Brain and Behavioral Sciences 20: 537–596.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quine, W. V and , J. S. Ullian (1978). The Web of Belief. New York, McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Reeke, G. N and , G. M. Edelman (1988). “Real brains and artificial intelligence.” Daedalus 117(1): 143–173.Google Scholar
Regier, T. and , L. Carlson (2001). “Grounding spatial language in perception: An empirical and computational investigation.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 130: 273–298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rehling, J. (2002). Results in Letter Spirit Project. Creativity, Cognition, and Knowledge. T. Dartnall. Westport: CN, Praeger: 272–278.Google Scholar
Reid, T. (1785/1969). Essays on the Intellectual Powers of Man. Cambridge, MA., The MIT Press.
Restivo, S. (2001). Bringing Up and Booting Up: Social Theory and the Emergence of Socially Intelligent Robots. IEEE Conference on Robotics and Cybernetics, IEEE.Google Scholar
Rhode, D. L. T. and , D. C. Plaut (1999). “Language acquisition in the absence of explicit negative evidence: how important is starting small?Cognition 72: 67–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riesbeck, C. and , R. C. Schank (1989). Inside Case-based Reasoning. Hillsdale, N.J., Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Riesbeck, C. K (1996). What Next? The Future of Case-Based Reasoning in Post-Modern AI. Case-Based Reasoning: Experiences, Lessons, and Future. D. B. Leake. Menlo park, CA., AAAI Press/MIT Press: 371–388.Google Scholar
Risan, L. (1997). Artificial Life: A technoscience leaving modernity? An Anthropology of subjects and objects. Accessed April 19, 2003 from: http://anthrobase.com/Txt/R/Risan_L_05.htm.
Riskin, J. (2007). Genesis Redux: Essays in the History and Philosophy of Artificial Life. Chicago, University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, S. and , H. Pashler (2000). “How persuasive is a good fit? A comment on theory testing.” Psychological Review 107: 358–367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rochester, N., , J Holland, et al. (1956). “Test on a cell assembly theory of the action of the brain, using a large digital computer.” IRE Transactions on Information Theory IT-2: 80–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenblatt, P. (1958). “The Perceptron: A probabilistic model for information storage and organization in the brain.” Psychological Review 65: 386–408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roszak, T. (1994). The Cult of Information: A Neo-Luddite Treatise on High-Tech, Artificial Intelligence, and The True Art of Thinking. Berkeley, CA, University of California Press.Google Scholar
Rouse, J. (1987). Knowledge and Power: Toward a Political Philosophy of Science. Ithaca: N.Y., Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Rouse, J. (1999). Understanding Scientific Practices: Cultural Studies of Science as a Philosophical Program. The Science Studies Reader. M. Biagioli. New York and London, Routledge: 442–456.Google Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E (1989). The Architecture of Mind: A Connectionist Approach. Mind Design II: Philosophy, Psychology, Artificial Intelligence. J. Haugeland. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press: 205–232.Google Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E (1997). The Architecture of Mind: A Connectionist Approach. Mind Design II. J. Haugeland. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press: 205–232.Google Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E., G. E. Hinton, et al. (1986). Learning internal representations by error propagation. Parallel Distributed Processing. D. E. R. a. J. L. McClelland. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press. 2.Google Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E and , D. Norman (1982). “Simulating a Skilled Typist: A Study of Skilled Cognitive–Motor Performance.” Cognitive Science 6(1): 1–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E and , D. Zipser (1985). “Feature discovery by competitive learning.” Cognitive Science 9: 75–112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, S. J and , P. Norvig (1995). Artificial Intelligence: A Modern Approach, Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Saffran, J. R., R. N. Aslin, et al. (1996). “Statistical learning by 8-month old infants.” Science 274(5294): 1926–1928.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Santambrogio, M. and , P. Violi (1988). Introduction. Meaning and Mental Representation. M. Santambrogio, P. Violi and U. Eco. Bloomington, IN, Indiana University Press: 3–23.Google Scholar
Scassellati, B. (1998). Imitation and Mechanisms of Joint Attention: A Developmental Structure for Building Social Skills on a Humanoid Robot. Computation for Metaphors, Analogy and Agents. C. Nehaniv, Springer-Verlag. 1562.Google Scholar
Scassellati, B. (1999). A Binocular, Foveated Active Vision System, MIT, Artificial Intelligence Laboratory.
Scassellati, B. (2000). Investigating Models of Social Development Using a Humanoid Robot. Biorobotics. B. Webb and T. Consi. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Schaffer, S. (1994) Babbage's Intelligence: Calculating Engines and the Factory System. Accessed April 14, 2004 from: http://www.wmin.ac.uk/mad/schaffer/schaffer01.html.
Schank, R. C (1972). “Conceptual Dependency: A theory of natural language understanding.” Cognitive Psychology 3(4): 552–631.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schank, R. C (1982). Dynamic Memory: A Theory of Learning in Computers and People. Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Schank, R. C (1986). Explanation Patterns. Hillsdale, N.J., Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc.Google Scholar
Schank, R. C (1995). Information Is Surprises. The Third Culture: Beyond the Scientific Revolution. J. Brockman. New York, Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
Schank, R. C and , R. P. Abelson (1977). Scripts, Plans, Goals, and Understanding. Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc.Google Scholar
Schank, R. C and , L. Birnbaum (1984). Memory, Meaning, and Syntax. Talking Minds: The Study of Language in Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press: 209–251.Google Scholar
Schank, R. C and , A. Kass (1988). Knowledge Representation in People and Machines. Meaning and Mental Representations. U. Eco, M. Santambrogio and P. Violi. Bloomington and Indianapolis, Indiana University Press: 181–200.Google Scholar
Schank, R. C., , A Kass, et al. (1994). Inside Case-based Explanation. Hillsdale, N.J., Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc.Google Scholar
Schank, R. C and , D. B. Leake (2001). Natural Language Processing: Models of Roger Schank and His Students. Encycopedia of Cognitive Science.Google Scholar
Schank, R. C and , C. Riesbeck (1981). Inside Computer Understanding: Five Programs with Miniatures. Hillsdale, NJ., Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Scheutz, M. (2002). Agents with or without Emotions? Proceedings of the Fifteenth International Florida Artificial Intelligence Research Society Conference. 89–93.
Scheutz, M., , P Schemerhorn, et al. (2007). “First Steps toward Natural Human-Like HRI.” Autonomous Robots 22: 411–423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searle, J. (1992). Rediscovery of The Mind. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Searle, J. (2002). I Married A Computer. Are We Spiritual Machines? Ray Kurzweil vs. the Critics of Strong A.I. J. W. Richards. Seattle, WA., Discovery Institute.Google Scholar
Searle, J. R (1980). Minds, Brains, and Programs. Mind Design II. J. Haugeland. Cambridge, Mass., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Searle, J. R (1984). Minds, Brains, and Science. Cambridge, MA., Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Seidenberg, M. S and , M. C. MacDonald (1999). “A Probabilistic Constraints Approach to Language Acquisition and Processing.” Cognitive Science 23(4): 569–588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sejnowski, T. and , C. Rosenberg (1987). NETtalk: a parallel network that learns to read aloud, Technical Report JHU/EECS-86/01, Johns Hopkins University.
Selfridge, O. (1959). Pandemonium: A paradigm for learning. Symposium on the Mechanization of Thought, London, HMSO.Google Scholar
Shanker, S. G (1987). AI at the Crossroads. The Question of Artificial Intelligence. B. Bloomfield. London, Croom Helm: 1–58.Google Scholar
Shannon, C. (1950). “Programming a computer for playing chess.” Philosophical Magazine 41: 256–275.Google Scholar
Shapin, S. (1994). A Social History of Truth: Civility and Science in Seventeenth-Century England. Chicago: IL, University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Shimojima, A. (1996). On the Efficacy of Representation. Philosophy. Bloomington, Indiana University.Google Scholar
Shultz, T. R and , W. C. Schmidt (1991). A Cascade-Correlation Model of Balance Scale Phenomenon. 13th Annual Conference of Cognitive Science Society, Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Simon, H. A (1969). The Sciences of The Artificial. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Simon, H. A (1995). “Artificial Intelligence: an empirical science.” Artificial Intelligence 77(1): 95–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F (1985). “Cognitive Science and Behaviourism.” British Journal of Psychology 76: 33–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sloman, A. (2001). “Beyond Shallow Models of Emotion.” Cognitive Processing 2(1): 177–198.Google Scholar
Sloman, A. (2005a). AI in a New Millenium: Obstacles and Opportunities. IJCAI. Birmingham, UK.Google Scholar
Sloman, A. (2005b). Do Machines, Natural or Artificial, Really Need Emotions? NWO Cognition Program. Utrecht.Google Scholar
Smith, B. C (1991). “The Owl and the Electric Encyclopedia.” Artificial Intelligence 47(1–3): 252–288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, B. C (1996). The Origin of Objects. Cambridge, MA, The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Smith, B. C (1997) One Hundred Billion Lines of C++. Accessed October 15, 2005 from: http://www.ageofsignificance.org/people/bcsmith/papers/smith-billion.html.
