Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-09T06:20:46.993Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 2 - The Supporting Theory of Mentalization-Based Treatment

from Part I - Overview of the Model

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 May 2023

Anthony Bateman
Affiliation:
Anna Freud National Centre for Children and Families, London
Peter Fonagy
Affiliation:
University College London
Chloe Campbell
Affiliation:
University College London
Patrick Luyten
Affiliation:
University College London
Martin Debbané
Affiliation:
University College London

Summary

The development of mentalizing and non-mentalizing is discussed along with the importance of mentalizing as a psychotherapy process in the treatment of mental health problems. A defining feature of mental disorder is the experience of “wild imagination,” and we consider that mentalizing difficulties—that is, the tendency to get caught up in unhelpful ways of imagining what is going on both for ourselves and for other people—are the price we as a species pay for the immense benefits of the human imagination. Mental health problems arise when mentalizing is lost and we use mentalizing processes that are from earlier stages of development. The relationships between mentalizing, use of low mentalizing modes, epistemic trust and distrust, and social processes are summarized.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023

Introduction

Given that mentalizing is, as we have described in Chapter 1, rather difficult and prone to going wrong, and that it can cause us considerable psychological distress when it does go wrong, why do we do it so much? Humans are a social species and, in order to support our survival, natural selection has provided us with the tools necessary to interact with and manage the hugely complex terrain of our social world. Understanding the thoughts, intentions, and mental states of our fellow humans is the key to the creation of human societies and our attainment of dominance over other species on our planet. We constantly infer thoughts, feelings, and beliefs in the minds of other people, and much of our mental life is spent engaging with the task of processing social information [Reference Meyer, Davachi, Ochsner and Lieberman1]. Many writers and researchers believe that the evolutionary advantages of living in relatively large social groups were made by possible by humans developing the capacities to interpret, explain, and predict each other’s behavior, and using these capacities to share and accumulate experiences, to plan and collaborate, and to attain joint goals that they would not have been able to achieve as individuals or as members of smaller groups [Reference Bloom2Reference Tomasello4]. There are excellent neuroscientific experimental studies which provide ample evidence that mentalizing, as indicated by the activation (or inhibition) of the network of neurons involved in Theory of Mind, is critical for evaluating the morality of others (including those who are harmful), for predicting how competitors will behave, and for learning from those who have special information about a shared environment (e.g., stock markets) [Reference Park, Kim, Young, Gilead and Ochsner5].

We have argued that vulnerability to mental health disorders is the flipside of the advantages that mentalizing brings [Reference Fonagy, Campbell and Constantinou6]. On the basis of data from epidemiological studies, only one in five people will go through life without experiencing a diagnosable mental health condition [Reference Schaefer, Caspi and Belsky7]. Looking at such prevalence figures from the perspective of natural selection, it is clear that whatever the neural systems are that underpin mental disorder, they must have other functions that are critical for survival. A defining feature of mental disorder is the experience of “wild imagination,” and we consider that mentalizing difficulties—the tendency to get caught up in unhelpful ways of imagining what is going on both for ourselves and for other people—are the price we as a species pay for the immense benefits of the human imagination [Reference Fonagy, Campbell and Constantinou6].

The Non-Mentalizing Modes

Mentalizing is a complicated and multifaceted process. We have pinpointed three non-mentalizing modes, which describe different states of mind that are encountered when mentalizing is ineffective, to help clinicians to understand and recognize when a client’s mentalizing is poor. These modes are called psychic equivalence, teleological mode, and pretend mode.

Psychic Equivalence

Psychic equivalence is a way of thinking that involves assuming that what you are thinking is what everyone else is thinking, is obviously true, and is the same as external reality. Mental is equivalent to physical. An example of psychic equivalence in a young child can be seen in their feelings of fear about being alone in the dark at bedtime translating into a genuine terror that there is a monster under the bed, or that the unpalatable cabbage they are being encouraged to eat is poisonous. An adult in a state of psychic equivalence, if feeling angry and upset, may assume that others around them are hostile and angry too—potentially generating a destructive interpersonal cycle in which the other person does become hostile in response to the first person’s behaviors, which were prompted by their assumptions.

Teleological Mode

In teleological mode, only the behavioral outcomes of mental states are taken seriously. Mental states themselves lose their significance. Again, if we think about this in terms of the pre-mentalizing states of young children, we can see the teleological mode in the ways that toddlers behave—actions speak louder than words, affection is accompanied by gifts, kisses, or hugs, and “badly behaved” toys are physically punished. In adulthood, this mode may manifest in an individual requiring physical or sexual contact from others in order to escape their feeling of being unlovable or worthless, or it may play out in a tendency toward violence or self-harm when feeling overwhelmed or angry.

Pretend Mode

In pretend mode, there is a lack of appropriate connection between one’s thoughts about mental states and external reality. The concept of pretend mode was based on observation of the ways in which young children enjoy using and enacting mental states in play, but only when they can clearly separate the game from physical reality. When an adult breaks the pretense by clumsily joining in from the real world, the game is spoiled. In adults, pretend mode can appear as pseudomentalizing, which is important to recognize in clinical work—it can involve what seems to be rather elaborate mentalizing, with plenty of discussion and apparent thought about mental states, but in rather aimless, contradictory, or meaningless ways. Many of us have been in meetings where possible points of action are endlessly discussed, but with a sinking background feeling that there will be little real progress.

We shall refer back to these three modes in our discussions of clinical work, as we encounter different clinical scenarios and expressions of mentalizing breakdown. It is also important to bear in mind that these different modes can overlap and be observed at the same time. It is not uncommon, for example, for the distress generated in a state of psychic equivalence, because a thought is experienced as real without the moderation of psychological status, to spill over into teleological mode. For example, the thought “I feel terrible; obviously everybody hates me and I am a bad person” may trigger and converge with the thought, in teleological mode, “I need to hurt myself to deal with how hateful I feel.

The Mentalizing Dimensions

To help us understand the how and why of the ways in which mentalizing and non-mentalizing can take these different forms, we need to appreciate that mentalizing draws on several different kinds of social-cognitive activities, underpinned by different neurobiological processes. We have categorized these into four mentalizing dimensions (which are sometimes called polarities, as they are not dimensional in the everyday sense):

  • automatic/controlled

  • self/other

  • cognitive/affective

  • internal/external.

We all move backward and forward along these dimensions in response to changes in our environment and adjustments in what we are thinking. At times, for example, we might be focused on our own mental states, while at other moments we might be more caught up in what we think is in the minds of other people. We often find that individuals who are experiencing emotional distress or behavioral difficulties tend to be more “stuck” at one pole of one or more of the dimensions. We shall briefly explain these dimensions here, but we shall also refer to them in our account of clinical processes, to illustrate how thinking about them can help us in our work.

Automatic/Controlled Mentalizing

Most of the time, we mentalize in a fairly automatic way—we do not stop to think very explicitly or reflectively about what is going on. In fact, when someone is excessively controlled in their mentalizing, the interpersonal experience can feel rather like hard work and unspontaneous. To return to the example given at the beginning of Chapter 1, when we meet a partner, flatmate, or friend after work we respond to their demeanor in quite an immediate, unreflective way. But on registering that they look unhappy, worried, or preoccupied, we may slow things down a little and start to think in a more conscious and controlled way about what might be going on for them—was there a particular problem at work that has been bothering them, and how should we best respond to their upset in a helpful or sympathetic manner? In that moment, we are moving along this dimension of mentalizing, from automatic to controlled, in response to the demands of the situation.

Self/Other Mentalizing

Traditionally, Theory of Mind has focused on the way in which we think about the mental states of others. Mentalizing theory—and here it reflects some of its origins in psychoanalysis—assumes that the ability to understand our own mental states cannot be taken for granted either. Even though we tend to have a lot more information about our own states (e.g., our physical sensations, our internal narrative voice, an understanding of what the limits of our knowledge are) than about those of other people, we are still vulnerable to substantial biases and significant errors when making judgments about the reasons for our actions [Reference Nisbett and Ross8Reference Johansson, Hall, Sikstrom and Olsson10], or we may not want to admit to ourselves the real reasons for our behavior.

Effective mentalizing requires us to be aware of someone else’s mental states as being distinct from our own. But because we are such a socially driven species, the mentalizing of self and other often interfere with each other—for example, the mere awareness of the presence of another perspective slows down thinking [Reference Samson, Apperly and Braithwaite11]. Indeed, there is evidence that overlapping neural networks are activated when reflecting on the self or others [Reference Beeney, Hallquist, Ellison and Levy12]. This neural correspondence reflects the way our own mental experiences (thoughts, feelings, and memories) can create a template for understanding someone else [Reference Bradford, Jentzsch and Gomez13,Reference Gordon, Gilead and Ochsner14], just as the effort to understand others’ thoughts, feelings, and reflections about us (which we call personal second-order mentalizing) might clarify and elaborate our own sense of ourselves [Reference Cooley15Reference Fonagy, Gergely, Jurist and Target17].

From a clinical perspective, an individual who tends to be more stuck at the “other” pole of the self/other dimension might be more vulnerable to being overwhelmed by the emotions of others, and to being swept up by another’s perspective. This can leave them vulnerable to contagious emotional storms and also, in some cases, to exploitation, as they allow the mental states of another person to dominate them. In contrast, some individuals who have a strong capacity to mentalize others but who lack emotional empathy (see this chapter: Cognitive/Affective Mentalizing) can have a tendency to misuse or exploit others. Meanwhile, an excessive focus on self-mentalizing can deprive an individual of access to the regulating effects of other people’s perspectives, and reduces their social connectedness.

Cognitive/Affective Mentalizing

This mentalizing dimension covers, at the cognitive pole, the capacity to identify, label, and invoke reasoning about mental states in oneself or in others. The opposite pole, affective mentalizing, is concerned with the feeling of what is going on—again in oneself or in others. If an individual is stuck at the affective end, without the labeling and contextualizing of emotions that cognition provides, they may experience overwhelming dysregulated emotions, which come to dominate their behavior. The lack of balance and the absence of contextualizing of feelings can lead to catastrophizing. Cognition adds reflection and questioning to mentalizing, whereas the domination of emotion leads to an unnatural lack of hesitancy and to a particular certainty about ideas that emotional conviction can add to beliefs. Domination by affect potentially makes anything that is thought feel as if it is real, and leads to an intolerance of alternative ways of seeing things. We are equally familiar with the constraint that accompanies being stuck at the cognitive pole. In this state, an individual can describe feelings in the absence of these emotions actually being experienced. Such an intellectual understanding can serve as helpful protection against being overwhelmed by emotions; however, in the absence of the experience of feeling it can feel disconnected from reality, becoming a form of pretend mode functioning (discussed earlier in this chapter), and it can lack genuine empathy.

Internal/External Mentalizing

The internal/external dimension refers to the possibility of inferring someone’s mental states from external cues, such as their facial expressions, as opposed to thinking about what is going on for them on the basis of what we imagine about their internal state—that is, what we imagine that they know, think, or believe. An exclusively external orientation can make a person hypervigilant, constantly looking for external evidence and judging by appearance. If evidence for attitudes and other internal states has to come from the outside, there will be a corresponding lack of conviction about one’s own intuitions and an excessive need to seek external reassurance. Similarly, we might, for example, find it difficult sometimes to recognize how anxious we are feeling until we notice how much we are fidgeting. An excessive external focus can lead to a neglect of the internal, and the absence of a way of generating a model for how one feels can lead to overwhelming feelings of “not knowing” and even emptiness. This vacuum can drive the individual toward seeking intense experiences to fill the gap. Excessive focus on external states can also cause an individual to be highly reactive to other people’s physical actions and behaviors, even if these actions are not directed at them. This is because the individual is unable to anchor their response in a coherent understanding of the other person’s mental state, which would require contextualizing and creating hypotheses about the reasons for the other person’s actions. Meanwhile, an excessive focus on internal states can lead to unwarranted inferences and complicated suppositions about another’s state of mind without a sufficient anchor in external reality—that is, a tendency toward the pretend mode that we described earlier in this chapter.

We hope that this description of the four dimensions captures some common mentalizing difficulties. Good or effective mentalizing assumes that there is flexible “movement” between the poles of each dimension, achieving a balance between them in processing mental representations. Depending on the context and subject, ineffective mentalizing can look quite different. We all have different strengths and weakness according to where we tend to land on each mentalizing dimension. Although we often (probably unhelpfully!) talk of “breakdowns” or “failures” in mentalizing, as if mentalizing is a single process that simply stops, these breakdowns can appear in different ways, depending on how the individual is functioning across the different dimensions.

Developmental Aspects of Mentalizing: Child/Adolescent/Adult

Mentalizing is a developmental achievement, and the full repertoire of mentalizing skills emerges over the course of childhood and adolescence. There are two reasons why it is helpful to understand how mentalizing develops. The first reason is that, when we are working with children and adolescents, it is useful to know what level of mental-state understanding we can expect from them, and what signs there might be of problems in the development of their individual functioning. The second reason why it is useful to understand the unfurling of ever more complex mentalizing skills across development is that, from the perspective of developmental psychopathology, it helps us to make sense of a patient’s vulnerabilities and how these affect their functioning.

The non-mentalizing modes, discussed earlier in this chapter, were partly inspired by careful observations of how children operate—recognizing the literality of the toddler’s horror of “poisonous” cabbage, or understanding the precursor of pretend mode in young children’s play. Indeed, in some of our earlier work we described these modes as pre-mentalizing rather than non-mentalizing modes. Moving forward through development from toddlers to adolescents, our growing understanding of the heightened and unstable experience of mentalizing in adolescence helps to explain why this life stage is a period of particular vulnerability in terms of the emergence of mental health difficulties.

Bertram Malle has provided a helpful summary of the developmental emergence of mentalizing in what he describes as the “tree of social cognition” [Reference Malle, Gilead and Ochsner18]. At the base of the “tree,” in the first 6 months of life, infants show capacities that are shared with non-human primates, such as differentiating between biological and non-biological agents. They develop a special interest in reading faces, and can detect that biological agents have goals, in contrast to movement by objects that is not goal directed. By the end of the first year, they can follow another person’s gaze (i.e., they show an interest in the direction of other people’s attention), they socially reference with others (e.g., they might look at a caregiver as if to ask “Should I be frightened of this stranger or new object?”), and, critically, they can engage in shared social attention—they enjoy the feeling of looking at things together. After another 6 months, infants are able to detect intent behind an action and can mimic others, usually to establish social links. Between the second and third years of life, a capacity for genuine empathy emerges, along with deliberate imitation and the capacity to infer wishes and desires. During the third and fourth years, children start inferring knowledge (which can be tested in the well-known false-belief test of Theory of Mind) and start to show self-knowledge and self-awareness. Non-human primates can also make inferences about what others want, but the evidence for their ability to infer false beliefs is debated and, if this ability is present, it is certainly not robust.

From middle childhood onward, the child develops an ever-increasing sophistication in ascribing mental states to others. Arguably, this ability continues to improve gradually right through to young adulthood. For example, by the age of 7–9 years, children master third- and fourth-order false beliefs (e.g., “If you think that I think that you think …” [Reference Osterhaus, Koerber and Sodian19]), and develop increasingly complex explanations of actions [Reference Atance, Metcalf, Martin-Ordas and Walker20]. At the top of the “tree” is explaining action in terms of the stable characteristics of an agent (so-called trait inference). This capacity reflects how the child has come to view the world as being occupied by people with different behavioral tendencies, based on a sense of “what so-and-so is like,” and therefore, by sampling another person’s actions, we can create a general picture of them that will help us to predict their behavior in a new situation. Of course, infants do this implicitly (behaviorally) via non-mentalizing processes from the earliest days of attachment to their caregivers, but at this point attachment moves to the level of representation and is experienced in terms of the anticipated mental states of attachment figures.

A defining feature of adolescence is the increased importance of social relationships, marked by an increase in social sensitivity [Reference Somerville21]. There is evidence that adolescents are more likely to report bad mood and anxiety following an experience of rejection [Reference Sebastian, Viding, Williams and Blakemore22], and that they expect less favorable positive feedback from their peers [Reference Moor, Guroglu and Op de Macks23]. The network of brain regions involved in mentalizing (especially the medial prefrontal cortex) is more responsive to social rejection in adolescents than in adults [Reference Sebastian, Tan and Roiser24], perhaps as a result of greater functional connectivity between the regions of the mentalizing network [Reference Burnett and Blakemore25].

This brief review of the development of mentalizing across childhood and adolescence has highlighted two things. The first is that mentalizing capacities are based on the emergence of multiple interlinked components, with explicit, content-led mental state ascriptions—what we think of as fully fledged mentalizing—developing quite late. A second repeated theme concerns the role of the relationship between the self and the other in the emergence of these capacities. The abilities that we describe may be conceptualized in purely cognitive terms, but the content of mental-state attributions, especially in the early years when a child is totally dependent on their caregivers, can be of the utmost seriousness, implying the availability of food, protection, and warmth, or indeed the opposite—risk, danger, and death. Self-awareness and self-regulation play a critical role in differentiating and modifying numerous mentalizing capacities, thus providing important functions that make social living possible, and this self-awareness and self-regulation is supported by the relationships and social environment that construct the sense of self. In the next section we shall describe the interpersonal nature of these developmental processes in more detail.

The Impact of the Environment on the Emergence of Mentalizing

How do we acquire our understanding of mind across development? Findings from neuroscience research show that the ability to mentalize is a prewired evolutionary adaptation—the process of acquiring increased social cognition, described in the preceding section, is a normative developmental trajectory. However, the achievement of full and robust mentalizing is dependent on environmental input. There are good reasons for this—in the same way that we are prewired for language acquisition, but not for the acquisition of any particular language, we need to be open to learning a different “language” for thinking and talking about mental states. We learn to understand mental states from how others around us frame them. Infants and young children have wants and feelings, and caregivers “read” their behaviors—their need for food, or assistance, or comfort—and offer evidence of intuitive understanding through actions or verbal responses [Reference de Villiers, Gilead and Ochsner26]. Carers are predisposed to guess, with typical utterances such as “You are tired, you have had enough of this game” or “Oh, it was the shiny silver foil that you were after!” Different cultures vary in the interpretations that they give to a child’s behavioral reactions [Reference Aival-Naveh, Rothschild‐Yakar and Kurman27], and even within the same culture caregivers will not give the same labels to expressions of supposed internal states. However it is expressed, toddlers rapidly acquire internal-state language from those around them. They like to explore and often talk confidently about others’ feelings, preferences, desires, and perceptions, allowing rich communication within families that includes increased empathy but also, for example, teasing [Reference Dunn, Brophy, Astington and Baird28]. The constant asking of questions by young children—the notoriously persistent “why” questions—is, we argue, partly about obtaining information, but more fundamentally about reveling in the process of joint attention on a shared object. The more children hear mental-state words and are exposed to mentalizing talk, the more their ability to successfully interpret false-belief tasks is likely to improve [Reference Dunn, Brophy, Astington and Baird28,Reference Meins, Fernyhough and Arnott29]. Some families engage in a lot of mental-state talk, and their children’s ability to mentalize is advanced, whereas others do much less—parents’ self-reported use of elaborated mentalizing conversation appears to predict higher scores on tasks that assess Theory of Mind [Reference Ebert, Peterson, Slaughter and Weinert30].

A young child develops a sense of their own mind, and then of other people’s minds, through early experiences of having how they are feeling being accurately recognized, mirrored, and responded to by a caregiver. These interpretations of and responses to what is going on for children help them to build a sense of who they are and to develop a sense of agency—that is, the ability to carry an idea of themselves as an active presence in the world, whose experiences matter, are of interest to those around them, and can make things happen. The caregiver’s responses that reflect back to the child what they are feeling are called “secondary representations.” It is as a result of having adults around us to decipher and delineate “who we are” for us that we can develop a coherent sense of self (sometimes called self-representation); in that sense selfhood, rather than being all about individuality, is an intrinsically social construct. Individuals who have not had the benefit of such secondary representations may have a diminished or less coherent self-representation (see this chapter: The Alien Self); these may include individuals who have been exposed to such responsive care but, for a complex range of possible reasons, have not been able to benefit from it.

There is plenty of evidence of the importance of being mentalized for a child’s development. The significance of parental mentalizing was first revealed in a large empirical study exploring parents’ capacity for reflective functioning, which found that a parent’s ability to think about and understand their childhood relationship to their own parents, measured during pregnancy, strongly predicted their infant’s attachment security [Reference Fonagy, Steele and Moran31]. A follow-up study found that antenatal parental mentalizing continued to be a predictor of the child’s reflective functioning 17 years later, when the children had grown into young adults [Reference Steele, Perez, Segal and Steele32]. Since this initial research, there has been a great deal of work on the effects of caregivers’ mentalizing on outcomes for their children. In the area of child attachment outcomes, a meta-analysis by Zeegers et al. examined the relationships between parental mentalizing, parental sensitivity, and infant attachment outcomes. This meta-analysis found that both mentalizing and sensitivity had significant direct effects on infant–parent attachment after controlling for the effects of each other, which suggests that parental mentalizing is directly related to infant–parent attachment security [Reference Zeegers, Colonnesi, Stams and Meins33].

As well as evidence for the impact of parental mentalizing on attachment security, there is also a growing body of evidence which indicates that better parental mentalizing fosters mentalizing in children [Reference Meins, Fernyhough and Wainwright34] and in adolescents [Reference Rosso and Airaldi35,Reference Rosso, Viterbori and Scopesi36]. Whereas associations between parental mentalizing and infant attachment typically represent small effect sizes (defined as Cohen’s d=0.20), the association between parental and infant mentalizing is typically stronger, representing medium to large effect sizes (Cohen’s d=0.500.80). For instance, Rosso and Airaldi found a particularly strong association between mothers’ ability to mentalize negative and mixed-ambivalent mental states, but not positive mental states, and the corresponding ability in their adolescent children (r=0.400.50) [Reference Rosso and Airaldi35]. Findings such as these suggest that the capacity of caregivers to reflect on difficult and affect-charged mental states is particularly important in the context of the transmission of mentalizing between generations, from parents to their children.

Studies on mentalizing and adversity during childhood have provided some of the strongest evidence for the potential role of caregivers’ mentalizing capacities in the development of their children’s mentalizing capacity. Early adversity and complex trauma (i.e., negative experiences in early life involving neglect or abuse, typically within an attachment/caregiving context) in particular have been shown to have the potential to severely impair mentalizing. This impairment is apparent in strongly biased mentalizing, hypersensitivity to the mental states of others, and a defensive inhibition of mentalizing, or a combination of these features (for reviews, see Borelli et al. [Reference Borelli, Cohen and Pettit37] and Luyten and Fonagy [Reference Luyten, Fonagy, Bateman and Fonagy38]). At the same time, there is increasing evidence that high levels of caregivers’ reflective functioning, particularly reflective functioning with regard to their own traumatic experiences (see Chapter 11), may be an important buffer in the relationship between early adversity and child outcomes (reviewed in Borelli et al. [Reference Borelli, Cohen and Pettit37]). For instance, higher trauma-related reflective functioning in parents with a history of sexual abuse and neglect has been shown to be related to lower risk of infant attachment disorganization [Reference Berthelot, Ensink and Bernazzani39], and to a substantially lower risk of exposure to childhood sexual abuse in their own infants [Reference Borelli, Cohen and Pettit37]. These findings are of particular clinical interest as they suggest the value of both prevention and intervention work, making use of mentalizing, with vulnerable parents. Finally, mentalizing difficulties reported in childhood are associated with social-emotional and cognitive problems, such as difficulties in emotion regulation and interpersonal problems, internalizing and externalizing problems, and difficulties in attentional control, effortful control, and academic achievement (for reviews, see Fonagy and Luyten [Reference Fonagy, Luyten and Cicchetti40Reference Luyten and Fonagy42]).

Whereas our earlier work tended to heavily emphasize the role of attachment in supporting or inhibiting the development of mentalizing [Reference Fonagy43,Reference Fonagy, Steele and Steele44], more recently our views have evolved into a more comprehensive set of considerations about the role of family members, peers, and broader sociocultural factors, such as socioeconomic deprivation, social isolation, and school climate, in the development of mentalizing [Reference Fonagy, Luyten, Allison and Campbell45,Reference Fonagy, Luyten, Allison and Campbell46]. In keeping with this view, there is growing evidence for mentalization-based treatment (MBT)-related interventions that focus on the family and the broader social context that surrounds an individual. Several studies have provided evidence of the effectiveness of MBT in various groups, such as substance-abusing mothers and their infants Reference Suchman, DeCoste, Borelli and McMahon[47], fostered and adopted children Reference Redfern, Wood and Lassri[48], mothers living in underserved poor urban communities with children at high risk of maltreatment [Reference Slade, Holland and Ordway49,Reference Byrne, Sleed and Midgley50], individuals supporting a family member with borderline personality disorder (BPD) Reference Bateman and Fonagy[51], and school-based prevention and intervention programs Reference Fonagy, Twemlow and Vernberg[52]. These types of family and “system-level” interventions might be most effective in addressing the problems that non-mentalizing social environments (e.g., neighborhoods with high levels of crime and violence, or schools with a culture of bullying) tend to generate, by creating a mentalizing climate as a counterweight against competitive, hostile, and aggressive wishes and tendencies. We are of the view that the social network around an individual serves to support the recovery of mentalizing when it is inevitably lost in the course of the daily difficulties encountered in life—the “micro-traumas” to which we are all continuously exposed. Connecting to others at moments of stress affords powerful protection against these problems because it ensures some balanced mentalizing, and thus resilience, in relation to the experience [Reference Fonagy, Allison, Campbell, Bateman and Fonagy53,Reference Fonagy, Steele and Steele54]. We consider that creating a mentalizing climate around the individual or family is particularly important for children and young people, and for providing support to their families/carers (for further discussion of mentalizing-based interventions that aim to do this, see Chapters 15 and 17). An important additional aspect of this view is that it acknowledges the need to provide a supportive mentalizing system around mental health professionals, given the many internal and external pressures and anxieties that they face in their work.

The Alien Self

The idea of the alien self is something we use in MBT to capture the experience of individuals who, in a high state of stress or distress, find their sense of self to be so frightened/frightening and incoherent that an “alien self” steps in. As we shall see in Chapters 14 and 18, the alien self has been particularly useful in understanding some cases of suicidality, serious acts of self-harm, interpersonal aggression/violence, and other mental health crises. When we speak of the alien self, we mean that disturbances in core self, sensory, and cognitive psychological processes lead the individual to experience various forms of loss in their personal agency. Their experience, which could be related to bodily, cognitive, or affective (dys)control, is of a “not-me,” and there is a failure to integrate the experience of self coherently—essentially, parts of selfhood, experienced in the here and now, are alienated. We consider this representation to originate from the internalization of an insensitive or overwhelming caregiver, as a consequence of the child’s exposure to failed mirroring by the caregiver. By “mirroring,” we mean interactions where the caregiver recognizes the infant’s mental states and is able to reflect those states back to the infant in a “marked,” or regulated, way which shows that they understand and empathize with the infant’s feelings but are not overwhelmed by them. Later in development, hostile or abusive caregiving is managed by the activation of a split-off part of the self-representation—the alien self—which presents the child with the possibility of obtaining a degree of control over an uncontrollable social environment by identifying with the aggressor (the abusive caregiver) using this alien self; the individual then becomes host to a torturing figure within the self-structure. In a sense, alien self-representations can help individuals to cope with disturbances of the self; the emergence of the alien self is an attempt at an emergency rescue from the intense emotional experiences that the individual is unable to manage or regulate. Although this defensive process affords something of a sense of temporary control, it also causes an acute experience of fragmentation and a feeling of being tortured from within. The alien self is a threat to psychological integrity, and can often lead to behaviors such as self-harm and suicide attempts that are linked to the affective pressure coming from the alien self-experience. Importantly, psychological contact with alien self-representations may also lead to dissociation, which is a hallmark of a mental process coming in as an attempt to protect psychological self-integrity. The importance of the alien self in clinical work is considered further in Chapters 10 and 12.

As we described in the previous section, the sense of self, and the ongoing coherence of the sense of self, is partly constructed on the basis of the secondary representations of who we are that are communicated to us, in the first instance, by our caregivers in early life. An individual who has missed out on sufficiently benign, coherent, or accurate secondary representations, or who has difficulty in recognizing and internalizing these secondary representations (possibly because of a lack of exposure to such representations), may be particularly vulnerable to developing a fragmented self-structure (i.e., an alien self), which is then called upon in response to a chaotic or empty sense of self-experience. The latter is sometimes called a “black hole” by individuals with BPD, who unsurprisingly find it terrifying, as it threatens their very existence.

Attachment and MBT

MBT is strongly shaped by attachment theory—the idea that the development of mentalizing emerges in the context of attachment relationships has always been central to our thinking, and evidence from research supports the view that there is a “loose coupling” between attachment, emotional sensitivity, and parental mentalizing capacities [Reference Luyten, Campbell, Allison and Fonagy55]. In MBT work, a patient’s attachment experiences and representations will play a key part in the therapist’s understanding of the challenges that the patient faces when thinking about other people’s mental states. However, in the practice of MBT, it is also vital that the therapist understands the impact of attachment arousal in the here and now of treatment, and how the experience of such strong emotions can hinder effective mentalizing.