Smith, B. C (1999). Situatedness. MIT Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science (MITECS). Cambridge, MA., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Smith, B. C (2000). Indiscrete Affairs. Vital Signs: Cultural Perspectives on Coding Life and Vitalizing Code. L. Suchman and J. Fujimura. (in press).Google Scholar
Smith, B. C (2002). Reply to Dennett. Philosophy of Mental Representation. H. Clapin. Oxford, Oxford University Press: 237–266.Google Scholar
Smith, B. C (forthcoming). The Age of Significance: Introduction. Cambidge, MA, The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Smolensky, P. (1988). “On the proper treatment of connectionism.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11: 1–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smolensky, P. (1999). “Grammar-based connectionist approaches to language.” Cognitive Science 23: 589–613.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Star, L. S., Ed. (1995). Ecologies of Knowledge Work and Politics in Science and Technology. SUNY Series in Science, Technology, and Society. New York: NY, SUNY.Google Scholar
Steedman, M. (1999). “Connectionist Sentence Processing in Perspective.” Cognitive Science 23(4): 615–634.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steels, L. (1990). Cooperation between distributed agents through self-organization. Decentralized AI. Y. Demazeau and J. P. Muller. Amsterdam, North-Holland: 175–196.Google Scholar
Stewart, J. (1992). Life=Cognition: The epistemological and ontological significance of Artificial Life. First European Conference on Artificial Life, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Suchman, L. (1987). Plans and Situated Actions. Cambridge, UK., Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Suchman, L. A (2007). Human-Machine Reconfigurations. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Thelen, E. and , L. B. Smith (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. K. R., D. L. Oden, et al. (1997). “Language-Native Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) Judge Relations Between Relations in a Conceptual Matching-to-sample Task.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 23: 31–43.Google Scholar
Toole, B. A (1992). Ada, the Enchantress of Numbers. Mill Valley: CA, Strawberry Press.Google Scholar
Townsend, J. T and , J. Busemeyer (1995). Dyamic Representation of Decision-Making. Mind as Motion. R. F. a. T. v. G. Port. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press: 101–119.Google Scholar
Turing, A. M (1936). On Computable Numbers, with an Application to Entscheidungsproblem. London Mathematical Society.Google Scholar
Turing, A. M (1950). “Computing Machinery and Intelligence.” Mind 59: 433–460.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turing, A. M (1953). Digital Computers Applied to Games. Faster than Thought. B. V. Bowden. London, UK, Pitman.Google Scholar
Turkle, S. (1985). The Second Self: Computers and The Human Spirit. New York, NY, Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
Turkle, S. (1995). Life on the Screen: Identity in the Age of Internet. New York, N.Y., Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
van Gelder, T. (1998). “The dynamical hypothesis in cognitive science.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21(5): 615–628.Google ScholarPubMed
Varela, F. J., , E Thompson, et al. (1991). The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience. Cambridge, Mass., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Foerster, H. (1970). Thoughts and Notes on Cognition. Cognition: A multiple view. P. L. Garvin. New York, Spartan Books: 25–48.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. (1986). Thought and Language. Cambridge, Mass., MIT Press Translations.Google Scholar
Weizenbaum, J. (1976). Computer Power and Human Reason. San Francisco, CA, W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Wellman, B. (2001). “Computer Networks as Social Networks.” Science 293: 2031–2034.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wellman, H. M and , S. A. Gellman (1992). “Cognitive Development: Foundational theories of core domains.” Annual Review of Psychology 43: 337–375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiener, N. (1948). Cybernetics. New York, Wiley.Google ScholarPubMed
Wills, L. M and , J. L. Kolodner (1996). Toward More Creative Case-Based Design Systems. Case-Based Reasoning: Experiences, Lessons, and Future Directions. D. B. Leake. Menlo Park, CA., AAAI Press/MIT Press: 81–91.Google Scholar
Wilson, E. O (1971). The Insect Society. Cambridge, MA., Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Winner, L. (1986). The Whale and The Reactor: A Search for Limits in an Age of High Technology. Chicago, IL, University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Winograd, T. (1972). Understanding Natural Language. New York, Academic Press.Google Scholar
Winograd, T. and , F. Flores (1986). Understanding Computers and Cognition: A New Foundation for Design. Norwood, N.J., Ablex.Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical Investigations. New York, Blackwell.Google Scholar
Woods, W. A (1975). What's in a link? Foundations for semantic networks. Representation and Understanding: Studies in Cognitive Science. D. G. Bobrow and A. M. Collins. New York, Academic Press: 35–82.Google Scholar
Woolgar, S. (1985). “Why Not a Sociology of Machines? The Case of Sociology and Artificial Intelligence.” Sociology 19(4): 557–572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woolgar, S. (1987). Reconstructing Man and Machine: A Note on Sociological Critiques of Cognitivism. The Social Construction of Technological Systems. T. H. W. Bijker, and T. Pinch. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press: 311–328.Google Scholar
Woolgar, S. (1995). Representation, Cognition, and Self: What Hope for the Integration of Psychology and Sociology? Ecologies of Knowledge: Work and Politics in Science and Technology. S. L. Star. New York State University of New York Press: 154–179.Google Scholar
Zipser, M. (2002). Introduction to the Theory of Computation. Boston, MA, PWS Publishing.Google Scholar
Agre, P. E. (1995). “The Soul Gained and Lost: Artificial Intelligence as A Philosophical Project.” Stanford Humanity Review 4(2).Google Scholar
Agre, P. E. (1997a). Computation and Human Experience. Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Agre, P. E. (1997b). Toward a Critical Technical Practice: Lessons Learned in Trying to Reform AI. Bridging the Great Divide: Social Science, Technical Systems, and Cooperative Work. G. Bowker, L. S. Star, B. Turner and L. Gasser. New York, NY, Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Agre, P. E. (2002). The Practical Logic of Computer Work. Computationalism: New Directions. M. Scheutz. Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Agre, P. E. and , D. Chapman (1987). Pengi: An implementation of a theory of activity. Sixth National Conference on Artificial Intelligence, San Mateo, CA., Morgan Kaufmann.Google Scholar
Agre, P. E. and , I. D. Horswill (1992). Cultural Support for Improvisation. Tenth National Conference on Artificial Intelligence, Los Altos, CA., Morgan Kaufmann.Google Scholar
Agre, P. E. and , D. Schuler (1997). Reinventing Technology, Rediscovering Community: Critical Explorations of Computing as a Social Practice. New York, NY, Ablex.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. A (1995). An Introduction to Neural Networks. Cambridge, MA., A Bradford Book/ MIT Press.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R (1993). Rules of the Mind. Hillsdale: NJ, Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Athanasiou, T. (1985). Artificial Intelligence: cleverly disguised politics. Compulsive Technology. T. Solomonides and L. Levidow. London, UK, Free Association Books: 13–35.Google Scholar
Axelrod, R. and , L. Tesfatsion (2007). A Guide for Newcomers to Agent-Based Modeling in the Social Sciences. Handbook of Computational Economics, Vol. 2: Agent-Based Computational Economics. K. L. Judd and L. Tesfatsion. Amsterdam, North-Holland, Available at http://www.econ.iastate.edu/tesfatsi/abmread.htm.Google Scholar
Bajscy, R. and , E. W. Large (1999). “When and Where Will AI Meet Robotics.” AI Magazine 20(3): 57–65.Google Scholar
Ballard, D. H. (1997). An Introduction to Natural Computation. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Barad, K. (2003). “Posthumanist Performativity: Toward an Understanding of How Matter Comes to Matter.” Journal of Women in Culture and Society 28(3): 801–831.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A Study in Experimental and Social Psychology. New York, Macmillan.Google Scholar
Barwise, J. and , J. Etchemendy (1994). Hyperproof. New York, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bechtel, W. (1998). “Representations and Cognitive Explanations: Assessing the Dynamicist's Challenge in Cognitive Science.” Cognitive Science 22(3): 295–318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bechtel, W. and , R. C. Richardson (1992). Discovering Complexity: Decomposition and localization as strategies in scientific research. Princeton, NJ., Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Becker, H. (1982). The Art Worlds. Berkeley: CA, University of California Press.Google Scholar
Becker, H. S (forthcoming). What About Mozart? What About Murder? Accessed January 21, 2007 from: http://home.earthlink.net/∼hsbecker/mozart.htm.
Biagioli, M., Ed. (1999). Science Studies Reader. New York and London, Routledge.Google Scholar
Bijker, W. E., , T. E. Hughes, et al., Eds. (1997). The Social Construction of Technological Systems: new Directions in the Sociology and History of Technology. Cambridge, MA, The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bloomfield, B. P. (1987). The Culture of Artificial Intelligence. The Question of Artificial Intelligence. B. Bloomfield. London, UK, Croom Helm: 59–105.Google Scholar
Bobrow, D. G. (1964). Natural Language Input for a Computer Problem-Solving System. Report TR-1, Project MAC, MIT, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Boden, M. A. (1970). “Intentionality and Physical Systems.” Philosophy of Science 37: 200–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boden, M. A., Ed. (1990). The Creative Mind: Myths and Mechanisms. New York, Basic Books.Google Scholar
Boden, M. A (1994). Creativity and Computers. Artificial Intelligence and Creativity: An Interdisciplinary Approach. T. H. Dartnall. Dordrecht, Kluwer Academic Publishers.Google Scholar
Bolter, D. (1984). Turing's Man: Western Culture in the Computer Age. Chapel Hill, NC, University of North Carolina Press.Google Scholar
Born, R., Ed. (1987). Artificial Intelligence: The Case Against. London, Croom Helm.Google Scholar
Braitenberg, V. (1984). Vehicles: Experiments in Synthetic Psychology. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Breazeal, C. and , B. Scassellati (2000). Challenges in Building Robots that Imitate People. Imitation in Animals and Artifacts. K. Dautenhahn and C. Nehaniv. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Brick, T. and , M. Scheutz (2007). Incremental Natural Language Processing for HRI. HRI'07, Arlington, VA.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooks, R., , C. Breazeal, et al. (1998). The Cog Project: Building a Humanoid Robot. Computation for Metaphors, Analogy and Agents. C. Nehaniv, Springer-Verlag. LNCS1562.Google Scholar
Brooks, R. A (1986). “A Robust Layered Control System for a Mobile Robot.” IEEE Transactions on Robotics and Automation 2(1): 14–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooks, R. A (1991/1997). Intelligence without representation. Mind Design II. J. Haugeland. Cambridge, MA, The MIT Press. 395–420.