Brain-imaging studies have shown that the activation of the attachment system inhibits the neural systems that support mentalizing [Reference Bartels and Zeki56Reference Nolte, Bolling and Hudac58]. Trauma can activate the attachment system, and attachment trauma may cause chronic hyperactivation. This explains how some patients may be able to mentalize when they are under little or no stress; however, arousal related to attachment may cause a collapse of mentalizing at moments of stress in individuals who are prone to this form of hyperactivation. This has important implications for clinical work. Any intervention that calls for reflection—for example, by asking the patient to clarify or elaborate on a thought—is by its very nature asking the patient to engage in controlled mentalizing. If the topic that the patient is being asked to reflect on triggers arousal related to attachment, it may be very difficult for them to engage in such reflective activity. This may be one of the reasons why, historically, dynamic therapy for BPD was often found to be ineffective—traditional psychotherapeutic methods trigger the attachment system, both through the nature of the relationship between therapist and patient and through the subject matter that is explored in therapy. In effect, the patient is being asked to carry out a potentially impossible task—to think about mental states in a regulated way while being placed under circumstances that make such reflection almost impossible.

For all that MBT is about mentalizing, the work of MBT should not involve a relentless onslaught of reflective mentalizing on the part of the therapist. A patient who is in a state of high emotion, whose attachment system is ringing alarm bells, and whose mentalizing has gone “offline” should not be met with further demands to mentalize—these will either be ignored or be experienced as insensitive and unhelpful. Rather, the appropriate response from the therapist is recognition and validation of the patient’s feelings at that moment. The reasons for this will unfold in the next sections of this chapter, in which we shall explain how the capacity for shared attention and thinking together is made possible by a sense of personal recognition. First, we shall explain how our clinical experiences directed our thinking in this regard.

Borderline Personality Disorder and MBT

As we mentioned in Chapter 1, MBT was first developed as a response to an acute awareness of how few patients with a diagnosis of BPD were receiving the therapeutic help and support that they needed. MBT was deliberately built on the awareness that a form of treatment was required that (a) really addressed the clinical needs and real-life experiences and difficulties endured by these patients, and (b) was sufficiently usable for a broad range of mental health workers to implement reliably without undergoing years of intensive training.

Given how much thinking about BPD has influenced the development of MBT, we believe it will be useful to give an overview of the nature of the difficulties associated with BPD, to help the reader to understand some key elements of the general mentalizing approach and the rationale for this approach. One of the most common features of BPD is emotional dysregulation, which involves a hypersensitivity to emotional stimuli that can spark intense and unstable emotional responses. Individuals with BPD may struggle to find coping strategies that help to reduce their distress, and the emotional dysregulation is often coupled with impulsive symptoms such as self-injurious behavior (including impulsive self-harming behaviors), cognitive disturbances (including dissociation), and interpersonal problems. Individuals with BPD often experience intense anguish, and they are 50 times as likely to attempt suicide as members of the general population [Reference Pompili, Girardi, Ruberto and Tatarelli59].

Stress-related dissociation is common in individuals with BPD Reference Korzekwa, Dell and Links[60], and can affect the sense of identity, episodic memory, perception, and consciousness [Reference Miller, Townsend and Grenyer61]. Disruptions of such fundamental social-cognitive processes can be understood as defensive (i.e., self-protective) in nature, by which we mean they are adaptations to stressful internal experiences that function to limit the impact of these experiences on the person (or their body, as in derealization or emotional numbing). We see the symptoms of BPD as understandable mental adjustments made by the individual to cope with what would otherwise be overwhelming subjective experiences of stress or distress Reference Vermetten and Spiegel[62]. Sometimes the impulsivity observed in BPD plays out as impulsive aggression [Reference Ende, Cackowski and Van Eijk63,Reference Sebastian, Jung and Krause-Utz64]. Aggression shares with emotional dysregulation an association with high levels of stress and distress Reference Krause-Utz, Keibel-Mauchnik and Ebner-Priemer[65], but also features a lack of consideration for the mental state of the person toward whom the anger is directed Reference Bateman, Bolton and Fonagy[66]. A range of negative behaviors, such as aggressive outbursts, binge eating, risky sexual behavior, substance abuse, or suddenly ending a relationship, can be triggered in negative emotional contexts where the individual perceives these actions as uncontrollable and inevitable reactions to an impossible (social) situation [Reference Cackowski, Reitz and Ende67,Reference Jacob, Zvonik and Kamphausen68].

Interpersonal disturbances are widely thought to be central in the work of helping people with BPD. There is strong evidence that individuals with BPD have difficulty in developing trust in others, a heightened sensitivity to social exclusion and rejection, and lengthy histories of conflicted unstable relationships with unresolved conflict about separating [Reference Lis and Bohus69,Reference King-Casas, Sharp and Lomax-Bream70]. Often the histories of individuals who present for treatment amply explain their reluctance to place trust in social relationships [Reference Reinhard, Dewald-Kaufmann and Wustenberg71,Reference Jowett, Karatzias and Albert72]. Whether interpersonal disturbances should be regarded as attempts to manage earlier developmental problems (e.g., as in transference-focused psychotherapy) or as caused by other difficulties (as conceptualized in dialectical behavior therapy) remains a source of controversy, but from the pragmatic standpoint of a clinician, both the emotions themselves and their impact on relationships need to be tackled.

Mentalizing and BPD

A growing literature suggests that mentalizing difficulties are an intrinsic aspect of BPD and may drive the interpersonal disturbance, affect dysregulation, and impulsivity that are characteristically shown by individuals with this diagnosis. The mentalizing difficulties found in people with BPD can cover all four of the mentalizing dimensions (which were outlined in detail earlier in this chapter).

In relation to automatic/controlled mentalizing, the dysregulation and impulsivity associated with BPD reflect a tendency toward automatic mentalizing. In what was perhaps the first demonstration of the failure of reflective mentalizing in BPD, Fonagy et al. showed a dramatically reduced ability of BPD patients to reflect on their own and others’ mental states in the context of attachment relationships [Reference Fonagy, Leigh and Steele73]; this deficit in reflective functioning has been shown to be reversible by psychotherapy [Reference Levy, Meehan and Kelly74]. The findings on reflective functioning have been replicated by two relatively large studies [Reference Fischer-Kern, Schuster and Kapusta75,Reference Gullestad, Johansen and Høglend76] and confirmed in a recent meta-analysis [Reference Bora77].

A number of studies have shown that BPD patients show mentalizing impairments when tested with the Movie for the Assessment of Social Cognition (MASC), which is currently probably the best validated and most reliable measure of this mentalizing domain [Reference Preissler, Dziobek and Ritter78Reference Sharp, Pane and Ha80]. The MASC is a video-based test of mentalizing that requires participants to recognize the mental states of characters as they interact in an everyday-life scenario involving relationships. The misinterpretation of interpersonal situations because of a limited reflective capacity may generate extreme or inappropriate emotions in social interactions [Reference Dziobek, Preissler and Grozdanovic81], particularly if there is also an overemphasis on externally oriented social cognition.

The self/other mentalizing dimension can often appear in BPD as an excessive focus on the mental states of others. Within the MBT model, knowledge of self and knowledge of other are complementary but interdependent. We learn about ourselves from others, and this self-knowledge helps us to place ourselves in another’s shoes. We would expect mentalizing problems to manifest as a reduction in self-knowledge and unstable self-representations. Another area of mentalizing the self is its role in generating a sense of ownership over one’s own actions. Several studies suggest that the experience of agency may be reduced in people with BPD relative to those without BPD [Reference Bekrater-Bodmann, Chung and Foell82Reference Pavony and Lenzenweger84]. There is emerging evidence of difficulties with self–other differentiation in BPD [Reference De Meulemeester, Lowyck and Luyten85], with, for example, observations of greater proneness to the Rubber Hand Illusion [Reference Bekrater-Bodmann, Chung and Foell82,Reference Neustadter, Fineberg and Leavitt86], and confusion in identifying the self from graded morphed self-images [Reference De Meulemeester, Lowyck and Panagiotopoulou87]. Several investigations have shown that people with BPD are more likely to recognize negative than positive self-attributes, suggesting distortions in their self-concept toward greater negativity [Reference Auerbach, Tarlow and Bondy88Reference Kanske, Schulze and Dziobek91]. However, these studies gave a less clear picture in relation to the structural complexity of the self-concept. One study of adolescents and young adults reported that patients with BPD endorsed a greater number of attributes as being self-related, indicating a more diffuse sense of self [Reference Auerbach, Tarlow and Bondy88], whereas another study of adults did not mirror this observation [Reference Vater, Schroder-Abe and Weissgerber90]. The differences in the findings of these studies could reflect the tendency of adolescents with emerging BPD to hypermentalize, which is likely to decrease with age [Reference Sharp, Ha and Carbone92]. In line with the greater prominence of the self in BPD, memory studies suggest that individuals with BPD have enhanced recall of self-relevant social events uninfluenced by emotional valence [Reference Winter, Koplin and Schmahl93]. A “mind-wandering” paradigm suggested that self-related thoughts fluctuate more, and are more extreme, in individuals with BPD [Reference Kanske, Schulze and Dziobek91].

The evidence in relation to the internal/external dimension of mentalizing in BPD is mixed. Accuracy of emotion recognition appears to be reduced for static facial stimuli in individuals with BPD in some studies [Reference Ritzl, Csukly, Balazs and Egerhazi94,Reference Lowyck, Luyten and Vanwalleghem95], but not in others [Reference Bertsch, Krauch and Stopfer96Reference Bertsch, Gamer and Schmidt98]. Similarly, whereas some studies reported that patients with BPD take longer than individuals without BPD to decode faces with emotional expressions [Reference Bertsch, Gamer and Schmidt98], other studies did not find this difference [Reference Kobeleva, Seidel and Kohler99,Reference Dyck, Habel and Slodczyk100], and a meta-analysis confirmed the absence of a reliably identifiable deficit in the speed of emotional face processing in people with BPD [Reference Hanegraaf, van Baal, Hohwy and Verdejo-Garcia101]. There are also mixed findings regarding facial reactivity to brief emotion-induction videos. One study reported reduced reactivity in individuals with BPD (effect size = –0.67) [Reference Renneberg, Heyn, Gebhard and Bachmann102], whereas another found reduced reactivity only to positive emotions, and also reported that BPD patients displayed more negative and mixed facial emotions in response to social exclusion than individuals without BPD [Reference Staebler, Renneberg and Stopsack103]. There is evidence that facial mimicry is enhanced in BPD. In a task in which participants’ facial muscle activity in response to dynamic facial expressions was recorded, BPD patients showed enhanced responses to angry, sad, and disgusted facial expressions, and attenuated responses to happy and surprised faces [Reference Matzke, Herpertz and Berger104]. This finding suggests a specific rather than general hypersensitivity to the emotional state of others, expressed by exaggerated responses to negative social signals and attenuated responses to positive signals. Some studies have also identified a strong negative attribution bias, with BPD patients attending more quickly to negative faces and spending more time looking at them [Reference Schulze, Schmahl and Niedtfeld105].

A large number of studies have explored mental-state discrimination in BPD. Most of these studies reported a medium-sized effect across a large number of measures. The most prominent of these measures is the Reading the Mind in the Eyes test, which has yielded both the largest reported advantage for mind reading [Reference Fertuck, Jekal and Song106,Reference Frick, Lang and Kotchoubey107] and the greatest deficits in those with BPD [Reference Berenson, Dochat and Martin108,Reference Anupama, Bhola, Thirthalli and Mehta109]. A meta-analysis by Hanegraaf et al. revealed a substantial and significant heterogeneity between the studies (Q=151.55, p<0.0001) [Reference Hanegraaf, van Baal, Hohwy and Verdejo-Garcia101]. However, this comprehensive meta-analysis provides extensive and robust evidence that people with BPD tend to be poorer at mental-state discrimination than people from other clinical populations, including those with other personality disorders (cluster C, avoidant, and narcissistic personality disorders) and those with major depression. Obviously this adds empirical support to the MBT approach and justifies our focus on mentalizing in our therapeutic work with patients with a diagnosis of BPD. BPD tends to be associated with difficulties in both selecting and displaying optimal social signals during interpersonal interactions, through facial expressions, language, and caregiver–infant behavior, and when interpreting the social signals of others.

Not surprisingly, given that the diagnostic criteria for BPD include rejection sensitivity, in experimental studies of social exclusion it has been found that individuals with this diagnosis report more severe experiences of ostracism than control participants without BPD; in fact, BPD patients are more likely to report feelings of exclusion even in conditions of social inclusion (e.g., [Reference Brown, Plener and Groen110]). There is evidence that rejection sensitivity is mediated by social-cognitive factors (tolerance of ambiguity, effortful control) and attachment-related factors (attachment anxiety, feeling of belonging, self-criticism) [Reference Sato, Fonagy and Luyten111,Reference Sato, Fonagy and Luyten112]. In the meta-analysis by Hanegraaf et al., BPD patients were also found to tend to feel more excluded than individuals with social anxiety disorder, major depressive disorder, or non-suicidal self-injury [Reference Hanegraaf, van Baal, Hohwy and Verdejo-Garcia101]. A failure of mentalizing combined with the expectation of negative outcomes from social interactions leaves individuals with BPD vulnerable to misinterpreting such interactions as instances of rejection.

Widening the Lens: Mentalizing in Psychopathology

We hope the description of the mentalizing challenges that individuals with BPD have to face has helped to provide a good sense of the rationale for the MBT approach. We shall now explain how and why this thinking can also inform the psychological treatment of other mental health disorders, ranging from other personality disorders to anxiety and depression, and to psychosis. This widening application of MBT is based on the view that mentalizing difficulties are common to many forms of mental health problems.

There is a lively and growing body of research around what is called the general psychopathology factor, or “p factor,” in mental disorder. This is the idea that it might be more useful to understand the many overlapping mental health diagnoses that exist as sharing a common vulnerability factor, which then interacts with individual circumstances and genetic tendencies to manifest as a particular set of symptoms. To give quite a crude example, adolescent boys on the whole are statistically more likely to show externalizing symptoms, such as conduct disorder, whereas adolescent girls are more likely to develop internalizing symptoms, such as anxiety or depression. What is more, an adolescent boy with conduct disorder may well go on to experience depression and anxiety as an adult, with or without continuing conduct problems. So, across different populations, or across one individual’s life course, there will be a range of experiences and behaviors that appear quite different in terms of both outward behavior and emotional experience, but, according to the thinking around the p factor, the underpinning vulnerability that leads to all of these different symptoms remains the same—it is just that they play out differently for different people as a result of a combination of their different social circumstances and genetic factors. There are various reasons for giving credence to this idea—for example, how do we make sense of the numerous concurrent and often overlapping difficulties that people experience? Findings from behavioral genetics and molecular biology are also consistent with the argument that there is a common underlying factor. Studies of families and twins tend to show that genetic risk is not specific to particular disorders, but is largely a transdiagnostic vulnerability factor [Reference Franic, Dolan and Borsboom113Reference Pettersson, Larsson and Lichtenstein116]. Similarly, a genome study of 17 mental disorders with over one million participants found that, in terms of genomic markers, the mental disorders shared common variance risks [Reference Anttila, Bulik-Sullivan and Finucane117].