Brooks, R. A (1999). Cambrian Intelligence: The Early History of the New AI. Cambridge, MA., A Bradford Book/MIT Press.Google Scholar
Brooks, R. A (2002a). Flesh and Machines: How Robots Will Change Us. New York, Pantheon Books.Google Scholar
Brooks, R. A (2002b). “Lord of the Robots.” Technology Review 105(3): 78–83.Google Scholar
Brooks, R. A and , L. A. Stein (1994). “Building Brains for Bodies.” Autonomous Robots 1(1): 7–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruce, B. (1999) Educational Reform: How Does Technology Affect Educational Change? Journal of Adolescent and Adult Education. Accessed Nov. 14, 2006 from: http://www.readingonline.org/electronic/jaal/9-99_Column.html.Google Scholar
Brucell, V., , M. Burton, et al. (1993). “Sex Discrimination: how do we tell the difference between male and female faces?Perception 22: 131–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buchanan, B. G., Sutherland, G. L., et al. (1969). Heurstic DENDRAL: A program for generating explanatory hypothesis in organic chemistry. Machine Intelligence. B. Meltzer, Michie, D., and Swann, M. Edinburgh, Scotland, Edinburgh University Press. 4: 209–254.Google Scholar
Castañeda, C. and , L. Suchman (2005). Robots Visions. Accessed April 25, 2007 from:
http://www.comp.lancs.ac.uk/sociology/papers/***.pdf
Castells, M. (1996). The Rise of The Network Society. Malden: MA, Blackwell Publishers.Google Scholar
Castells, M. (2001). The Internet Galaxy: Reflections on the Internet, Business, and Society. Oxford: UK, Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chalmers, D. J., R. M. French, et al. (1992). “High-level Perception, Representation, and Analogy.” Journal of Experimental and Theoretical Artificial Intelligence 4(3): 185–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chemero, A. (2000). “Anti-Representationalism and the Dynamical Stance.” Philosophy of Science 67: 626–647.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic Structures. The Hague, Mouton.Google Scholar
Chrisley (2002). Artificial Intelligence. The Oxford Companion to the Mind. R. Gregory. Oxford, Oxford University Press.
Christiansen, M. H and , N. Chater (1999). “Connectionist Natural Language Processing: The State of the Art.” Cognitive Science 23(4): 417–437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Churchland (1997). On the Nature of Theories: A Neurocomputational Perspective. Mind Design II. J. Haugeland. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press: 251–292.
Churchland, P. M (1989). On the Nature of Theories: A Neurocomputatoinal Perspective. Mind Design II. J. Haugeland. Cambridge, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clancey, W. J (1997). Situated Cognition: On Human Knowledge and Computer Representations. Cambridge, UK, Univeristy of Cambridge.Google Scholar
Clapin, H., Ed. (2002). Philosophy of Mental Representation. Oxford, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (1997). Being There: Putting Brain, Body, and World Together Again. Cambridge, MA, A Bradford Book/MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2001). Mindware. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2003). Natural-Born Cyborgs: Minds, Technologies, and the Future of Human Intelligence. Oxford, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V (1993). The Lexicon in Acquisition. Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clynes, M. E and N. S. Kline (1960). “Cyborgs and Space.” Astronautics (Sept. 1960): 33–42.Google Scholar
Cohen, D. I. A. (1997). Theory of Computer Theory. New York, John Wiley and Sons.Google Scholar
Cohen, H. (1995). “The Further Exploits of AARON, Painter.” Stanford Humanities Review 4(2).Google Scholar
Cohen, P. R and , A. E. Howe (1988). “How Evaluation Guides AI Research.” AI Magazine Winter 1988: 35–43.Google Scholar
Collins, G. (1989). Coach. Inside Case-based Reasoning. C. Riesbeck and R. C. Schank. Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Collins, H. (1990). Artificial Experts: Social Knowledge and Intelligent Machines. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Collins, H. (1992). “Hubert L. Dreyfus, Forms of Life, and a Simple Test for Machine Intelligence.” Social Studies of Science 22: 726–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collins, H. and , M. Kusch (1998). The Shape of Actions: What Humans and Machines Can Do. Cambridge, MA., MIT press.Google Scholar
Collins, H. and , T. Pinch (1998). The Golem: What everyone should know about science. Cambirdge, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cope, D. (1991). Computers and Musical Style. Madison: WI, A-R Editions, Inc.Google Scholar
Cope, D. (2001). Virtual Music: Computer Synthesis of Musical Style. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Copeland, J. B (1997). “CYC: A case Study in Ontological Engineering.” Retrieved April 2003, from http://phil.indiana.edu/ejap/1997.Spring/copeland976.2.html.
Cottrell, G. W., C. Padgett, et al. (2000). Is all face processing holistic? The View from UCSD, ScientificCommons.
Crevier, D. (1993). The Tumultuous History of The Search for Artificial Intelligence. New York, NY, Basic Books.Google Scholar
Cussins, A. (1998). Nonconceptual Content of Experience and Conceptual Content of Thought. Unpublished manuscript.
Cussins, A. (in press). “Content, Embodiment and Objectivity – The Theory of Cognitive Trails.” Unpublished manuscript.
Dartnall, T., Ed. (2002). Creativity, Cognition, and Knowledge. Perspectives on Cognitive Science. Westport: CN, Praeger.Google Scholar
Davidson, A. (1990). Closing Up the Corpses. Meaning and Method. G. Boulos. Cambridge: MA, Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Davis, M. (1988). Mathematical Logic and the Origin of Modern Computers. The Universal Turing Machine: A Half-Century Survey. R. Herken. Oxford, UK, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Day, R. (2005). “Clearing up “implicit knowledge”: Implications for Knowledge Management, information science, psychology, and social epistemology.” Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology 56(6): 630–635.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dell, G. S (1986). “A spreading-activation theory of retreival in sentence production.” Psychological Review 93: 283–321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dell, G. S., , M. F. Schwartz, , N. Martin, , E. M Saffran, , D. A Gagnon (1997). “Lexical access in aphasic and nonaphasic speakers.” Psychological Review 104(801–939).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dell, G. S., F. Chang, Z. M. Griffin (1999). “Connectionist models of language production: Lexical access and grammatical encoding.” Cognitive Science 23: 517–542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dell, G. S., C. Juliano, et al. (1993). “Structure and content in language production: A theory of frame constraints in phonological speech errors.” Cognitive Science 17: 149–195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeLong, H. (1970). A Profile of Mathematical Logic. Reading, MA, Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. (1998). “Revolution, no! Reform.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21(5): 636–637.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennett, D. C (1990). Cognitive Wheels: The Frame Problem of AI. The Philosophy of Artificial Intelligence. M. A. Boden. Oxford, UK, Oxford University Press: 147–170.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C (1991). “Real Patterns.” Journal of Philosophy 88(1): 27–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennett, D. C (1995). Darwin's Dangerous Idea. New York, Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C (1997). True Believers: Intentional Strategy and Why It Works. Mind Design II. J. Haugeland. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press: 57–79.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C (1977). Brainstorms. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Descartes, R. (1979). Meditations on First Philosophy. Indianapolis, IN, Hackett Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (1920/1948). Reconstruction of Philosophy. Boston, MA., Beacon Press.CrossRef
Dewey, J. (1938). Logic: The Theory of Inquiry. Carbondale, IL., Southern Illinois University Press.Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (1950). Common Sense and Science. Carbondale, IL, Southern Illinois University Press.Google Scholar
Dietrich, E. and , A. B. Markman (1998). “All information processing entails computation.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21(5): 637–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dretske, F. I (1994). If You Can't Make One, You Don't Know How It Works. Midwest Studies in Philosophy. P. French and T. Uehling. Notre Dame, IN., University of Notre Dame Press: 468–482.Google Scholar
Dreyfus, H. L (1972). What Computers Can't Do?New York, Harper Collins.Google Scholar
Dreyfus, H. L (1991). “Response to Collins, Artificial Experts.” Social Studies of Science 22: 717–726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dreyfus, H. L (1992). What Computers Still Can't Do? A critique of Artificial Reason. Cambridge, MA, The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dyer, F. C and , T. D. Seeley (1989). “On the evolution of the dance language.” The American Naturalist 133(4): 580–590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edelman, G. M (1987). Neural Darwinism: The Theory of Neural Group Selection. New York, Basic Books.Google Scholar
Edwards, P. N (1996). The Closed World: Computers and The Politics of Discourse in Cold War America. Cambridge, MA, The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ekbia, H. R (2001). Once Again, Artificial Intelligence at A Crossroads. Annual Meeting of the Society for Social Studies of Science, Boston, MA.Google Scholar
Ekbia, H. R (2002a). The Two Senses of “Common Sense.” 13th Midwest Artificial Intelligence and Cognitive Science Conference, Chicago, IL.Google Scholar
Ekbia, H. R (2002b). AI's Disparity Syndrome. Annual Meeting of the Society for Social Studies of Science, Milwaukee, WI.Google Scholar
Ekbia, H. R (2006). Taking Decisions into the Wild: An AI Perspective on the Design of iDSS. Intelligent Decision-Making Support Systems (I-DMSS): Foundations, Applications and Challenges J. Gupta, G. Forgionne and M. More. Berlin, Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Ekbia, H. R (forthcoming). “Informational Artifacts as Quasi-Objects.”
Ekbia, H. R and , L. Gasser (forthcoming). “Common Ground: For a Sociology of Code.”
Ekbia, H. R., , J Goldberg, , D Landy (2003). Starting Large or Small? An Unresolved Dilemma for Connectionist Models of Language Learning. Bootstrapping in Language Acquisition: Psychological, Linguistic and Computational Aspects. Bloomington, IN.Google Scholar
Ekbia, H. R and , N. Hara (2005). Incentive Structures in Knowledge Management. Encyclopedia of Knowledge Management. D. Schwartz. Hershey, PA, Idea Group.Google Scholar
Ekbia, H. R and , R. Kling (2003). Power in Knowledge Management in Late Modern Times. Academy of Management, Seattle, WA.Google Scholar
Ekbia, H. R and , A. Maguitman (2001). Context and Relevance: A Pragmatic Approach. 3rd International Conference on Modeling and Using Context, Dundee, UK, Springer Verlag.Google Scholar
Elman, J. L (1990). “Finding structure in time.” Cognitive Science 14: 179–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elman, J. L (1993). “Learning and development in neural networks: The importance of starting small.” Cognition 48: 71–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enfield, N. J and , S. C. Levinson (2006). Roots of Human Sociality: Culture, Cognition and Interaction. Oxford, Berg.Google Scholar
Epstein, J. M (2005). Remarks on the Foundations of Agent-Based Generative Social Science. The Brookings Institution: CSED Working Paper. Washington DC.Google Scholar
Falkenheiner, B., K. D. Forbus, et al. (1990). “The structure-mapping engine.” Artificial Intelligence 41(1): 1–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farah, M. J., K. D. Wiilson, et al. (1998). “What Is “Special” About Face Perception?Psychological Review 105(3): 42–498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fauconnier, G. (1994). Mental Spaces: Aspects of Meaning Construction in Natural Language. Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feigenbaum, E. A and A. Barr, Eds. (1982). The Handbook of Artificial Intelligence. Reading, MA., Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Fikes, R. E., P. E. Hart, et al. (1972). “Learning and Executing Generalized Robot Plans.” Artificial Intelligence 3(4): 251–288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. and , Z. Pylyshyn (1997). Connectionism and Cognitive Architecture: A Critical Analysis. Mind Design II: Philosophy, Psychology, Artificial Intelligence. J. Haugeland. Cambridge, MA, The MIT Press: 309–350.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A (1980). Methodological Solipsism Considered as a Research Strategy in Cognitive Psychology. Representations: Philosophical Essays on the Foundations of Cognitive Science, The Harvester Press.Google Scholar
Forbus, K. D., , D Gentner, et al. (1998). “Analogy just looks like high level perception: why a domain-general approach to analogical mapping is rightJ. EXPT. THFOR. ARTIF. INTELL 10: 231–257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forrest, S., Ed. (1991). Emergent Computation. Cambridge: MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Forsythe, D. E (2001). Studying Those Who Study Us: An Anthropologist in the World of Artificial Intelligence. Stanford, CA., Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Foucault, M. (1977). Knowledge and Power: Selected Interviews and Other Writings. New York, Pantheon Books.Google Scholar
Foucault, M. (1994). The Order of Things: An Archaelogy of the Human Sciences. New York: NY, Random House/Vintage Books.Google Scholar
Foundalis, H. (2006). Phaeaco: A Cognitive Architecture Inspired by Bongard's Problems. Computer Science Dept. Bloomington, Indiana University.Google Scholar
Friedman, M. (1999). Reconsidering Logical Positivism. Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Funt, B. V (1980). “Problem-Solving with Diagrammatic Representations.” Artificial Intelligence 13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gadamer, H.-G. (1976). Philosophical Hermeneutics. Berkeley, CA, University of California Press.Google Scholar
Gallistel, C. R (1998). Symbolic Processes in the Brain: The Case of Insect Navigation. An Invitation to Cognitive Science: Methods, Models, and Conceptual Issues. D. Scarborough and S. Sternberg. Cambridge, Mass., MIT Press/A Bradford Book.Google Scholar
Gasser, L. (1991). “Social conceptions of knowledge and action.” Artificial Intelligence 47(1): 107–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gasser, M. and , E. Colunga (2000). Babies, Variables, and Relational Correlations. 22nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Philadelphia, Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S., , R Ivry, et al. (1998). Fundamentals of Cognitive Neuroscience. New York, W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
Gee, J. P (1999). An Introduction to Discourse Analysis. UK, Routledge.Google Scholar
Gell, A. (1998). Art and Agency: An Anthropological Theory. Oxford, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Genter, D. (1983). “Structure-Mapping: A Theoretical Framework for Analogy.” Cognitive Science 7(2): 155–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerschenfeld, N. (1999). When Things Start to Think. New York, Henry Holt.Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J (1978). “The Ecological Approach to the Visual Perception of Pictures.” Leonardo 11(3): 227–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giere, R. N (1999). Science without Laws. Chicago, The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Gieryn, T. F (1999). Cultural Boundaries of Science: Credibility on the line. Chicago: IL, Univeristy of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Gilbert, N. (2004). Agent-Based Social Simulation. Center for Research on Social Simulation. Guilford: UK.Google Scholar
Gilbert, N. and , K. G. Troitzsch (2005). Simulation for the Social Scientist. Berkshire: UK, Open University Press.Google Scholar
Goodman, N. (1976). Languages of Art. Indianapolis, IN, Hacket Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Govindarajan, K. K., , S Grossberg, et al. (1994). A neural network model of auditory scene analysis and source segregation. Technical report. Boston, MA.Google Scholar
Graubard, S. R (1988). The Artificial Intelligence Debate: False Starts, Real Foundations. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Greenberg, K., Ed. (2000). The Robot in the Garden. Cambridge: MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1976). “Adaptive pattern classification and universal recoding: Part I. Parallel development and coding of neural feature detectors.” Biological Cybernetic 21: 121–134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1987). “Competetive Learning: From Interactive Activation to Adaptive Resonance.” Cognitive Science 11(1): 23–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grossberg, S. (1995). “The Attentive Brain.” American Scientist 83: 438–449.Google Scholar
Grossberg, S. and , C. W. Myers (1999). The Resonant Dynamics of Conscious Speech. Technical Report. Boston, MA.Google Scholar
Grover, L. (1996). A Fast Quantum Mechanical Algorithm for Data base Search. 28th Annual ACM Symposium on the Theory of Computing.Google Scholar
Hacking, I. (1979). “What is logic?The Journal of Philosophy LXXVI(6): 285–319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hacking, I. (1983). Representing and Intervening: Introductory Topics in the Philosophy of Natural Science. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hacking, I. (1999). Making Up People. Science Studies Reader. M. Biagioli. New York and London, Routledge: 161–171.Google Scholar
Hammond, K. J (1989). Case-Based Planning: Viewing Planning as a Memory Task. San Diego, CA., Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hammond, K. J., T. M. Converse, et al. (1995). “The Stabilization of Environments.” Artificial Intelligence 72(1–2): 305–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haraway, D. (1991). Simians, Cyborgs, and Women: The Reinvention of Nature. New York, Routledge.Google Scholar
Haraway, D. (1997). Modest_Witness@Second_Millenium.FemaleMan_Meets_OncoMouseTM. New York, Routledge.Google Scholar
Haraway, D. (2003). The Companion Species Manifesto: Dogs, People and Significant Others. Chicago, Prickly Paradigm Press.Google Scholar
Haraway, D. J (1989). Primate Visions: Gender, Race, and Nature in the World of Modern Science. New York and London, Routledge.Google Scholar
Harnad, S. (1990). “The symbol grounding problem.” Physica D 42: 335–346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haugeland, J. (1978/98). The Plausibility of Cognitivism. Having Thought. J. Haugeland. Cambridge, MA., Harvard University Press.