The argument for the possibility that a single common factor underpins vulnerability also arises from population studies that use bifactor modeling of symptoms and diagnoses. Bifactor modeling involves exploring a possible general factor that a broad range of different variables—in this case symptoms and diagnoses—may share in common. Work in this area has found that psychiatric disorders are more convincingly explained if a general psychopathology factor is assumed to exist alongside clusters of symptoms (internalizing, externalizing, and psychosis) and individual mental disorders—for example, schizophrenia, generalized anxiety disorder, and depression [Reference Caspi and Moffitt118Reference Lahey, Applegate and Hakes120]. Numerous studies with samples of children, adolescents, and adults indicate that the p factor appears to capture an underlying propensity for any kind of psychopathology as indicated through the apparent symptoms [Reference Lahey, Krueger and Rathouz121Reference Patalay, Fonagy and Deighton123].

The emphasis on the p factor leaves us with the question of what this factor actually is, other than a useful statistical construct. Many studies suggest that all mental disorders involve a dysfunction of just one or two parts of the brain—the prefrontal cortex and perhaps the limbic regions [Reference Macdonald, Goines, Novacek and Walker124,Reference Wise, Radua and Via125]. There may be, and indeed there are very likely to be, many other areas of the brain that place an individual at risk of having a mental disorder. In fact, it is increasingly likely that it is not so much the structure of particular parts of the brain but the connections between different areas that cause these problems [Reference Hinton, Lahey and Villalta-Gil126]. So general psychopathology, or the p factor, or perhaps most accurately the risk of mental disorder, at the level of the brain, may be to do with irregular connectivity in the cortex of the brain. This hypothesis is consistent with the growing consensus that psychopathology is best seen as a dysfunction of brain networks rather than of localized brain regions [Reference Protzner, An, Jirsa, Diwadkar and Eickhoff127]. A study in which high-quality resting-state brain scans were obtained in 6593 children aged 9–10 years found that a high p factor was associated with reduced connectivity within the default mode network (DMN), and increased connectivity between the DMN and multiple control networks [Reference Sripada, Angstadt and Taxali128]. The DMN is active when the mind is not engaged in any specific cognitive task—that is, when a person is “mind-wandering”. The DMN overlaps with and extends beyond the network of brain regions involved in mentalizing. One study showed that a set of tasks concerned with social cognition (i.e., reasoning about the mental states of other persons) activates the DMN [Reference Jack, Dawson and Begany129]. The DMN is activated in thought processes such as daydreaming, imagining the perspectives of others, imagining the future, and recalling autobiographical memories [Reference Ekhtiari and Paulus130], and is generally considered to subserve mentalizing. In other words, vulnerability to psychopathology, as measured by a high p factor, is associated with anomalies in connectivity in the neural network that is implicated in mentalizing processes.

Neuroscience research has consistently implicated two related major domains of functioning across most mental health disorders. The first is emotional dysregulation. Poorly managed emotions interfere with purposeful activity [Reference Beauchaine131]. People who regulate their emotions well have an accurate idea about the risks of a given situation that triggered emotion, and can direct their attention to what they need to do to cope with it, use their attention to focus but also to distract themselves if focusing is unlikely to help, and reassess the risks as the results of their actions unfold [Reference Gross and Gross132]. Emotional dysregulation has been shown to be a feature of almost every psychiatric diagnostic condition [Reference Beauchaine and Cicchetti133]. Researchers believe that a weakness in emotion regulation can amplify emotional experience that is going on in the background, cause distortions in the way children see and experience social situations, and lead to intense emotional reactions just because the person is anticipating and experiencing an intense feeling. It can generate inappropriate feelings and lead to sometimes dramatic actions to avoid unpleasant intense emotion.

The second general domain that is consistently associated with mental disorder is executive function [Reference McTeague, Huemer and Carreon134]. Executive function is the capacity to control thought and generally direct information in the appropriate direction relevant to any specific task; it includes self-regulation, decision making, sequencing of actions, planning, prioritizing, and navigating new tasks [Reference Banich135]. These cognitive processes are deemed to be essential for healthy functioning. The quality of executive function is thought to affect the p factor. Individuals with a high p factor score, signifying what we might term high persistent psychological distress, are oversensitive to difficult social interactions, and they find it hard to reliably interpret the reasons for others’ actions and put out of their mind potentially upsetting memories of experiences. This state leaves them vulnerable to emotional storms.

To return to the role of mentalizing, we can see that because mentalizing supports executive function and affect regulation, both emotional dysregulation and poor executive function—the two areas reliably associated with psychopathology—are associated with poor mentalizing. We do not know where cause and effect lie in relation to mentalizing here; from a pragmatic clinical perspective, whether mentalizing problems are the consequence or the cause of emotional dysregulation and failures in executive function may be something of a “chicken-and-egg” question. The issue, clinically, is that all of these processes are closely linked to one another. The restoration of the capacity to think about mental states, both in oneself and in others, may be critical to making affect regulation and more protective executive function possible; this provides the rationale for the application of MBT approaches to a range of mental health disorders. In the next section, we shall set out why we consider mentalizing to have such great significance for healthy functioning.

Mentalizing, Social Learning, and Epistemic Trust

We have defined epistemic trust, which is a key part of mentalizing theory, as “trust in the authenticity and personal relevance of interpersonally transmitted knowledge about how the social environment works and how best to navigate it” (Fonagy et al. [Reference Fonagy, Campbell and Bateman136], p. 177). We have argued that mentalizing has a specific function in enabling individuals to take a position in relation to having epistemic trust in others—something that is vital if they are to benefit fully from opportunities to learn from others about the world in general and the social world, including themselves, in particular. We suggest that this is one of the reasons why disruptions in mentalizing appear to be so detrimental to social functioning [Reference Fonagy, Campbell and Constantinou6] and so closely linked to vulnerability to any long-term mental disorder, and why they appear to be associated with the p factor [Reference Sripada, Angstadt and Taxali128]. To survive in the socially complex networks that humans inhabit, we have to be able to learn from other people [Reference Masten, Morelli and Eisenberger137]. In learning the skills that we need to survive, we observe and listen to experts, we avoid punishments and gain rewards by imitating others, we make complex strategic decisions after taking others’ experience and views into account, and throughout all of this we learn about ourselves through social interactions. Learning efficiently from others is a central part of human existence. The transmission of information from parents to children, as well as between peers, constitutes a core mechanism of adaptive cultural learning, but can also cause maladaptive behaviors, such as antisocial actions, exaggerated avoidance, and anxiety [Reference Espinosa, Golkar, Olsson, Gilead and Ochsner138].

The development of mentalizing is intricately related to epistemic trust and openness to social learning—that is, the ability to learn from others by trying to understand what they have to teach us. Human infants quickly learn to infer a model’s goal and copy successful (but not failed) actions, but non-human primates acquire this capacity earlier than humans. Humans take a slightly different approach to learning. By the age of 3, children start showing what is called over-imitation, by which we mean that young children readily learn and copy behaviors that are unnecessarily complicated and do not seem to have a more functional purpose, if they seem to be the “right” way of doing things (a simple example would be learning to eat with a spoon rather than the faster and more efficient method of using one’s hands) [Reference Hoehl, Keupp and Schleihauf139]. For a period, young children are prone to unselectively and unreflectively copy the behavior and acquire the beliefs of the majority of people with whom they interact [Reference Lyons, Damrosch and Lin140], and will copy a person’s failed as well as successful attempts to complete a task [Reference Huang, Heyes and Charman141]—something that non-human primates have not been observed to do [Reference Clay and Tennie142]. The reasons for this are that what the child is learning when, for example, they learn to use a spoon is complex. They are not only learning how to break up food and transport it to their mouth [Reference Gergely, Csibra, Enfield and Levinson143]. In fact, most small children find it easier to use their hands to eat—but learning to use a spoon correctly is one tiny example of the huge ocean of cultural and social knowledge that infants are born into. Trusting the demonstration by reliable elders that spoons are the correct way to eat, and copying how to use them, is an example of the small steps the young child starts to make in learning to navigate the world. Relatively inefficient routines are frequently socially transmitted as part of the cultural knowledge shared by members of a child’s community [Reference Clay and Tennie142]. Acquiring such routines is therefore an important developmental task that allows children to become members of their cultural group. The term “over-imitation” does not signify that children imitate knowledgeable adults when they should not. Rather, the tendency to over-imitate actions that are cognitively opaque is a cultural learning strategy, presumably identified through natural selection, based on the assumption that learning to do things that one does not fully understand is beneficial because they are culturally relevant, despite their opacity to the learner [Reference Altınok, Hernik, Király and Gergely144]. Preschool children’s detailed mimicking of new behaviors may represent an openness to learning novel skills, unusual social norms, and rituals, and thus to affiliating with members of their community [Reference Nielsen145]. Over-imitating declines as mentalizing emerges, but it remains part of culture and of how humans learn, because a comprehensive understanding of causation is beyond humans even in relation to the physical world. The predisposition to over-imitate is sustained into adulthood [Reference Flynn and Smith146], and is enhanced by the social context [Reference Gruber, Deschenaux, Frick and Clement147,Reference Marsh, Ropar and Hamilton148].

This account of social learning is relevant to our thinking about mentalizing and MBT because we suggest that learning and trusting others so that we can learn from them—that is, epistemic trust—is enabled by mentalizing. We have said elsewhere, “If I feel that I am understood, I will be disposed to learn from the person who understood me, who I feel is a trustworthy potential collaborator. This will include learning about myself but also learning about others and about the world I live in” (Fonagy et al. [Reference Fonagy, Campbell and Constantinou6], p. 7). The outcome of this process is what we have called an epistemic match. This sounds like quite a simple proposition, but there are several mentalizing stages involved in this process. It requires the listener to (a) have a coherent enough sense of self to be able to recognize the match in the first place, (b) recognize the image of themselves that the communicator is describing to them (in a situation where the communicator is somehow expressing their own perception of the listener), and (c) judge the similarity or difference between their own self-perception and how the communicator is describing them. In addition, the process requires the communicator to have mentalized the listener well enough to have a good understanding of them. Thus epistemic trust as a social process depends on mentalizing on the part of both the listener and the communicator.

When co-mentalizing as occurs in an epistemic match is achieved, a particular subjective experience of social cognition is generated, which has been labelled the “we-mode.” The we-mode is a mental state that describes an interpersonal experience of being enjoined with someone else, where intentional states are shared with a common purpose [Reference Gallotti and Frith149,Reference Higgins150]. The we-mode triggers trust, which in turn triggers the potential for learning from the trusted source. The task of restoring mentalizing in MBT is thus undertaken to (re)open the possibility of experiencing the we-mode and an epistemic match that is crucial for enabling the social learning that underlies social functioning. In addition, there is a relational aspect to the we-mode, which generates the feelings of both agency and belonging that arise from a sense of “jointness,” or the epistemic connectedness that shared intentionality creates. It is important to note that the we-mode is not some kind of merging of minds. The self, with its full sense of agency, remains important; in fact, in the absence of a robust self that temporarily subverts itself for a joint purpose, access to the we-mode may be only illusory and achieved by self-distortion. The we-mode is not an abdication of the awareness of separate mental states, nor is it some sort of psychic fusion. Rather, it involves holding in mind the idea that others are separate “agents or persons just as real as oneself” (Tomasello [Reference Tomasello151], p. 56), with whom we momentarily share a view of the world, an ambition, a plan, or an understanding that supersedes our individual perspective. Indeed, the benefits of the we-mode accrue from separate minds joining together to focus on a shared object; joint attention brings new knowledge and perspectives that a single mind cannot [Reference Tomasello4]. We shall describe in more detail in the next sections how disruptions in epistemic trust and in experiencing the we-mode are implicated in the development of psychopathology.

Trust in BPD

Mistrust as a Core Feature of BPD

An investigation of core dysfunctional beliefs in 288 individuals with a diagnosis of personality disorder found that the item that best distinguished individuals with a diagnosis of BPD from those with other personality disorders on the 126-item Personality Belief Questionnaire was “I cannot trust other people”[Reference Butler, Brown, Beck and Grisham152]. Another study comparing patients with BPD, patients with mood disorder, and control participants without a mental health diagnosis used a different self-rated instrument, the Trust Scenario Questionnaire, which taps into interpersonal trust between one person and another, and reported that patients with BPD scored significantly lower than either of the other groups [Reference Botsford, Schulze, Bohlander and Renneberg153]. These robust findings justify both a behavioral and a developmental focus on issues of trust in BPD, as well as consideration of the implications for other social and emotional aspects of the disorder.

Given that mistrust is such a key feature of BPD, it is hardly surprising that individuals with this disorder rate faces they do not know as less trustworthy and take longer to make these ratings [Reference Fertuck, Grinband and Stanley154], as well as identifying less trustworthiness and approachability in faces [Reference Nicol, Pope and Sprengelmeyer155]. A more recent study replicated the untrustworthiness bias among BPD patients, who showed more biased trustworthiness and less ability to discriminate facial expressions than a matched control group [Reference Fertuck, Grinband and Mann156]. The BPD group also exhibited slower reaction times when appraising ambiguous trustworthiness compared with the controls. Furthermore, the neural activation of BPD patients during trustworthiness ratings, assessed using functional magnetic resonance imaging, evidenced less activity in the anterior insula and lateral prefrontal cortex compared with the controls. This decrease in activity was proportional to the degree of trustworthiness bias and impaired discriminability demonstrated by both the BPD patients and the controls.

Development of Epistemic Mistrust

We suggest that one of the consequences of childhood adversity is that it can lead to the establishment of epistemic mistrust—that is, the misattribution of intentions and the assumption of malevolent motives behind another person’s actions. Campbell and colleagues recently published a study of a self-report trait measure of epistemic trust that distinguishes three dimensions—epistemic trust, mistrust, and credulity (an indication of lack of discernment) [Reference Campbell, Tanzer and Saunders157]. The study found that mistrust and heightened credulity characterize individuals with histories of childhood adversity. We suggest that, for these individuals, attachment trauma following neglect in childhood generates a general background expectation of an imminent rupture of communication between them (as either listeners or communicators) and others in any social exchange. When an individual has a history of being repeatedly exposed to the communication of unreliable or ill-intentioned information, they may learn to reject these communications [Reference Mascaro and Sperber158,Reference Fonagy, Luyten and Allison159]. Orme et al. observed BPD symptoms from admission to treatment and to discharge in a sample of adolescents, and reported a significant negative correlation between BPD symptoms at admission and participants’ self-reported trust in their parents [Reference Orme, Bowersox and Vanwoerden160]. The findings support the assumption that lower levels of trust in their parents may lead to an individual developing stable and rigid dysfunctional beliefs, and untrusting dispositions toward others in general, during adolescence and adulthood.

The social context of neuro-economic tasks, which tend to quantify interpersonal trust in monetary terms, seems to affect the performance of BPD patients. A study contrasted the performance of patients with major depressive disorder or BPD in two contexts—social interaction and playing against a computer [Reference Preuss, Brandle and Hager161]. BPD patients behaved more inconsistently, showing volatile and unpredictable reactions, but only in the condition involving social interactions. When exposed to social situations, those with BPD showed a proneness to perceive counterparts as threatening, and this perception activated untrusting behaviors with trustees. Social interactions trigger the mentalizing system and bring forth expectations of interpersonal behaviors about the counterpart, which might explain the BPD patients’ inconsistent behaviors in this study.

Ebert et al. followed up findings related to a potential mediating role of the neuropeptide oxytocin [Reference Ebert, Kolb and Heller162], where abnormalities have been linked to lower interpersonal trust [Reference Theodoridou, Rowe, Penton-Voak and Rogers163]. BPD patients and controls were randomly assigned to receive oxytocin or a placebo, and were then invited to play a trust game. In this game there is an investor with a fixed number of points (say, 20), who is asked to give whatever number of points they would like to a trustee, who will look after the points for them. The trustee automatically triples the number and then has to decide how many points to give back to the investor. So if the investor gives 10 points, the trustee then has 30 points to play with and may, for example, give back half, leaving the investor with 10 points remaining from the start of the game and a further 15 points, while the trustee now has 15 points. Another round then takes place. A lot of mentalizing is required in this game. Intentions and causes that underlie lending/giving behavior between partners are given meaning and determine the interactional attitudes in the next round—one person’s behavior (how many points they give) has an effect on the other’s mind, which subsequently determines their behavior, and so on. This study found no correlation between childhood trauma scores and trust behavior in the control group of people without BPD, whether they had been given oxytocin or the placebo. The BPD patients who had a history of childhood neglect showed more limited trusting behavior and gave less money to the trustee, but only in the oxytocin condition, which activated interpersonal relational experience. It seems that increased oxytocin levels reduce trusting behaviors in BPD patients with a history of early parental neglect. These patterns of findings make sense if we assume that oxytocin generally acts to make social stimuli more salient [Reference Shamay-Tsoory and Abu-Akel164], which may have a negative impact on individuals with a history of social adversity.

These findings and assumptions about the developmental roots of the high level of mistrust in individuals with BPD can be linked to their rejection sensitivity. The experience of being rejected could explain their tendency to mistrust. There is evidence from a study of a non-clinical population that there is a negative correlation between the facial appraisal of trust from unfamiliar faces and BPD features, which is explained by rejection sensitivity [Reference Miano, Fertuck, Arntz and Stanley165]. Rejection sensitivity, particularly its emotional aspect (anxiety and anger) [Reference Sato, Fonagy and Luyten111, Reference Sato, Fonagy and Luyten112], is likely to have a role in mediating the relationship between mistrust and BPD [Reference Preti, Casini and Richetin166].

The triggering of emotional arousal would be expected to decrease interpersonal trust. Masland and Hooley studied groups of non-clinical adults with higher or lower numbers of BPD descriptive symptoms who rated the trustworthiness of unfamiliar faces after completing an affective priming paradigm in which they looked at negative, neutral, or positive images [Reference Masland and Hooley167]. Participants with higher numbers of BPD symptoms made less trusting appraisals. The negative priming images had a greater impact on the high-BPD group than on the low-BPD group, which suggests that the influence of negative emotional states on trust appraisal performance is stronger in individuals with more BPD symptoms.

One study investigated interpersonal trust in romantic relationships involving individuals with BPD compared with that in romantic relationships involving controls [Reference Miano, Fertuck, Roepke and Dziobek168]. The authors asked heterosexual couples in which the women had been diagnosed with BPD, and control couples with no psychiatric history, to discuss three topics: neutral (favorite films), personal (personal fears), and relationship-threatening (possible reasons for separation from their partner). After each discussion, the participants rated their partner’s trustworthiness. The authors expected a lower appraisal of the partner’s trustworthiness in the BPD couples than the control couples, especially after the relationship-threatening discussions. Women with BPD expressed lower trust toward their partner following the personal or relationship-threatening discussions but not the neutral discussions. These findings fit with the assumption that emotional arousal has a negative impact on mentalizing in individuals with BPD [Reference Fonagy and Luyten169].

Mentalizing and Social Rejection

Social rejection, when repeated and severe, is implicated in the development of almost all psychiatric disorders [Reference Hsu and Jarcho170]. Unusual reactivity and vulnerability to social rejection is used as a criterion in the diagnosis of social anxiety disorder, major depression, BPD, avoidant personality disorder, premenstrual dysphoric disorder, bulimia nervosa, body dysmorphic disorder, acute suicidal ideation, and substance/alcohol use disorders, although the specific behavioral, affective, and neural responses to rejection may differ among these disorders [Reference Reinhard, Dewald-Kaufmann and Wustenberg71]. Social rejection or acceptance is directly linked to self-perception, which is dependent on mentalizing and is at the core of many mood, anxiety, and personality disorders.

Feeling rejected interacts with mentalizing. If we know that someone who appears to be deliberately ignoring us is actually unusually shy, we can rethink why they are avoiding eye contact with us and feel less rejected by them, because rejection normally has something to do with how we think the other person might be thinking and feeling about us. Over-interpreting reactions that are actually neutral as distant, cold, or uninterested might lead us to ask “Why don’t they like me?” Many instances of rejection are linked to how another’s thoughts, feelings, or intentions are perceived.

Social rejection has been investigated in the exclusion condition of the Cyberball paradigm (a virtual ball-toss game), which consistently elicits feelings of social distress. Here, rejection consists of participants observing that they are not being passed a virtual ball that is being passed by two avatars belonging to presumed players who are strangers to the participant. The intensity of the experience of social rejection is such that Eisenberger and Lieberman have suggested that the social signaling system may have piggybacked on to the neural system involved in perceiving physical pain (the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and anterior insula) [Reference Eisenberger and Lieberman171, Reference Eisenberger172]. The affective pain signal indicates the grave risk of broken social bonds that could compromise access to shared resources, support, and beneficial social relationships.

Sahi and Eisenberger have suggested that the neural underpinnings of social rejection entail the activation of the mentalizing neural network [Reference Sahi, Eisenberger, Gilead and Ochsner173], based on findings that the dorsomedial and ventromedial prefrontal cortex and precuneus are generally active in the exclusion phase of Cyberball and other rejection paradigms [Reference Vijayakumar, Cheng and Pfeifer174]. They also argue that the development of self-conscious emotions and fear of social rejection emerge in early childhood, associated with increased mentalizing competence and maturation of mentalizing networks. The characteristic hypermentalizing of adolescents may account for their heightened sensitivity to social rejection rooted in greater responsiveness of the mentalizing network at this age. Neural responses to peer acceptance and rejection in socially anxious early adolescents are greater than those in older adolescents [Reference Smith, Nelson and Kircanski175] or in adults [Reference Jarcho, Tanofsky-Kraff and Nelson176]. Although Sahi and Eisenberger [Reference Sahi, Eisenberger, Gilead and Ochsner173] make a case for mentalizing anomalies mediating the abnormal reactions to social rejection of patients with schizophrenia [Reference Gradin, Waiter and Kumar177] and individuals with autism [Reference Sebastian, Blakemore and Charman178], it is not conclusive.

There is better evidence to support the existence of mentalizing anomalies in individuals with a diagnosis of BPD who respond atypically to social rejection. A significant number of studies have linked rejection sensitivity to BPD (e.g., [Reference Feldman and Downey179Reference Zhang, Hu, Ding and Li181]). A large meta-analysis of 28 Cyberball studies confirmed a robust disposition of individuals with BPD to expect rejection [Reference Cavicchioli and Maffei182]. Individuals diagnosed with BPD perceived that they received fewer ball tosses compared with the control groups in both the exclusion condition (when they did not receive the ball as often as others) and inclusion condition (when they received the ball more often) of Cyberball. They also showed more overall negative affectivity after exposures to rejection, and an even larger effect following social inclusion. Notably, the reaction to social inclusion in a situation where participants had been excluded distinguished individuals with BPD from typical controls. Mentalizing theory would understand this effect as part of an inability to overcome a pervasive pre-existing bias toward rejection underpinning hypervigilance about the intentions of others, and an inability to mentalize a change in attitude in the context of acceptance. Evidence consistent with this hypothesis includes the findings that individuals with BPD were impaired in processing positive social feedback but integrated negative feedback better than controls [Reference Korn, La Rosee, Heekeren and Roepke183], experienced reduced positive emotions such as pride and happiness after reading self-relevant appreciative sentences [Reference Reichenberger, Eibl and Pfaltz184], and changed their social expectations in response to negative, but not positive, social feedback [Reference Liebke, Koppe and Bungert185]. These observations are critical from a clinical perspective, where the therapist often waits in vain for a positive reaction from the patient in response to their unconditional acceptance and interventions aimed at increasing therapeutic inclusion, only to encounter the resentment and even deeper suspicion that arises from the patient’s confusion in the face of the experience of being accepted.

Trauma and Mentalizing

The mentalizing model originated from understanding of the psychological consequences of trauma (see also Chapter 11), with the assumption that one possible outcome of childhood adversity was a limited capacity for mentalizing, driven by anxiety. A child’s fear of understanding the mental states of an individual who poses a genuine threat to them is understandable, and generalization of this fear to other minds might be expected [Reference Allen, Fonagy, Bateman, Vermetten and Lanius186]. Considerable evidence has accumulated to suggest that limitations in mentalizing are commonly associated with the experience of trauma, particularly post-traumatic stress disorder. Differences in mentalizing have been demonstrated in tests of emotional intelligence [Reference Janke, Driessen and Behnia187], tests of empathy and compassion [Reference Palgi, Klein and Shamay-Tsoory188,Reference Mazza, Tempesta and Pino189], and tests developed specifically to measure cognitive mentalizing, such as the Faux Pas test and the Strange Stories task [Reference Nietlisbach, Maercker, Rossler and Haker190,Reference Mazza, Giusti and Albanese191]. Our theoretical approach predicts a bias against internal cues to mental states (mental-state understanding) but a potential hypersensitivity to external indicators of mental states (observations) to balance the defensive avoidance of “looking behind” the external. In line with this prediction, patients with a history of trauma appear to show no or only small deficits in emotion-recognition tests [Reference Nazarov, Frewen and Oremus192Reference Bell, Colhoun and Frampton194].

Our assumption in relation to childhood trauma is rooted in the psychoanalytic history of MBT, but a rare aspect of this history is shared by modern psychiatry. Anna Freud and Dorothy Burlingham, who observed children’s responses to the bombing of London during the Second World War, noted that the objective danger a child was in appeared to be less predictive of a phobic reaction than the mother’s level of anxiety [Reference Freud and Burlingham195]. Their observations antedated the substantial interest in the vicarious learning of children in the context of social referencing [Reference Klinnert, Campos, Sorce, Plutchhik and Kellerman196,Reference Sorce, Emde, Campos and Klinnert197] and other related, particularly facially transmitted, ways of deriving emotional information about risk and danger [Reference Askew and Field198Reference Olsson, Knapska and Lindstrom200].

In our developmental approach to trauma, we have suggested that adversity becomes traumatic when it is compounded by the sense that one’s mind is alone. Normally, an accessible other mind provides the social referencing that enables one to frame a frightening and otherwise overwhelming experience [Reference Allen, Fonagy, Bateman, Vermetten and Lanius186]. Recent experimental studies have provided strong support for this view. A study of conditioning found that vicarious safety learning (when participants watch a calm-looking demonstrator modeling safety) leads to better attenuation of a conditioned threat response than traditional direct safety learning training when a model is not present [Reference Golkar, Haaker, Selbing and Olsson201]. This observation suggests that the mind is programmed to be attuned to socially accessible agents so as to judge the threat response through the availability of another person, such as an attachment figure.

Three Communication Systems

In drawing out some of the clinical implications of the theory we have covered so far in this chapter, we have developed the idea that there are three “communication systems” which are associated with effective therapeutic help [Reference Bateman, Campbell, Luyten and Fonagy202]. These communication systems do not just apply to MBT; rather, we suggest that any form of meaningful psychotherapeutic help tends to involve the communication, internalization, and (re-)application of new forms of learning about oneself, and about oneself in relation to other people.

Communication System 1: The Teaching and Learning of Content

All different therapeutic schools activate the first communication system when the clinician conveys to the patient a model for understanding the mind that feels relevant to the patient and makes them feel recognized and understood. The experience of being recognized as an independent agent in this way reduces the patient’s epistemic vigilance and begins to prime them to become open to social learning. The clinician’s capacity to mentalize the patient is crucial to this system, as it requires the clinician to apply and communicate their therapeutic model in a way that is experienced as meaningful by the patient, creating an epistemic match. The initial stages of MBT, particularly the MBT-Introductory group (see Chapter 4), attest to the importance of this communication system, and the ability of the clinician to make mentalizing relevant to how patients think about themselves and others is key to engagement in treatment. This is the beginning of patients rethinking themselves and seeing themselves in a different light.

Communication System 2: The Re-Emergence of Mentalizing

When the patient is once again open to social communication in contexts that had previously been blighted by epistemic disruption, they show increased interest in the clinician’s mind and use of thoughts and feelings, which stimulates and strengthens the patient’s capacity for mentalizing: “How does this person see me as they do?” The not-knowing stance of the clinician, with assiduous focus on the patient’s experience, kick-starts this process. The emergence of mentalizing in the patient leads to a “virtuous circle” in which curiosity about mental states and social learning through greater epistemic openness support each other within the therapeutic relationship.

Communication System 3: Applying Social Learning in the Wider Environment

Being mentalized by another person frees the patient from their state of temporary or long-term social isolation, and (re-)activates their capacity to learn. This frees the patient to grow in the context of relationships outside therapy. This view implies that it is not just the content and techniques of the therapy, or the insight acquired in the course of treatment, that are key to its success; perhaps primarily it is the patient’s capacity for social learning and thinking about mental states that improves their functioning as they become able to “use” their environment in a different way. A further implication is, of course, that there may be a need to intervene at the level of the patient’s social environment when necessary or appropriate.

Although we have described the three communication systems as forming a neat, numerically linear sequence, in fact—particularly for patients who are receiving fairly lengthy treatment—the process is rather less linear and straightforward. There are inevitable disruptions, ruptures, and work on repair across treatment, which may involve the activation of different communication systems, or the overlapping activation of more than one system at once. An example may involve a patient receiving a knock-back in a close personal relationship outside therapy, which creates feelings of intense distress and triggers the emergence of one or more of the non-mentalizing modes described earlier in this chapter. In such a state of mind, the patient may regard anything that the clinician says as meaningless, useless, or provocative; communication breaks down and any expectation that the patient will take in or apply social learning is likely to fail. In this situation, the clinician needs to go back to the first communication system, in which the patient’s state of mind is understood and recognized, and their capacity for thinking and learning is gently reinstated.

Summary of Empirical Research on MBT

In 2020, a Cochrane Review identified MBT as one of two therapies (the other being dialectical behavior therapy) with a reasonable evidence base for the treatment of BPD [Reference Storebø, Stoffers-Winterling and Völlm203]. A meta-analysis that included 33 randomized controlled trials (RCTs) of specialized psychotherapies for BPD compared with non-specialized psychotherapies for adult patients diagnosed with BPD supported the efficacy of MBT in patients with BPD [Reference Cristea, Gentili and Cotet204]. Another meta-analysis of psychological treatments for BPD, which covered 87 studies, found that MBT (along with schema therapy and reduced dialectical behavior therapy) was associated with higher than average effect sizes compared with treatment as usual (TAU), which was associated with lower than average effect sizes [Reference Rameckers, Verhoef and Grasman205]. A review of RCTs found that MBT for BPD tended to be associated with medium to large or very large effect sizes on a range of outcome measures, including BPD symptoms, educational attainment, and interpersonal functioning [Reference Volkert, Hauschild and Taubner206]. A systematic review similarly concluded that MBT is associated with significant improvements in BPD symptom severity, comorbid symptom severity, and quality of life [Reference Vogt and Norman207].