Haugeland, J., Ed. (1981). Mind Design I. Cambridge, MA, A Bradford Book/MIT Press.Google Scholar
Haugeland, J. (1985). Artificial Intelligence: The Very Idea. Cambridge, MA, Bradford/MIT Press.Google Scholar
Haugeland, J., Ed. (1997). Mind Design II: Philosophy, Psychology, Artificial Intelligence. Cambridge, Mass., A Bradford Book/MIT Press.Google Scholar
Haugeland, J. (1998). Having Thought: Essays in the Metaphysics of Mind. Cambridge, Mass., Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Hayes, P. J (1973). Computation and Deduction. 2nd Mathematical Fooundations of Computer Science Symposium, Czechoslovakian Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
Hayes, P. J (1990). The Naive Physics Manifesto. The Philosophy of Artificial Intelligence. M. A. Boden. Oxford, UK, Oxford University Press: 171–205.Google Scholar
Hayles, N. K (1999). How we became posthuman: Virtual bodies in cybernetics, literature, and informatics. Chicago, Chicago University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayles, N. K (2005). “Computing the Human.” Theory, Culture & Society 22(1): 131–151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hebb, D. (1949). Organization of Behavior. New York, Wiley.Google Scholar
Hempel, C. G and , P. Oppenheim (1948). “Studies in the Logic of Explanation.” Philosophy of Science 15: 135–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hempel, C. G and , P. Oppenheim (1949). Studies in the Logic of Explanation. Aspects of Scientific Explanation. C. G. Hempel. New York, Macmillan.Google Scholar
Hennessy, J. L and , D. A. Patterson (1996). Computer Architecture: A Quantitative Approach. San Francisco, CA., Morgan Kaufmann Publishers.Google Scholar
Hertz, J., , A Krogh, et al. (1991). Introduction to the Theory of Neural Computation. Redwood City, CA, Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Hinton, G. and , T. Sejnowski (1986). Learning and relearning in Boltzmann machines. Parallel Distributed Processing. D. Rumelhart and J. McClelland. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press. 1: 282–317.Google Scholar
Hirsh-Pasek, K. and , R. M. Golinkoff (1996). The Origins of Grammar: Evidence from Early Language Comprehension. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hofstadter, D. R (1979). Gödel, Escher, Bach: an Eternal Golden Braid. New York, N.Y., Basic Books.Google Scholar
Hofstadter, D. R (1985). Metamagical Themas: Questing for the Essence of Mind and Pattern. New York, N.Y., Basic Books.Google Scholar
Hofstadter, D. R (1985b). Waking Up from the Boolean Dream, Or, Subcognition as Computation. Metamagical Themas: Questing for the Essence of Mind and Pattern. New York, Basic Books.Google Scholar
Hofstadter, D. R (1995). Fluid Concepts and Creative Analogies. New York, N.Y., Basic Books.Google Scholar
Hofstadter, D. R (1997). Le Ton beau de Marot: In Praise of the Music of Language. New York, Basic Books.Google Scholar
Hofstadter, D. R (1999). Moore's Law, Artificial Evolution, and the Fate of Humanity. Festschrift in Honor of John Holland's 70th Birthday. M. Mitchell.Google Scholar
Hofstadter, D. R (2001). Analogy as the Core of Cognition. Integration of Theory and Data from the Cognitive, Computational, and Neural Sciences. K. J. Holyoak, Gentner, D., and Kokinov, B. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hofstadter, D. R (2002). Staring Emmy Straight in the Eye – and Doing My Best Not to Flinch. Creativity, Cognition, and Knowledge. T. Dartnall. Westport: CN, Praeger: 67–100.Google Scholar
Hofstadter, D. R and , D. C. Dennett (1982). The Mind's I: Fantasies and Reflections on Self and Soul. New York, Basic Books.Google Scholar
Hofstadter, D. R and , D. Moser (1989). “To Err is Human: To Study Error-Making in Cognitive Science.” Michigan Quarterly Review 28(2): 185–215.Google Scholar
Holland, J. (1975). Adaptation in Natural and Artificial Systems. Ann Arbor, Michigan, University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Holland, J. (1995). Hidden Order: How Adaptation Builds Complexity. Reading, MA., Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Holland, J. H (1986). Escaping Brittleness: The Possibilities of General-purpose Learning Algorithms Applied to Parallel Rule-based Systems. Machine Learning: An Artificial Intelligence Approach. R. S. Michalski, J. G. Carbonell and T. M. Mitchell. Los Altos, CA., Morgan Kaufmann. II: 593–623.Google Scholar
Hölldobler, B. and , E. O. Wilson (1990). Ants. Cambridge, MA, Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holyoak, K. J and , P. Thagard (1995). Mental Leaps: Analogy in Creative Thought. Cambridge: MA, MIT Press/Bradford Books.Google Scholar
Hopfield, J. (1982). Neural Networks and Physical Systems with Emergent Collective Computational Abilities. National Academy of Sciences, USA79.Google ScholarPubMed
Hume, D. (1739). A Treatise of Human Nature. London, Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Hummel, J. E and , K. J. Holyoak (1997). “Distributed Representations of Structure: A Theory of analogical access and mapping.” Psychological Review 104: 427–466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hurlbert, A. and , T. Poggio (1988). “Making machines (and artificial intelligence).” Daedalus 117(1): 213–239.Google Scholar
Huskey, V. R and , H. D. Huskey (1980). “Lady Lovelace and Charles Babbage.” Annals of History of Computing 2: 299–329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in The Wild. Cambridge, Mass., MIT Press.Google Scholar
James, W. (1907/1998). Pragmatism and The Meaning of Truth. Cambridge, MA., Harvard University Press.
Johnson-Laird, P. (1988). The Computer and the Mind: An Introduction to Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Jordan, M. I (1986). Attractor Dynamics and Parallelism in a Connectionist Sequential Machine. 8th Annual Conference of Cognitive Science Society, Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Joy, B. (2000). “Why the future doesn't need us?Wired 8(4).Google Scholar
Joyce, J. (1996). When/why/of what is less more? Center for Cogntive Science. Edinburgh, University of Edinburgh. M.Sc. Dissertation.Google Scholar
Kail, R. (1984). The development of memory. New York, W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Kaipainen, M. and , R. F. Port (unpublished). Speech Cycling.
Kanerva, P. (1988). Sparse Distributed Memory. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: a developmental perspcetive on cognitive science. Cambridge: MA, MIT Press/Bradford Books.Google Scholar
Keller, E. F (2007). Booting up baby. Genesis Redux Essays in the History and Philosophy of Artificial Life. J. Riskin. Chicago, University of Chicago Press: 334–345.Google Scholar
Kelso, J. A. S. (1995). Dynamic Patterns: The Self-Organization of Brain and Behavior. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kennedy, J. and , R. C. Eberhart (2001). Swarm Intelligence. New York, Morgan Kaufmann.Google Scholar
Kintsch, W. (2000). “Metaphor comprehension: A computational theory.” Psyhonomic Bulletin and Review 7: 257–266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirsh, D. (1991). “Today the earwig, tomorrow man?Artificial Intelligence 47: 161–184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kling, R. (1980). “Social analyses of computing: Theoretical perspectives in recent empirical research.” Computing Surveys 12(1): 61–110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kling, R. (1997). Hopes and Horrors: Technological Utopianism and Anti-Utopianism in Narratives of Computerization. Computerization and Controversy: Value Conflicts and Social Choices. R. Kling. San Diego, CA, Academic Press: 40–58.Google Scholar
Kling, R. (1999). What is Social Informatics and Why Does it Matter? D-Lib Magazine. 5.
Kling, R. and , S. Iacono (1995). Computerization movements and the mobilization of support for computerization. Ecologies of knowledge. L. S. Star. New York, SUNY: 119–153.Google Scholar
Knuth, D. (1983). The Art of Computer Programming. Reading, MA., Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Koestler, A. (1964). The Act of Creation. New York: NY, Macmillan.Google Scholar
Kohler, R. (1999). Moral Economy, Material Culture, and Community in Drosophilia Genetics. The Science Studies Reader. M. Biagioli. New York and London, Routledge: 243–257.Google Scholar
Kolodner, J. L (1996). Making the Implicit Explicit: Clarifying the Principles of Case-Based Reasoning. Case-Based Reasoning: Experiences, Lessons, and Future Directions. D. B. Leake. Menlo Park, CA., AAAI Press/MIT Press: 349–370.Google Scholar
Kolodner, J. L and , D. Leake (1996). A Tutorial Introduction to Case-Based Reasoning Case-Based Reasoning: Experiences, Lessons and Future Directions. D. Leake. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press: 31–65.Google Scholar
Kowalski, R. A (1977). Algorithm=Logic+Control. Memorandum, Imperial College. London, UK.Google Scholar
Kuhn, T. (1962). The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. Chicago, University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kurzweil, R. (1999). The Age of Spiritual Machines: When Computers Exceed Human Intelligence. New York, N.Y., Viking.Google Scholar
Laham, D. (1997). Latent Semantic Analysis approaches to categorization. 19th annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, Fire and Dangerous Things. Chicago, University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landauer, T. K., Foltz, P. W., & Laham, D. (1998). “Introduction to Latent Semantic Analysis.” Discourse Processes 25: 259–284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landauer, T. K., Laham, D., & Foltz, P. W. (1998). “Learning human-like knowledge by Singular Value Decomposition: A progress report. In M. I. Jordan, M. J. Kearns & S. A. Solla (Eds.), Advances.”
Landauer, T. K and , S. T. Dumais (1997). “A solution to Plato's problem: the latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge.” Psychological Review 104: 211–240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landauer, T. K., , D Laham, et al. (1997). How well can passage meaning be derived without using word order? A comparison of Latent Semantic Analysis and humans. 19th annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Landes, D. (2000). Revolution in Time: Clocks and the Making of the Modern World. London, Viking Press.Google Scholar
Langton, C. G (1989). Artificial Life. Redwood City, CA., Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Lanier, J. (2000) One Half of A Manifesto: Why stupid software will save the future from neo-Darwinian machines. Wired: 8(12).Google Scholar
Larkin, J. and , H. A. Simon (1987). “Why a Diagram is (Sometimes) Worth Ten Thousand Words.” Cognitive Science 11(1): 65–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Latash, M. L (1993). Control of Human Movement. Champaign, IL., Human Kinetics: 1–48.Google Scholar
Latour, B. (1987). Science in Action. Cambridge, MA., Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Latour, B. (1993). We Have Never Been Modern. Cambridge: MA, Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Latour, B. (1999). Pandora's Hope: Essays on the Reality of Science Studies. Cambridge, MA., Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Latour, B. (2005). Reassembling the Social: An Introduction to Actor-Network Theory. Oxford, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Leake, D. B., Ed. (1996). Case-Based Reasoning: Experiences, Lessons, and Future Directions. Menlo Park, CA., AAAI Press/MIT Press.Google Scholar
Leclerc, J. and , F. Gosselin (2004). Processes of artistic creativity: The case of Isabelle Hayeur. Twenty-Seventh Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Mahwah: NJ, Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Leclerc, J. and , F. Gosselin (2005). ACE: A Model of the Cognitive Strategies of a Contemporary Artist. Twenty-Seventh Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Mahwah: NJ, Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lenat, D. B (1997a). From 2001 to 2001: Common Sense and the Mind of HAL. Hal's Legacy: 2001's Computer As Dream & Reality. D. G. Stork. Cambridge, Mass., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lenat, D. B (1997b). “The Dimensions of Context Space.” Retrieved April, 2003, from www.cyc.com.
Lenat, D. B (2002). Cyc Workshop. Presentation.Google Scholar
Lenat, D. B and , E. A. Feigenbaum (1991). “On the Thresholds of Knowledge.” Artificial Intelligence 47(1–3): 185–250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lenat, D. B and , R. V. Guha (1990). Building Large Knowledge-Based Systems. Reading, Mass., Addison Wesley.Google Scholar
Lenat, D. B., , M Prakash, et al. (1986). “Using common-sense knowledge to overcome brittleness and knowledge acquisition bottlenecks.” AI Magazine 7: 65–85.Google Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lewis, D. (1971). “Analog and Digital.” Nous 5(3): 321–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewontin, R. C (1998). The Evolution of Cognition: Questions We Will Never Answer. An Invitation to Cognitive Science: Methods, Models, and Conceptual Issues. D. Scarborough and S. Sternberg. Cambridge, MA., A Bradford Book/MIT Press. 4: 107–132.Google Scholar
Lloyd, E. A (1994). The Structure and Confirmation of Evolutionary Theory. Princeton, N.J., Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Llyod, G. (1999). Science in Antiquity: The Greek and Chinese Cases and Their Relevance to the Problems of Culture and Cognition. Science Studies Reader. M. Biagioli. New York and London, Routledge: 302–316.Google Scholar
Loy, G. (1989). Composing with Computers: A Survey of Some Compositional Formalisms and Music Programming Languages. Current Directions in Computer Music Research. M. Mathews and J. R. Pierce. Cambridge: MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mackay, W. A., D. J. Crammond, et al. (1986). “Measurements of human forearm viscoelasticity.” Journal of Biomechanics 19: 231–238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manna, Z. and , R. Waldinger (1985). The Logical Basis for Computer Programming. Reading, Mass., Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Maris, M. and , R. T. Boekhorst (1996). Exploiting physical constraints: Heap formation through behavioral error in a group of robots. IEEE/RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems.Google Scholar
Marr, D. (1982). Vision. San Francisco, CA, Freeman.Google Scholar
Marshall, J. and , D. R. Hofstatder (1997). “The Metacat Project: a self-watching model of analogy-making.” Cognitive Studies: The Bulletin of Japanese Cogn.Sci.Soc. 4(4).Google Scholar
Marshall, J. B (1999). Metacat: A Self-Watching Cognitive Architecture for Analogy-Making and High-Level Perception. Computer Science Dept. Bloomington, Indiana University.Google Scholar
Mataric, M. J (1992). “Intergation of reprenstation into goal-driven behavior-based robots.” IEEE Transactions on Robotics and Automation 8(3): 304–312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mazlish, B. (1993). The Fourth Discontinuity: the co-evolution of humans and machines. New Haven, Yale University Press.Google Scholar
McCarthy, J. (1980). “Circumscription – A Form of Non-Monotonic Reasoning.” Artificial Intelligence 13: 27–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCarthy, J. (1986). “Applications of Circumscription to Formalizing Commonsense Knowledge.” Artificial Intelligence 28(1): 89–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCarthy, J. and , P. J. Hayes (1969). Some Philosophical Problems from the Standpoint of Artificial Intelligence. Machine Intelligence. B. M. D. Michie. Edinburgh, UK, Edinburgh University Press. 4.Google Scholar
McClelland, J. and , D. Rumelhart (1986). Parallel Distributed Processing. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
McCorduck, P. (1979). Machines who think. San Francisco, CA, W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
McCorduck, P. (1991). Aaron's Code: meta-art, artificial intelligence and the work of Harold Cohen. New York, Freeman.Google Scholar
McCulloch, W. S and , W. H. Pitts (1943). “A Logical Calculus of the Ideas Immanent in Nervous Activity.” Bulletin of Mathematical Biophysics 5: 115–133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDermott, D. (1976). “Artificial intelligence meets natural stupidity.” SIGART Newsletter 57: 4–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDermott, D. (1982). “A Temporal Logic for Reasoning about Processes and Plans.” Cognitive Science 6: 101–155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDermott, D. (1987). “A Critique of Pure Reason.” Computational Intelligence 3(1): 151–160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonough, R. (2002). Emergence and Creativity. Creativity, Cognition, and Knowledge. T. Dartnall. Westport: CN, Praeger: 282–301.Google Scholar
McGraw, G. and , D. R. Hofstadter (2002). Letter Spirit: Perception and Creation of Diverse Alphabetic Styles. Creativity, Cognition, and Knowledge. T. Dartnall. Westport: CN, Praeger: 251–266.Google Scholar
McLeod, P., , K Plunkett, et al. (1998). Introduction to Connectionist Modelling of Cognitive Processes. New York, Oxford Univeristy Press.Google Scholar
McNeal, C. (1998). The Medieval Art of Love. New York, N.Y., Gaimann & King Ltd.Google Scholar
Mead, G. H (1934). Mind, Self, and Society from the Standpoint of a Social Behaviorist. Chicago, Chicago Univeristy Press.Google Scholar
Meglicki, Z. (2000). “Introduction to Quantum Computing.” Retrieved May 1, 2003, from http://www.ovpit.indiana.edu/B679.
Menzel, P. and F. D'Aluisio (2000). Robo sapiens. Cambridge: MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Minsky, M. (1975). A framework for representing knowledge. The Psychology of Computer Vision. P. H. Winston. New York, McGraw-Hill: 211–277.Google Scholar
Minsky, M. (1985). The Society of Mind. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Minsky, M. (1995). “Future of AI Technology.” Toshiba Review 47(7).Google Scholar
Minsky, M. (2005). The Emotion Machine. Accessed November 28, 2006 from: web.media.mit.edu/∼minsky/.
Mitchell, M. (1993). Analogy-Making as Perception: A Computer Model. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mitchell, M. (1996). An Introduction to Genetic Algorithms. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mitchell, M. (1998). “Theories of structure versus theories of change.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21(5): 645–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moravec, H. (1998). Mind Children: The Future of Robot and Human Intelligence. Cambridge and New York, Cambirdge University Press.Google Scholar
Morris, W. C., G. W. Cottrell, et al. (1999). “A Connectionist Simulation of the Empirircal Acquisition of Grammatical Relations.” Cognitive Science.Google Scholar
Mortensen, C. (1989). Mental Images: Should Cognitive Science Learn From Neurophysiology. Computers, Brains, and Minds: Essays in Cognitive Scienc. P. Slezak and W. R. Albury. Dodrecht, Kluwer Academic Publishers.Google Scholar
Nagel, T. (1974). “What is it like to be a bat?Philosophical Review 4: 435–450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neumaier, O. (1987). A Wittgensteinian View of Artificial Intelligence. Aritificial Intelligence: The Case Against. R. Born. London, Croom Helm: 132–174.Google Scholar
Newborn, M. (1997). Kasparov versus Deep Blue: Computer Chess Comes of Age. New York, N.Y., Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newell, A. (1990). Unified Theories of Cognition. Cambridge, MA., Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Newell, A., J. C. Shaw, et al. (1960). Report on a General Problem-Solving Program. Proceedings of the International Conference on Information Processing, Paris.Google Scholar
Newell, A. and , H. A. Simon (1961). GPS, a program that simulates human thought. Lernende Automaten. H. Billing. Munich, Oldenbourg: 109–124.Google Scholar
Newell, A. and , H. A. Simon (1976). “Computer Science as Empirical Inquiry.” Communications of the ACM 19(3): 113–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newport, E. L (1990). “Maturational constraints on language learning.” Cognitive Science 14: 11–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noble, D. F (1977). America by Design: Science, Technology, and the Rise of Corporate capitalism. Oxford, UK, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Noble, D. F (1984). Forces of Production: A Social History of Industrial Automation. New York, NY, Alfred A. Knopf.Google Scholar
Norton, A. (1995). Dynamics: An Introduction. Mind as Motion. R. F. Port and T. v. Gelder. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press: 45–68.Google Scholar
Oyama, S. (2000). Evolution's Eye: A Systems View of the Biology-Culture Divide. Durham, NC, Duke University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peirce, C. S (1905/1940). Critical Common-Sensism. The Philosophy of Peirce: Selected Writings. J. Buchler. New York: Harcourt, Brace & Company: 290–301.