Bateman and Fonagy tested the effectiveness of MBT as a treatment for BPD in a series of RCTs. A day-hospital program of MBT was investigated in outcome studies culminating in an 8-year follow-up study—the longest follow-up of treatment for BPD conducted to date [Reference Bateman and Fonagy208]. Compared with TAU, MBT achieved decreases in suicide attempts, emergency-room visits, inpatient admissions, medication and outpatient treatment use, and impulsivity. Far fewer patients in the MBT group than in the TAU group met the criteria for BPD at follow-up (13% versus 87%). Patients in the MBT group also showed greater improvements in interpersonal and occupational functioning.

An intensive outpatient MBT program was found to be more effective than structured clinical management for BPD at the end of the 18-month treatment period [Reference Bateman and Fonagy209], particularly for patients with more than two personality disorder diagnoses [Reference Bateman and Fonagy210]. Compared with TAU, the outpatient MBT treatment resulted in lower rates of suicidal behavior and non-suicidal self-injury, and fewer hospitalizations. The primary outcome measure of 6-month periods free of suicidal behaviors, severe self-injurious behaviors, and hospitalization showed improvements from 0% to 43% in the structured clinical management group and 73% in the MBT group. In addition, the MBT group showed improved social adjustment and reduced depression, symptom distress, and interpersonal distress [Reference Bateman and Fonagy209]. An 8-year follow-up of this study found that, of the 73% of the sample who agreed to participate, a significantly higher proportion of patients in the MBT group (74%, compared with 51% in the structured clinical management control group) still met the primary criteria for recovery [Reference Bateman, Constantinou, Fonagy and Holzer211]. Finally, an RCT of MBT for comorbid BPD and antisocial personality disorder found that MBT was effective in reducing symptoms such as hostility, paranoia, anger, and frequency of self-injurious behavior and suicide attempts, and achieved improvements in negative mood, general psychiatric symptoms, interpersonal problems, and social adjustment [Reference Bateman, O’Connell and Lorenzini212].

An RCT in Denmark that investigated the efficacy of MBT compared with a less intensive manualized supportive group therapy in patients with BPD found that MBT achieved better results in terms of clinician-rated Global Assessment of Functioning [Reference Jørgensen, Freund and Boye213]. These results were sustained 18 months later [Reference Jørgensen, Bøye and Andersen214]. In a second study in Denmark, patients who were treated with partial hospitalization followed by group MBT showed significant improvements after treatment (average duration 2 years) on a range of measures, including Global Assessment of Functioning, hospitalizations, and vocational status, with further improvements at 2-year follow-up [Reference Petersen, Toft and Christensen215]. A multi-site RCT in the Netherlands that compared day-hospital MBT with specialist TAU in patients with BPD found that both forms of treatment were effective, although the MBT program was associated with higher patient acceptability, indicated by significantly lower early dropout (9% for MBT versus 34% for TAU) [Reference Laurenssen, Luyten and Kikkert216].

Another study in the Netherlands investigated the effectiveness of an 18-month manualized program of MBT in 45 patients with severe BPD [Reference Bales, Bateman, Bateman and Fonagy217]. Patients showed significant positive changes in symptom distress, social and interpersonal functioning, and personality pathology and functioning, and effect sizes were moderate to large; however, it should be noted that this study did not use a control group. A naturalistic longitudinal study in Norway that compared psychodynamic group-based treatment and MBT found that greater clinical severity of BPD was associated with poorer outcomes in the group-based treatment, whereas it had no significant effect on treatment outcome in the MBT group [Reference Kvarstein, Pedersen and Folmo218]. This observation supports Bateman and Fonagy’s finding that MBT may be particularly indicated for patients with more severe BPD [Reference Bateman and Fonagy210].

An RCT compared a form of MBT for eating disorders and specialist supportive clinical management for patients with eating disorders and symptoms of BPD. This study had a high dropout rate, with only 15 of the 68 participants who were eligible for randomization (22%) completing the 18-month follow-up, making the results difficult to interpret. However, MBT was associated with a greater reduction in patients’ concerns about body shape and weight as assessed by the Eating Disorder Examination [Reference Robinson, Hellier and Barrett219].

In an RCT of MBT for adolescents (MBT-A) (see also Chapter 14), 80 adolescents who presented to mental health services and had engaged in self-harm in the preceding month were randomized to MBT or TAU; 97% were diagnosed with depression, and 73% met the criteria for BPD. At the end of 12 months of treatment, MBT-A was found to be more successful than TAU in reducing self-harm and symptoms of depression. On the basis of self-report, the recovery rate was 44% for MBT-A and 17% for TAU, whereas on the basis of interview assessment the recovery rates were 57% and 32%, respectively. There was also a greater reduction in depressive symptoms and BPD diagnoses and traits in the MBT-A group. Both groups showed significant reductions in self-harm and risk-taking behavior, based on Risk-Taking and Self-Harm Inventory scores, following both a linear and a quadratic pattern. The group × time interaction term was also significant for both self-harm and risk taking, indicating that the linear decrease was significantly greater for the MBT-A group on both variables. At 12 months, self-harm scores were significantly lower for the MBT-A group [Reference Rossouw and Fonagy220]. However, a recent Cochrane Review of treatments for self-harm in adolescents has suggested that more evidence for effectiveness is needed for MBT-A [Reference Witt, Hetrick and Rajaram221].

An uncontrolled pilot study in Denmark of 34 female adolescents with BPD who participated in 12 months of structured mentalization-based group therapy found that of the 25 adolescents who completed the study, 23 displayed improvements in BPD symptoms, depression, self-harm, peer and parent attachment, mentalizing, and general psychopathology. Enhanced trust in peers and parents in combination with improved mentalizing capacity was associated with a greater reduction in borderline symptoms, pointing to a candidate mechanism for the efficacy of the treatment [Reference Bo, Sharp and Beck222]. On the basis of this pilot study, an RCT was developed to compare group MBT with TAU (which involved individual supportive care) for 112 adolescents with BPD in an outpatient setting; this study found that both treatments had similar effectiveness. The authors suggest that group MBT might be better indicated as an early-stage intervention, and that a group MBT program also including individual sessions should be tested [Reference Beck, Bo and Jorgensen223]. Finally, a naturalistic multi-informant study of MBT in 118 adolescent inpatients with personality pathology symptoms found that the participants improved on general and personality pathology measures, and in terms of health-related and generic quality of life. These improvements were clinically important as well as statistically significant, particularly in internalizing domains [Reference Jørgensen, Storebø and Bo224].

Concluding Remarks

In Part II of this book we shall discuss the translation of this developmental, evidence-based theory into clinical practice. To summarize, the central question is how to help patients to generate robust and effective mentalizing that will be conducive to constructive personal and social functioning in their life. Better mentalizing, particularly when attachment processes are activated, fuels epistemic learning within the social world and allows personal change over time. MBT is designed to stimulate this pathway of change, and is organized as a coherent set of interventions that are systematically applied over time to help the patient to learn about and practice mentalizing skills in their everyday life, while taking into account how attachment anxiety and other stressors can undermine the stability and effective use of mentalizing.