Petitot, J., F. J. Varela, et al. (1999). Naturalizing Phenomenology: Issues in Contemporary Phenomenology and Cogntive Science. Stanford, CA., Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Petroski, H. (1989). The Pencil: A History of Design and Circumstances. New York, Knopf.Google Scholar
Pfeifer, R. and , C. Scheier (1999). Understanding Intelligence. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1952). The Origins of Intelligence in Children. New York, International University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. (1984). Language Learnability and Language Development. Cambridge, MA., Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1991). “Rules of Language.” Science 253: 530–535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinker, S. and , A. Prince (1988). “On Language and Connectionism: Analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition.” Cognition 28: 73–193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plate, T. (1995). Holographic Reduced Representations. Department of Computer Science. Toronto, University of Toronto.Google Scholar
Plunkett, K. and , P. Juola (1999). “A Connectionist Model of English Past Tense and Plural Morphology.” Cognitive Science 23(4): 463–490.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plunkett, K. and , V. Marchman (1991). “U-shaped learning and frequency effects in a multi-layered perceptron: Implications for child language acquisition.” Cognition 38: 43–102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poincaré, H. (1952). Science and Hypothesis. New York, Dover Publications.Google Scholar
Poincaré, H. (1982). The Foundations of Science: Science and hypothesis, the value of science, science and method. Washington, DC, University Press of America.Google Scholar
Polanyi, M. (1958). Personal Knowledge: Towards a Postcritical Philosophy. Chicago, Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Popper, K. R (1972). Two Faces of Common Sense. Objective Knowledge: An Evolutionary Approach. Oxford, UK, Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Port, R. F and T. van Gelder (1995). Mind as Motion: Explorations in the Dynamics of Cognition. Cambridge, MA., A Bradford Book/MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pratt, V. (1994). “Visit to Cyc.” Retrieved Accessed July 24, 2002, from http://boole.stanford.edu/pratt.html.
Preskill, J. (1997). “Lecture Notes for Physics 229: Quantum Information and Computation.” Retrieved Accessed May 4 2000, from www.theory.caltech.edu/people/preskill/ph229.
Prinz, J. J and , L. W. Barsalou (2002). Acquisition and Productivity in Perceptual Symbol Systems: An Account of Mundane Creativity. Creativity, Cognition, and Knowledge. T. Dartnall. Westport: CN, Praeger: 105–126.Google Scholar
Pulvermüller, F. (1999). “Words in the Brain's Language.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22: 253–279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnam, H. (1975). Philosophical Papers: Mind, Language and Reality. Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnam, H. (1990). Realism with a Human Face. Cambridge, MA., Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Pylyshyn, Z. W (1984). Computation and Cognition: Toward a Foundation for Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA, Bradford/MIT Press.Google Scholar
Quartz, S. R and , T. J. Sejnowski (1997). “The Neural Basis of Cognitive Development: A Constructivist Manifesto.” Brain and Behavioral Sciences 20: 537–596.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quine, W. V and , J. S. Ullian (1978). The Web of Belief. New York, McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Reeke, G. N and , G. M. Edelman (1988). “Real brains and artificial intelligence.” Daedalus 117(1): 143–173.Google Scholar
Regier, T. and , L. Carlson (2001). “Grounding spatial language in perception: An empirical and computational investigation.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 130: 273–298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rehling, J. (2002). Results in Letter Spirit Project. Creativity, Cognition, and Knowledge. T. Dartnall. Westport: CN, Praeger: 272–278.Google Scholar
Reid, T. (1785/1969). Essays on the Intellectual Powers of Man. Cambridge, MA., The MIT Press.
Restivo, S. (2001). Bringing Up and Booting Up: Social Theory and the Emergence of Socially Intelligent Robots. IEEE Conference on Robotics and Cybernetics, IEEE.Google Scholar
Rhode, D. L. T. and , D. C. Plaut (1999). “Language acquisition in the absence of explicit negative evidence: how important is starting small?Cognition 72: 67–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riesbeck, C. and , R. C. Schank (1989). Inside Case-based Reasoning. Hillsdale, N.J., Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Riesbeck, C. K (1996). What Next? The Future of Case-Based Reasoning in Post-Modern AI. Case-Based Reasoning: Experiences, Lessons, and Future. D. B. Leake. Menlo park, CA., AAAI Press/MIT Press: 371–388.Google Scholar
Risan, L. (1997). Artificial Life: A technoscience leaving modernity? An Anthropology of subjects and objects. Accessed April 19, 2003 from: http://anthrobase.com/Txt/R/Risan_L_05.htm.
Riskin, J. (2007). Genesis Redux: Essays in the History and Philosophy of Artificial Life. Chicago, University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, S. and , H. Pashler (2000). “How persuasive is a good fit? A comment on theory testing.” Psychological Review 107: 358–367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rochester, N., , J Holland, et al. (1956). “Test on a cell assembly theory of the action of the brain, using a large digital computer.” IRE Transactions on Information Theory IT-2: 80–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenblatt, P. (1958). “The Perceptron: A probabilistic model for information storage and organization in the brain.” Psychological Review 65: 386–408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roszak, T. (1994). The Cult of Information: A Neo-Luddite Treatise on High-Tech, Artificial Intelligence, and The True Art of Thinking. Berkeley, CA, University of California Press.Google Scholar
Rouse, J. (1987). Knowledge and Power: Toward a Political Philosophy of Science. Ithaca: N.Y., Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Rouse, J. (1999). Understanding Scientific Practices: Cultural Studies of Science as a Philosophical Program. The Science Studies Reader. M. Biagioli. New York and London, Routledge: 442–456.Google Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E (1989). The Architecture of Mind: A Connectionist Approach. Mind Design II: Philosophy, Psychology, Artificial Intelligence. J. Haugeland. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press: 205–232.Google Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E (1997). The Architecture of Mind: A Connectionist Approach. Mind Design II. J. Haugeland. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press: 205–232.Google Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E., G. E. Hinton, et al. (1986). Learning internal representations by error propagation. Parallel Distributed Processing. D. E. R. a. J. L. McClelland. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press. 2.Google Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E and , D. Norman (1982). “Simulating a Skilled Typist: A Study of Skilled Cognitive–Motor Performance.” Cognitive Science 6(1): 1–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E and , D. Zipser (1985). “Feature discovery by competitive learning.” Cognitive Science 9: 75–112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, S. J and , P. Norvig (1995). Artificial Intelligence: A Modern Approach, Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Saffran, J. R., R. N. Aslin, et al. (1996). “Statistical learning by 8-month old infants.” Science 274(5294): 1926–1928.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Santambrogio, M. and , P. Violi (1988). Introduction. Meaning and Mental Representation. M. Santambrogio, P. Violi and U. Eco. Bloomington, IN, Indiana University Press: 3–23.Google Scholar
Scassellati, B. (1998). Imitation and Mechanisms of Joint Attention: A Developmental Structure for Building Social Skills on a Humanoid Robot. Computation for Metaphors, Analogy and Agents. C. Nehaniv, Springer-Verlag. 1562.Google Scholar
Scassellati, B. (1999). A Binocular, Foveated Active Vision System, MIT, Artificial Intelligence Laboratory.
Scassellati, B. (2000). Investigating Models of Social Development Using a Humanoid Robot. Biorobotics. B. Webb and T. Consi. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Schaffer, S. (1994) Babbage's Intelligence: Calculating Engines and the Factory System. Accessed April 14, 2004 from: http://www.wmin.ac.uk/mad/schaffer/schaffer01.html.
Schank, R. C (1972). “Conceptual Dependency: A theory of natural language understanding.” Cognitive Psychology 3(4): 552–631.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schank, R. C (1982). Dynamic Memory: A Theory of Learning in Computers and People. Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Schank, R. C (1986). Explanation Patterns. Hillsdale, N.J., Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc.Google Scholar
Schank, R. C (1995). Information Is Surprises. The Third Culture: Beyond the Scientific Revolution. J. Brockman. New York, Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
Schank, R. C and , R. P. Abelson (1977). Scripts, Plans, Goals, and Understanding. Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc.Google Scholar
Schank, R. C and , L. Birnbaum (1984). Memory, Meaning, and Syntax. Talking Minds: The Study of Language in Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press: 209–251.Google Scholar
Schank, R. C and , A. Kass (1988). Knowledge Representation in People and Machines. Meaning and Mental Representations. U. Eco, M. Santambrogio and P. Violi. Bloomington and Indianapolis, Indiana University Press: 181–200.Google Scholar
Schank, R. C., , A Kass, et al. (1994). Inside Case-based Explanation. Hillsdale, N.J., Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc.Google Scholar
Schank, R. C and , D. B. Leake (2001). Natural Language Processing: Models of Roger Schank and His Students. Encycopedia of Cognitive Science.Google Scholar
Schank, R. C and , C. Riesbeck (1981). Inside Computer Understanding: Five Programs with Miniatures. Hillsdale, NJ., Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Scheutz, M. (2002). Agents with or without Emotions? Proceedings of the Fifteenth International Florida Artificial Intelligence Research Society Conference. 89–93.
Scheutz, M., , P Schemerhorn, et al. (2007). “First Steps toward Natural Human-Like HRI.” Autonomous Robots 22: 411–423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searle, J. (1992). Rediscovery of The Mind. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Searle, J. (2002). I Married A Computer. Are We Spiritual Machines? Ray Kurzweil vs. the Critics of Strong A.I. J. W. Richards. Seattle, WA., Discovery Institute.Google Scholar
Searle, J. R (1980). Minds, Brains, and Programs. Mind Design II. J. Haugeland. Cambridge, Mass., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Searle, J. R (1984). Minds, Brains, and Science. Cambridge, MA., Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Seidenberg, M. S and , M. C. MacDonald (1999). “A Probabilistic Constraints Approach to Language Acquisition and Processing.” Cognitive Science 23(4): 569–588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sejnowski, T. and , C. Rosenberg (1987). NETtalk: a parallel network that learns to read aloud, Technical Report JHU/EECS-86/01, Johns Hopkins University.
Selfridge, O. (1959). Pandemonium: A paradigm for learning. Symposium on the Mechanization of Thought, London, HMSO.Google Scholar
Shanker, S. G (1987). AI at the Crossroads. The Question of Artificial Intelligence. B. Bloomfield. London, Croom Helm: 1–58.Google Scholar
Shannon, C. (1950). “Programming a computer for playing chess.” Philosophical Magazine 41: 256–275.Google Scholar
Shapin, S. (1994). A Social History of Truth: Civility and Science in Seventeenth-Century England. Chicago: IL, University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Shimojima, A. (1996). On the Efficacy of Representation. Philosophy. Bloomington, Indiana University.Google Scholar
Shultz, T. R and , W. C. Schmidt (1991). A Cascade-Correlation Model of Balance Scale Phenomenon. 13th Annual Conference of Cognitive Science Society, Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Simon, H. A (1969). The Sciences of The Artificial. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Simon, H. A (1995). “Artificial Intelligence: an empirical science.” Artificial Intelligence 77(1): 95–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F (1985). “Cognitive Science and Behaviourism.” British Journal of Psychology 76: 33–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sloman, A. (2001). “Beyond Shallow Models of Emotion.” Cognitive Processing 2(1): 177–198.Google Scholar
Sloman, A. (2005a). AI in a New Millenium: Obstacles and Opportunities. IJCAI. Birmingham, UK.Google Scholar
Sloman, A. (2005b). Do Machines, Natural or Artificial, Really Need Emotions? NWO Cognition Program. Utrecht.Google Scholar
Smith, B. C (1991). “The Owl and the Electric Encyclopedia.” Artificial Intelligence 47(1–3): 252–288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, B. C (1996). The Origin of Objects. Cambridge, MA, The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Smith, B. C (1997) One Hundred Billion Lines of C++. Accessed October 15, 2005 from: http://www.ageofsignificance.org/people/bcsmith/papers/smith-billion.html.
Smith, B. C (1999). Situatedness. MIT Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science (MITECS). Cambridge, MA., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Smith, B. C (2000). Indiscrete Affairs. Vital Signs: Cultural Perspectives on Coding Life and Vitalizing Code. L. Suchman and J. Fujimura. (in press).Google Scholar
Smith, B. C (2002). Reply to Dennett. Philosophy of Mental Representation. H. Clapin. Oxford, Oxford University Press: 237–266.Google Scholar
Smith, B. C (forthcoming). The Age of Significance: Introduction. Cambidge, MA, The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Smolensky, P. (1988). “On the proper treatment of connectionism.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11: 1–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smolensky, P. (1999). “Grammar-based connectionist approaches to language.” Cognitive Science 23: 589–613.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Star, L. S., Ed. (1995). Ecologies of Knowledge Work and Politics in Science and Technology. SUNY Series in Science, Technology, and Society. New York: NY, SUNY.Google Scholar
Steedman, M. (1999). “Connectionist Sentence Processing in Perspective.” Cognitive Science 23(4): 615–634.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steels, L. (1990). Cooperation between distributed agents through self-organization. Decentralized AI. Y. Demazeau and J. P. Muller. Amsterdam, North-Holland: 175–196.Google Scholar
Stewart, J. (1992). Life=Cognition: The epistemological and ontological significance of Artificial Life. First European Conference on Artificial Life, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Suchman, L. (1987). Plans and Situated Actions. Cambridge, UK., Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Suchman, L. A (2007). Human-Machine Reconfigurations. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Thelen, E. and , L. B. Smith (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. K. R., D. L. Oden, et al. (1997). “Language-Native Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) Judge Relations Between Relations in a Conceptual Matching-to-sample Task.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 23: 31–43.Google Scholar
Toole, B. A (1992). Ada, the Enchantress of Numbers. Mill Valley: CA, Strawberry Press.Google Scholar
Townsend, J. T and , J. Busemeyer (1995). Dyamic Representation of Decision-Making. Mind as Motion. R. F. a. T. v. G. Port. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press: 101–119.Google Scholar
Turing, A. M (1936). On Computable Numbers, with an Application to Entscheidungsproblem. London Mathematical Society.Google Scholar
Turing, A. M (1950). “Computing Machinery and Intelligence.” Mind 59: 433–460.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turing, A. M (1953). Digital Computers Applied to Games. Faster than Thought. B. V. Bowden. London, UK, Pitman.Google Scholar
Turkle, S. (1985). The Second Self: Computers and The Human Spirit. New York, NY, Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
Turkle, S. (1995). Life on the Screen: Identity in the Age of Internet. New York, N.Y., Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
van Gelder, T. (1998). “The dynamical hypothesis in cognitive science.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21(5): 615–628.Google ScholarPubMed
Varela, F. J., , E Thompson, et al. (1991). The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience. Cambridge, Mass., MIT Press.Google Scholar
Foerster, H. (1970). Thoughts and Notes on Cognition. Cognition: A multiple view. P. L. Garvin. New York, Spartan Books: 25–48.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. (1986). Thought and Language. Cambridge, Mass., MIT Press Translations.Google Scholar
Weizenbaum, J. (1976). Computer Power and Human Reason. San Francisco, CA, W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Wellman, B. (2001). “Computer Networks as Social Networks.” Science 293: 2031–2034.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wellman, H. M and , S. A. Gellman (1992). “Cognitive Development: Foundational theories of core domains.” Annual Review of Psychology 43: 337–375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiener, N. (1948). Cybernetics. New York, Wiley.Google ScholarPubMed
Wills, L. M and , J. L. Kolodner (1996). Toward More Creative Case-Based Design Systems. Case-Based Reasoning: Experiences, Lessons, and Future Directions. D. B. Leake. Menlo Park, CA., AAAI Press/MIT Press: 81–91.Google Scholar
Wilson, E. O (1971). The Insect Society. Cambridge, MA., Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Winner, L. (1986). The Whale and The Reactor: A Search for Limits in an Age of High Technology. Chicago, IL, University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Winograd, T. (1972). Understanding Natural Language. New York, Academic Press.Google Scholar
Winograd, T. and , F. Flores (1986). Understanding Computers and Cognition: A New Foundation for Design. Norwood, N.J., Ablex.Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical Investigations. New York, Blackwell.Google Scholar
Woods, W. A (1975). What's in a link? Foundations for semantic networks. Representation and Understanding: Studies in Cognitive Science. D. G. Bobrow and A. M. Collins. New York, Academic Press: 35–82.Google Scholar
Woolgar, S. (1985). “Why Not a Sociology of Machines? The Case of Sociology and Artificial Intelligence.” Sociology 19(4): 557–572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woolgar, S. (1987). Reconstructing Man and Machine: A Note on Sociological Critiques of Cognitivism. The Social Construction of Technological Systems. T. H. W. Bijker, and T. Pinch. Cambridge, MA., MIT Press: 311–328.Google Scholar
Woolgar, S. (1995). Representation, Cognition, and Self: What Hope for the Integration of Psychology and Sociology? Ecologies of Knowledge: Work and Politics in Science and Technology. S. L. Star. New York State University of New York Press: 154–179.Google Scholar
Zipser, M. (2002). Introduction to the Theory of Computation. Boston, MA, PWS Publishing.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Bibliography
  • H. R. Ekbia, Indiana University
  • Book: Artificial Dreams
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511802126.017
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Bibliography
  • H. R. Ekbia, Indiana University
  • Book: Artificial Dreams
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511802126.017
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Bibliography
  • H. R. Ekbia, Indiana University
  • Book: Artificial Dreams
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511802126.017
Available formats
×