References

Meyer, ML, Davachi, L, Ochsner, KN, Lieberman, MD. Evidence that default network connectivity during rest consolidates social information. Cereb Cortex 2019; 29: 1910–20.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. Religion is natural. Dev Sci 2007; 10: 147–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunbar, RI, Shultz, S. Evolution in the social brain. Science 2007; 317: 1344–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. Becoming Human: A Theory of Ontogeny. Cambridge, MA: The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press, 2019.Google Scholar
Park, BK, Kim, M, Young, L. An examination of accurate versus “biased” mentalizing in moral economic decision-making. In: Gilead, M, Ochsner, K, eds. The Neural Basis of Mentalizing. Cham, Switzerland: Springer, 2021; 537–53.Google Scholar
Fonagy, P, Campbell, C, Constantinou, M et al. Culture and psychopathology: an attempt at reconsidering the role of social learning. Dev Psychopathol 2022; 34: 1205–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaefer, JD, Caspi, A, Belsky, DW et al. Enduring mental health: prevalence and prediction. J Abnorm Psychol 2017; 126: 212–24.Google Scholar
Nisbett, RE, Ross, L. Human Interface: Strategies and Shortcomings of Social Judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1980.Google Scholar
Nisbett, RE, Wilson, TD. Telling more than we can know: verbal reports on mental processes. Psychol Rev 1977; 84: 231–59.Google Scholar
Johansson, P, Hall, L, Sikstrom, S, Olsson, A. Failure to detect mismatches between intention and outcome in a simple decision task. Science 2005; 310: 116–9.Google Scholar
Samson, D, Apperly, IA, Braithwaite, JJ et al. Seeing it their way: evidence for rapid and involuntary computation of what other people see. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 2010; 36: 1255–66.Google Scholar
Beeney, JE, Hallquist, MN, Ellison, WD, Levy, KN. Self-other disturbance in borderline personality disorder: neural, self-report, and performance-based evidence. Personal Disord 2016; 7: 2839.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradford, EE, Jentzsch, I, Gomez, JC. From self to social cognition: Theory of Mind mechanisms and their relation to Executive Functioning. Cognition 2015; 138: 2134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gordon, RM. Simulation, predictive coding, and the shared world. In: Gilead, M, Ochsner, K, eds. The Neural Basis of Mentalizing. Cham, Switzerland: Springer, 2021; 237–56.Google Scholar
Cooley, CH. Human Nature and the Social Order, revised ed. New York, NY: Shocken Books, 1964/1902.Google Scholar
Mead, GH. Mind, Self, and Society. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press, 1934.Google Scholar
Fonagy, P, Gergely, G, Jurist, E, Target, M. Developmental issues in normal adolescence and adolescent breakdown. In: Affect Regulation, Mentalization, and the Development of the Self. New York, NY: Other Press LLC, 2002; 317–40.Google Scholar
Malle, BF. The tree of social cognition: hierarchically organized capacities of mentalizing. In: Gilead, M, Ochsner, K, eds. The Neural Basis of Mentalizing. Cham, Switzerland: Springer, 2021; 337–70.Google Scholar
Osterhaus, C, Koerber, S, Sodian, B. Scaling of advanced Theory-of-Mind tasks. Child Dev 2016; 87: 1971–91.Google Scholar
Atance, CM, Metcalf, JL, Martin-Ordas, G, Walker, CL. Young children’s causal explanations are biased by post-action associative information. Dev Psychol 2014; 50: 2675–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Somerville, LH. Special issue on the teenage brain: Sensitivity to social evaluation. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2013; 22: 121–7.Google Scholar
Sebastian, C, Viding, E, Williams, KD, Blakemore, SJ. Social brain development and the affective consequences of ostracism in adolescence. Brain Cogn 2010; 72: 134–45.Google Scholar
Moor, BG, Guroglu, B, Op de Macks, ZA et al. Social exclusion and punishment of excluders: neural correlates and developmental trajectories. Neuroimage 2012; 59: 708–17.Google Scholar
Sebastian, CL, Tan, GC, Roiser, JP et al. Developmental influences on the neural bases of responses to social rejection: implications of social neuroscience for education. Neuroimage 2011; 57: 686–94.Google Scholar
Burnett, S, Blakemore, SJ. Functional connectivity during a social emotion task in adolescents and in adults. Eur J Neurosci 2009; 29: 1294–301.Google Scholar
de Villiers, JG. The role(s) of language in theory of mind. In: Gilead, M, Ochsner, K, eds. The Neural Basis of Mentalizing. Cham, Switzerland: Springer, 2021; 423–48.Google Scholar
Aival-Naveh, E, Rothschild‐Yakar, L, Kurman, J. Keeping culture in mind: a systematic review and initial conceptualization of mentalizing from a cross‐cultural perspective. Clin Psychol: Sci Pract 2019; 26: e12300.Google Scholar
Dunn, J, Brophy, M. Communication relationships and individual differences in children’s understanding of mind. In: Astington, JW, Baird, JA, eds. Why Languages Matter for Theory of Mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, 2005; 5069.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meins, E, Fernyhough, C, Arnott, B et al. Mind-mindedness and theory of mind: mediating roles of language and perspectival symbolic play. Child Dev 2013; 84: 1777–90.Google Scholar
Ebert, S, Peterson, C, Slaughter, V, Weinert, S. Links among parents’ mental state language, family socioeconomic status, and preschoolers’ theory of mind development. Cogn Dev 2017; 44: 3248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fonagy, P, Steele, M, Moran, GS et al. Measuring the ghost in the nursery: a summary of the main findings of the Anna Freud Centre/University College London parent-child study. Bull Anna Freud Centre 1991; 14: 115–31.Google Scholar
Steele, H, Perez, A, Segal, F, Steele, M. Maternal Adult Attachment Interview (AAI) collected during pregnancy predicts reflective functioning in AAIs from their first-born children 17 years later. Int J Dev Sci 2016; 10: 117–24.Google Scholar
Zeegers, MAJ, Colonnesi, C, Stams, GJM, Meins, E. Mind matters: a meta-analysis on parental mentalization and sensitivity as predictors of infant-parent attachment. Psychol Bull 2017; 143: 1245–72.Google Scholar
Meins, E, Fernyhough, C, Wainwright, R et al. Maternal mind-mindedness and attachment security as predictors of theory of mind understanding. Child Dev 2002; 73: 1715–26.Google Scholar
Rosso, AM, Airaldi, C. Intergenerational transmission of reflective functioning. Front Psychol 2016; 7: 1903.Google Scholar
Rosso, AM, Viterbori, P, Scopesi, AM. Are maternal reflective functioning and attachment security associated with preadolescent mentalization? Front Psychol 2015; 6: 1134.Google Scholar
Borelli, JL, Cohen, C, Pettit, C et al. Maternal and child sexual abuse history: an intergenerational exploration of children’s adjustment and maternal trauma-reflective functioning. Front Psychol 2019; 10: 1062.Google Scholar
Luyten, P, Fonagy, P. Mentalizing and trauma. In: Bateman, A, Fonagy, P, eds. Handbook of Mentalizing in Mental Health Practice, 2nd ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing, 2019; 7999.Google Scholar
Berthelot, N, Ensink, K, Bernazzani, O et al. Intergenerational transmission of attachment in abused and neglected mothers: the role of trauma-specific reflective functioning. Infant Ment Health J 2015; 36: 200–12.Google Scholar
Fonagy, P, Luyten, P. A multilevel perspective on the development of borderline personality disorder. In: Cicchetti, D, ed. Developmental Psychopathology. Vol. 3: Maladaptation and Psychopathology, 3rd ed. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons, 2016; 726–92.Google Scholar
Fonagy, P, Luyten, P. Conduct problems in youth and the RDoC approach: a developmental, evolutionary-based view. Clin Psychol Rev 2018; 64: 5776.Google Scholar
Luyten, P, Fonagy, P. The stress–reward–mentalizing model of depression: an integrative developmental cascade approach to child and adolescent depressive disorder based on the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) approach. Clin Psychol Rev 2018; 64: 8798.Google Scholar
Fonagy, P. Attachment and borderline personality disorder. J Am Psychoanal Assoc 2000; 48: 1129–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fonagy, P, Steele, M, Steele, H et al. The capacity for understanding mental states: the reflective self in parent and child and its significance for security of attachment. Infant Ment Health J 1991; 12: 201–18.Google Scholar
Fonagy, P, Luyten, P, Allison, E, Campbell, C. What we have changed our minds about: Part 1. Borderline personality disorder as a limitation of resilience. Borderline Personal Disord Emot Dysregul 2017; 4: 11.Google Scholar
Fonagy, P, Luyten, P, Allison, E, Campbell, C. What we have changed our minds about: Part 2. Borderline personality disorder, epistemic trust and the developmental significance of social communication. Borderline Personal Disord Emot Dysregul 2017; 4: 9.Google Scholar
Suchman, NE, DeCoste, C, Borelli, JL, McMahon, TJ. Does improvement in maternal attachment representations predict greater maternal sensitivity, child attachment security and lower rates of relapse to substance use? A second test of Mothering from the Inside Out treatment mechanisms. J Subst Abuse Treat 2018; 85: 2130.Google Scholar
Redfern, S, Wood, S, Lassri, D et al. The Reflective Fostering Programme: background and development of a new approach. Adopt Foster 2018; 42: 234–48.Google Scholar
Slade, A, Holland, ML, Ordway, MR et al. Minding the Baby®: enhancing parental reflective functioning and infant attachment in an attachment-based, interdisciplinary home visiting program. Dev Psychopathol 2020; 32: 123–37.Google Scholar
Byrne, G, Sleed, M, Midgley, N et al. Lighthouse Parenting Programme: description and pilot evaluation of mentalization-based treatment to address child maltreatment. Clin Child Psychol Psychiatry 2019; 24: 680–93.Google Scholar
Bateman, A, Fonagy, P. A randomized controlled trial of a mentalization-based intervention (MBT-FACTS) for families of people with borderline personality disorder. Personal Disord 2019; 10: 7079.Google Scholar
Fonagy, P, Twemlow, SW, Vernberg, EM et al. A cluster randomized controlled trial of child-focused psychiatric consultation and a school systems-focused intervention to reduce aggression. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2009; 50: 607–16.Google Scholar
Fonagy, P, Allison, E, Campbell, C. Mentalizing, resilience, and epistemic trust. In: Bateman, A, Fonagy, P, eds. Handbook of Mentalizing in Mental Health Practice, 2nd ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association Publishing, 2019; 6377.Google Scholar
Fonagy, P, Steele, M, Steele, H et al. The Emanuel Miller Memorial Lecture 1992. The theory and practice of resilience. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1994; 35: 231–57.Google Scholar
Luyten, P, Campbell, C, Allison, E, Fonagy, P. The mentalizing approach to psychopathology: state of the art and future directions. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2020; 16: 297325.Google Scholar
Bartels, A, Zeki, S. The neural correlates of maternal and romantic love. Neuroimage 2004; 21: 1155–66.Google Scholar
Zeki, S, Romaya, JP. Neural correlates of hate. PLoS One 2008; 3: e3556.Google Scholar
Nolte, T, Bolling, DZ, Hudac, CM et al. Brain mechanisms underlying the impact of attachment-related stress on social cognition. Front Hum Neurosci 2013; 7: 816.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pompili, M, Girardi, P, Ruberto, A, Tatarelli, R. Suicide in borderline personality disorder: a meta-analysis. Nord J Psychiatry 2005; 59: 319–24.Google Scholar
Korzekwa, MI, Dell, PF, Links, PS et al. Dissociation in borderline personality disorder: a detailed look. J Trauma Dissociation 2009; 10: 346–67.Google Scholar
Miller, CE, Townsend, ML, Grenyer, BFS. Understanding chronic feelings of emptiness in borderline personality disorder: a qualitative study. Borderline Personal Disord Emot Dysregul 2021; 8: 24.Google Scholar
Vermetten, E, Spiegel, D. Trauma and dissociation: implications for borderline personality disorder. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2014; 16: 434.Google Scholar
Ende, G, Cackowski, S, Van Eijk, J et al. Impulsivity and aggression in female BPD and ADHD patients: association with ACC glutamate and GABA concentrations. Neuropsychopharmacology 2016; 41: 410–18.Google Scholar
Sebastian, A, Jung, P, Krause-Utz, A et al. Frontal dysfunctions of impulse control – a systematic review in borderline personality disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Front Hum Neurosci 2014; 8: 698.Google Scholar
Krause-Utz, A, Keibel-Mauchnik, J, Ebner-Priemer, U et al. Classical conditioning in borderline personality disorder: an fMRI study. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2016; 266: 291305.Google Scholar
Bateman, A, Bolton, R, Fonagy, P. Antisocial personality disorder: a mentalizing framework. Focus 2013; 11: 178–86.Google Scholar
Cackowski, S, Reitz, AC, Ende, G et al. Impact of stress on different components of impulsivity in borderline personality disorder. Psychol Med 2014; 44: 3329–40.Google Scholar
Jacob, GA, Zvonik, K, Kamphausen, S et al. Emotional modulation of motor response inhibition in women with borderline personality disorder: an fMRI study. J Psychiatry Neurosci 2013; 38: 164–72.Google Scholar
Lis, S, Bohus, M. Social interaction in borderline personality disorder. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2013; 15: 338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King-Casas, B, Sharp, C, Lomax-Bream, L et al. The rupture and repair of cooperation in borderline personality disorder. Science 2008; 321: 806–10.Google Scholar
Reinhard, MA, Dewald-Kaufmann, J, Wustenberg, T et al. The vicious circle of social exclusion and psychopathology: a systematic review of experimental ostracism research in psychiatric disorders. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2020; 270: 521–32.Google Scholar
Jowett, S, Karatzias, T, Albert, I. Multiple and interpersonal trauma are risk factors for both post-traumatic stress disorder and borderline personality disorder: a systematic review on the traumatic backgrounds and clinical characteristics of comorbid post-traumatic stress disorder/borderline personality disorder groups versus single-disorder groups. Psychol Psychother 2020; 93: 621–38.Google Scholar
Fonagy, P, Leigh, T, Steele, M et al. The relation of attachment status, psychiatric classification, and response to psychotherapy. J Consult Clin Psychol 1996; 64: 2231.Google Scholar
Levy, KN, Meehan, KB, Kelly, KM et al. Change in attachment patterns and reflective function in a randomized control trial of transference-focused psychotherapy for borderline personality disorder. J Consult Clin Psychol 2006; 74: 1027–40.Google Scholar
Fischer-Kern, M, Schuster, P, Kapusta, ND et al. The relationship between personality organization, reflective functioning, and psychiatric classification in borderline personality disorder. Psychoanal Psychol 2010; 27: 395409.Google Scholar
Gullestad, FS, Johansen, MS, Høglend, P et al. Mentalization as a moderator of treatment effects: findings from a randomized clinical trial for personality disorders. Psychother Res 2013; 23: 674–89.Google Scholar
Bora, E. A meta-analysis of theory of mind and ‘mentalization’ in borderline personality disorder: a true neuro-social-cognitive or meta-social-cognitive impairment? Psychol Med 2021; 51: 2541–51.Google Scholar
Preissler, S, Dziobek, I, Ritter, K et al. Social cognition in borderline personality disorder: evidence for disturbed recognition of the emotions, thoughts, and intentions of others. Front Behav Neurosci 2010; 4: 182.Google Scholar
Ritter, K, Dziobek, I, Preissler, S et al. Lack of empathy in patients with narcissistic personality disorder. Psychiatry Res 2011; 187: 241–7.Google Scholar
Sharp, C, Pane, H, Ha, C et al. Theory of mind and emotion regulation difficulties in adolescents with borderline traits. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2011; 50: 563–73.Google Scholar
Dziobek, I, Preissler, S, Grozdanovic, Z et al. Neuronal correlates of altered empathy and social cognition in borderline personality disorder. Neuroimage 2011; 57: 539–48.Google Scholar
Bekrater-Bodmann, R, Chung, BY, Foell, J et al. Body plasticity in borderline personality disorder: a link to dissociation. Compr Psychiatry 2016; 69: 3644.Google Scholar
Pavony, MT, Lenzenweger, MF. Somatosensory processing and borderline personality disorder features: a signal detection analysis of proprioception and exteroceptive sensitivity. J Personal Disord 2013; 27: 208–21.Google Scholar
Pavony, MT, Lenzenweger, MF. Somatosensory processing and borderline personality disorder: pain perception and a signal detection analysis of proprioception and exteroceptive sensitivity. Personal Disord 2014; 5: 164–71.Google Scholar
De Meulemeester, C, Lowyck, B, Luyten, P. The role of impairments in self-other distinction in borderline personality disorder: a narrative review of recent evidence. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2021; 127: 242–54.Google Scholar
Neustadter, ES, Fineberg, SK, Leavitt, J et al. Induced illusory body ownership in borderline personality disorder. Neurosci Conscious 2019; 2019: niz017.Google Scholar
De Meulemeester, C, Lowyck, B, Panagiotopoulou, E et al. Self–other distinction and borderline personality disorder features: evidence for egocentric and altercentric bias in a self–other facial morphing task. Personal Disord 2021; 12: 377–88.Google Scholar
Auerbach, RP, Tarlow, N, Bondy, E et al. Electrocortical reactivity during self-referential processing in female youth with borderline personality disorder. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging 2016; 1: 335–44.Google Scholar
Beeney, JE, Stepp, SD, Hallquist, MN et al. Attachment and social cognition in borderline personality disorder: specificity in relation to antisocial and avoidant personality disorders. Personal Disord 2015; 6: 207–15.Google Scholar
Vater, A, Schroder-Abe, M, Weissgerber, S et al. Self-concept structure and borderline personality disorder: evidence for negative compartmentalization. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 2015; 46: 5058.Google Scholar
Kanske, P, Schulze, L, Dziobek, I et al. The wandering mind in borderline personality disorder: instability in self- and other-related thoughts. Psychiatry Res 2016; 242: 302–10.Google Scholar
Sharp, C, Ha, C, Carbone, C et al. Hypermentalizing in adolescent inpatients: treatment effects and association with borderline traits. J Personal Disord 2013; 27: 318.Google Scholar
Winter, D, Koplin, K, Schmahl, C et al. Evaluation and memory of social events in borderline personality disorder: effects of valence and self-referential context. Psychiatry Res 2016; 240: 1925.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ritzl, A, Csukly, G, Balazs, K, Egerhazi, A. Facial emotion recognition deficits and alexithymia in borderline, narcissistic, and histrionic personality disorders. Psychiatry Res 2018; 270: 154–9.Google Scholar
Lowyck, B, Luyten, P, Vanwalleghem, D et al. What’s in a face? Mentalizing in borderline personality disorder based on dynamically changing facial expressions. Personal Disord 2016; 7: 72–9.Google Scholar
Bertsch, K, Krauch, M, Stopfer, K et al. Interpersonal threat sensitivity in borderline personality disorder: an eye-tracking study. J Pers Disord 2017; 31: 647–70.Google Scholar
Fenske, S, Lis, S, Liebke, L et al. Emotion recognition in borderline personality disorder: effects of emotional information on negative bias. Borderline Personal Disord Emot Dysregul 2015; 2: 10.Google Scholar
Bertsch, K, Gamer, M, Schmidt, B et al. Oxytocin and reduction of social threat hypersensitivity in women with borderline personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2013; 170: 1169–77.Google Scholar
Kobeleva, X, Seidel, EM, Kohler, C et al. Dissociation of explicit and implicit measures of the behavioral inhibition and activation system in borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Res 2014; 218: 134–42.Google Scholar
Dyck, M, Habel, U, Slodczyk, J et al. Negative bias in fast emotion discrimination in borderline personality disorder. Psychol Med 2009; 39: 855–64.Google Scholar
Hanegraaf, L, van Baal, S, Hohwy, J, Verdejo-Garcia, A. A systematic review and meta-analysis of ‘Systems for Social Processes’ in borderline personality and substance use disorders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2021; 127: 572–92.Google Scholar
Renneberg, B, Heyn, K, Gebhard, R, Bachmann, S. Facial expression of emotions in borderline personality disorder and depression. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 2005; 36: 183–96.Google Scholar
Staebler, K, Renneberg, B, Stopsack, M et al. Facial emotional expression in reaction to social exclusion in borderline personality disorder. Psychol Med 2011; 41: 1929–38.Google Scholar
Matzke, B, Herpertz, SC, Berger, C et al. Facial reactions during emotion recognition in borderline personality disorder: a facial electromyography study. Psychopathology 2014; 47: 101–10.Google Scholar
Schulze, L, Schmahl, C, Niedtfeld, I. Neural correlates of disturbed emotion processing in borderline personality disorder: a multimodal meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry 2016; 79: 97106.Google Scholar
Fertuck, EA, Jekal, A, Song, I et al. Enhanced ‘Reading the Mind in the Eyes’ in borderline personality disorder compared to healthy controls. Psychol Med 2009; 39: 1979–88.Google Scholar
Frick, C, Lang, S, Kotchoubey, B et al. Hypersensitivity in borderline personality disorder during mindreading. PLoS One 2012; 7: e41650.Google Scholar
Berenson, KR, Dochat, C, Martin, CG et al. Identification of mental states and interpersonal functioning in borderline personality disorder. Personal Disord 2018; 9: 172–81.Google Scholar
Anupama, V, Bhola, P, Thirthalli, J, Mehta, UM. Pattern of social cognition deficits in individuals with borderline personality disorder. Asian J Psychiatr 2018; 33: 105–12.Google Scholar
Brown, RC, Plener, PL, Groen, G et al. Differential neural processing of social exclusion and inclusion in adolescents with non-suicidal self-injury and young adults with borderline personality disorder. Front Psychiatry 2017; 8: 267.Google Scholar
Sato, M, Fonagy, P, Luyten, P. Rejection sensitivity and borderline personality disorder features: a mediation model of effortful control and intolerance of ambiguity. Psychiatry Res 2018; 269: 5055.Google Scholar
Sato, M, Fonagy, P, Luyten, P. Rejection sensitivity and borderline personality disorder features: the mediating roles of attachment anxiety, need to belong, and self-criticism. J Pers Disord 2020; 34: 273–88.Google Scholar
Franic, S, Dolan, CV, Borsboom, D et al. Three-and-a-half-factor model? The genetic and environmental structure of the CBCL/6-18 internalizing grouping. Behav Genet 2014; 44: 254–68.Google Scholar
Lahey, BB, Van Hulle, CA, Singh, AL et al. Higher-order genetic and environmental structure of prevalent forms of child and adolescent psychopathology. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2011; 68: 181–9.Google Scholar
Pettersson, E, Anckarsater, H, Gillberg, C, Lichtenstein, P. Different neurodevelopmental symptoms have a common genetic etiology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2013; 54: 1356–65.Google Scholar
Pettersson, E, Larsson, H, Lichtenstein, P. Common psychiatric disorders share the same genetic origin: a multivariate sibling study of the Swedish population. Mol Psychiatry 2016; 21: 717–21.Google Scholar
Anttila, V, Bulik-Sullivan, B, Finucane, HK et al. Analysis of shared heritability in common disorders of the brain. Science 2018; 360: eaap8757.Google Scholar
Caspi, A, Moffitt, TE. All for one and one for all: mental disorders in one dimension. Am J Psychiatry 2018; 175: 831–44.Google Scholar
Caspi, A, Houts, RM, Belsky, DW et al. The p factor: one general psychopathology factor in the structure of psychiatric disorders? Clin Psychol Sci 2014; 2: 119–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lahey, BB, Applegate, B, Hakes, JK et al. Is there a general factor of prevalent psychopathology during adulthood? J Abnorm Psychol 2012; 121: 971–7.Google Scholar
Lahey, BB, Krueger, RF, Rathouz, PJ et al. Validity and utility of the general factor of psychopathology. World Psychiatry 2017; 16: 142–4.Google Scholar
Smith, GT, Atkinson, EA, Davis, HA et al. The general factor of psychopathology. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2020; 16: 7598.Google Scholar
Patalay, P, Fonagy, P, Deighton, J et al. A general psychopathology factor in early adolescence. Br J Psychiatry 2015; 207: 1522.Google Scholar
Macdonald, AN, Goines, KB, Novacek, DM, Walker, EF. Prefrontal mechanisms of comorbidity from a transdiagnostic and ontogenic perspective. Dev Psychopathol 2016; 28: 1147–75.Google Scholar
Wise, T, Radua, J, Via, E et al. Common and distinct patterns of grey-matter volume alteration in major depression and bipolar disorder: evidence from voxel-based meta-analysis. Mol Psychiatry 2017; 22: 1455–63.Google Scholar
Hinton, KE, Lahey, BB, Villalta-Gil, V et al. White matter microstructure correlates of general and specific second-order factors of psychopathology. Neuroimage Clin 2019; 22: 101705.Google Scholar
Protzner, AB, An, S, Jirsa, V. Network modulation in neuropsychiatric disorders using the virtual brain. In: Diwadkar, VA, Eickhoff, SB, eds. Brain Network Dysfunction in Neuropsychiatric Illness. Cham, Switzerland: Springer Nature Switzerland AG, 2021; 153–70.Google Scholar
Sripada, C, Angstadt, M, Taxali, A et al. Widespread attenuating changes in brain connectivity associated with the general factor of psychopathology in 9- and 10-year olds. Transl Psychiatry 2021; 11: 575.Google Scholar
Jack, AI, Dawson, AJ, Begany, KL et al. fMRI reveals reciprocal inhibition between social and physical cognitive domains. Neuroimage 2013; 66: 385401.Google Scholar
Ekhtiari, H, Paulus, M. Preface: Neuroscience for addiction medicine: from prevention to rehabilitation. Prog Brain Res 2016; 224: xxvxxvi.Google Scholar
Beauchaine, TP. Future directions in emotion dysregulation and youth psychopathology. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 2015; 44: 875–96.Google Scholar
Gross, JJ. Emotion regulation: conceptual and empirical foundations. In: Gross, JJ, ed. Handbook of Emotion Regulation, 2nd ed. New York, NY: The Guilford Press, 2014; 320.Google Scholar
Beauchaine, TP, Cicchetti, D. Emotion dysregulation and emerging psychopathology: a transdiagnostic, transdisciplinary perspective. Dev Psychopathol 2019; 31: 799804.Google Scholar
McTeague, LM, Huemer, J, Carreon, DM et al. Identification of common neural circuit disruptions in cognitive control across psychiatric disorders. Am J Psychiatry 2017; 174: 676–85.Google Scholar
Banich, MT. Executive function: the search for an integrated account. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2009; 18: 8994.Google Scholar
Fonagy, P, Campbell, C, Bateman, A. Mentalizing, attachment, and epistemic trust in group therapy. Int J Group Psychother 2017; 67: 176201.Google Scholar
Masten, CL, Morelli, SA, Eisenberger, NI. An fMRI investigation of empathy for ‘social pain’ and subsequent prosocial behavior. Neuroimage 2011; 55: 381–8.Google Scholar
Espinosa, L, Golkar, A, Olsson, A. Mentalizing in value-based vicarious learning. In: Gilead, M, Ochsner, K, eds. The Neural Basis of Mentalizing. Cham, Switzerland: Springer, 2021; 517–36.Google Scholar
Hoehl, S, Keupp, S, Schleihauf, H et al. ‘Over-imitation’: a review and appraisal of a decade of research. Dev Rev 2019; 51: 90108.Google Scholar
Lyons, DE, Damrosch, DH, Lin, JK et al. The scope and limits of overimitation in the transmission of artefact culture. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2011; 366: 1158–67.Google Scholar
Huang, C-T, Heyes, C, Charman, T. Preschoolers’ behavioural reenactment of “failed attempts”: the roles of intention-reading, emulation and mimicry. Cogn Dev 2006; 21: 3645.Google Scholar
Clay, Z, Tennie, C. Is overimitation a uniquely human phenomenon? Insights from human children as compared to bonobos. Child Dev 2018; 89: 1535–44.Google Scholar
Gergely, G, Csibra, G. Sylvia’s recipe: human culture, imitation, and pedagogy. In: Enfield, NJ, Levinson, SC, eds. Roots of Human Sociality: Culture, Cognition, and Human Interaction. London: Berg Press, 2006; 229–55.Google Scholar
Altınok, N, Hernik, M, Király, I, Gergely, G. Acquiring sub-efficient and efficient variants of novel means by integrating information from multiple social models in preschoolers. J Exp Child Psychol 2020; 195: 104847.Google Scholar
Nielsen, M. The social glue of cumulative culture and ritual behavior. Child Dev Perspect 2018; 12: 264.Google Scholar
Flynn, E, Smith, K. Investigating the mechanisms of cultural acquisition. Soc Psychol 2012; 43: 185–95.Google Scholar
Gruber, T, Deschenaux, A, Frick, A, Clement, F. Group membership influences more social identification than social learning or overimitation in children. Child Dev 2019; 90: 728–45.Google Scholar
Marsh, LE, Ropar, D, Hamilton, AFC. Are you watching me? The role of audience and object novelty in overimitation. J Exp Child Psychol 2019; 180: 123–30.Google Scholar
Gallotti, M, Frith, CD. Social cognition in the we-mode. Trends Cogn Sci 2013; 17: 160–65.Google Scholar
Higgins, J. Cognising with others in the we-mode: a defence of ‘first-person plural’ social cognition. Rev Philos Psychol 2020; 12: 803–24.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. A Natural History of Human Morality. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 2016.Google Scholar
Butler, AC, Brown, GK, Beck, AT, Grisham, JR. Assessment of dysfunctional beliefs in borderline personality disorder. Behav Res Ther 2002; 40: 1231–40.Google Scholar
Botsford, J, Schulze, L, Bohlander, J, Renneberg, B. Interpersonal trust: development and validation of a self-report inventory and clinical application in patients with borderline personality disorder. J Pers Disord 2021; 35: 447–68.Google Scholar
Fertuck, EA, Grinband, J, Stanley, B. Facial trust appraisal negatively biased in borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Res 2013; 207: 195202.Google Scholar
Nicol, K, Pope, M, Sprengelmeyer, R et al. Social judgement in borderline personality disorder. PLoS One 2013; 8: e73440.Google Scholar
Fertuck, EA, Grinband, J, Mann, JJ et al. Trustworthiness appraisal deficits in borderline personality disorder are associated with prefrontal cortex, not amygdala, impairment. Neuroimage Clin 2019; 21: 101616.Google Scholar
Campbell, C, Tanzer, M, Saunders, R et al. Development and validation of a self-report measure of epistemic trust. PLoS One 2021; 16: e0250264.Google Scholar
Mascaro, O, Sperber, D. The moral, epistemic, and mindreading components of children’s vigilance towards deception. Cognition 2009; 112: 367–80.Google Scholar
Fonagy, P, Luyten, P, Allison, E. Epistemic petrification and the restoration of epistemic trust: a new conceptualization of borderline personality disorder and its psychosocial treatment. J Pers Disord 2015; 29: 575609.Google Scholar
Orme, W, Bowersox, L, Vanwoerden, S et al. The relation between epistemic trust and borderline pathology in an adolescent inpatient sample. Borderline Personal Disord Emot Dysregul 2019; 6: 13.Google Scholar
Preuss, N, Brandle, LS, Hager, OM et al. Inconsistency and social decision making in patients with Borderline Personality Disorder. Psychiatry Res 2016; 243: 115–22.Google Scholar
Ebert, A, Kolb, M, Heller, J et al. Modulation of interpersonal trust in borderline personality disorder by intranasal oxytocin and childhood trauma. Soc Neurosci 2013; 8: 305–13.Google Scholar
Theodoridou, A, Rowe, AC, Penton-Voak, IS, Rogers, PJ. Oxytocin and social perception: oxytocin increases perceived facial trustworthiness and attractiveness. Horm Behav 2009; 56: 128–32.Google Scholar
Shamay-Tsoory, SG, Abu-Akel, A. The social salience hypothesis of oxytocin. Biol Psychiatry 2016; 79: 194202.Google Scholar
Miano, A, Fertuck, EA, Arntz, A, Stanley, B. Rejection sensitivity is a mediator between borderline personality disorder features and facial trust appraisal. J Pers Disord 2013; 27: 442–56.Google Scholar
Preti, E, Casini, E, Richetin, J et al. Cognitive and emotional components of rejection sensitivity: independent contributions to adolescent self- and interpersonal functioning. Assessment 2020; 27: 1230–41.Google Scholar
Masland, SR, Hooley, JM. When trust does not come easily: negative emotional information unduly influences trustworthiness appraisals for individuals with borderline personality features. J Pers Disord 2020; 34: 394409.Google Scholar
Miano, A, Fertuck, EA, Roepke, S, Dziobek, I. Romantic relationship dysfunction in borderline personality disorder—a naturalistic approach to trustworthiness perception. Personal Disord 2017; 8: 281–6.Google Scholar
Fonagy, P, Luyten, P. A developmental, mentalization-based approach to the understanding and treatment of borderline personality disorder. Dev Psychopathol 2009; 21: 1355–81.Google Scholar
Hsu, DT, Jarcho, JM. “Next up for psychiatry: rejection sensitivity and the social brain.” Neuropsychopharmacology 2021; 46: 239–40.Google Scholar
Eisenberger, NI, Lieberman, MD. Why rejection hurts: a common neural alarm system for physical and social pain. Trends Cogn Sci 2004; 8: 294300.Google Scholar
Eisenberger, NI. The pain of social disconnection: examining the shared neural underpinnings of physical and social pain. Nat Rev Neurosci 2012; 13: 421–34.Google Scholar
Sahi, RS, Eisenberger, NI. Why don’t you like me? The role of the mentalizing network in social rejection. In: Gilead, M, Ochsner, K, eds. The Neural Basis of Mentalizing. Cham, Switzerland: Springer, 2021; 613–28.Google Scholar
Vijayakumar, N, Cheng, TW, Pfeifer, JH. Neural correlates of social exclusion across ages: a coordinate-based meta-analysis of functional MRI studies. Neuroimage 2017; 153: 359–68.Google Scholar
Smith, AR, Nelson, EE, Kircanski, K et al. Social anxiety and age are associated with neural response to social evaluation during adolescence. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2020; 42: 100768.Google Scholar
Jarcho, JM, Tanofsky-Kraff, M, Nelson, EE et al. Neural activation during anticipated peer evaluation and laboratory meal intake in overweight girls with and without loss of control eating. Neuroimage 2015; 108: 343–53.Google Scholar
Gradin, VB, Waiter, G, Kumar, P et al. Abnormal neural responses to social exclusion in schizophrenia. PLoS One 2012; 7: e42608.Google Scholar
Sebastian, C, Blakemore, SJ, Charman, T. Reactions to ostracism in adolescents with autism spectrum conditions. J Autism Dev Disord 2009; 39: 1122–30.Google Scholar
Feldman, S, Downey, G. Rejection sensitivity as a mediator of the impact of childhood exposure to family violence on adult attachment behavior. Dev Psychopathol 1994; 6: 231–47.Google Scholar
Gao, S, Assink, M, Cipriani, A, Lin, K. Associations between rejection sensitivity and mental health outcomes: a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev 2017; 57: 5974.Google Scholar
Zhang, W, Hu, N, Ding, X, Li, J. The relationship between rejection sensitivity and borderline personality features: a meta-analysis. Adv Psychol Sci 2021; 29: 1179–94.Google Scholar
Cavicchioli, M, Maffei, C. Rejection sensitivity in borderline personality disorder and the cognitive-affective personality system: a meta-analytic review. Personal Disord 2020; 11: 112.Google Scholar
Korn, CW, La Rosee, L, Heekeren, HR, Roepke, S. Social feedback processing in borderline personality disorder. Psychol Med 2016; 46: 575–87.Google Scholar
Reichenberger, J, Eibl, JJ, Pfaltz, MC et al. Don’t praise me, don’t chase me: emotional reactivity to positive and negative social-evaluative videos in patients with borderline personality disorder. J Pers Disord 2017; 31: 7589.Google Scholar
Liebke, L, Koppe, G, Bungert, M et al. Difficulties with being socially accepted: an experimental study in borderline personality disorder. J Abnorm Psychol 2018; 127: 670–82.Google Scholar
Allen, JG, Fonagy, P, Bateman, AW. The role of mentalizing in treating attachment trauma. In: Vermetten, E, Lanius, R, eds. The Hidden Epidemic: The Impact of Early Life Trauma on Health and Disease. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press, 2010; 247–56.Google Scholar
Janke, K, Driessen, M, Behnia, B et al. Emotional intelligence in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder, borderline personality disorder and healthy controls. Psychiatry Res 2018; 264: 290–96.Google Scholar
Palgi, S, Klein, E, Shamay-Tsoory, SG. Oxytocin improves compassion toward women among patients with PTSD. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2016; 64: 143–9.Google Scholar
Mazza, M, Tempesta, D, Pino, MC et al. Neural activity related to cognitive and emotional empathy in post-traumatic stress disorder. Behav Brain Res 2015; 282: 3745.Google Scholar
Nietlisbach, G, Maercker, A, Rossler, W, Haker, H. Are empathic abilities impaired in posttraumatic stress disorder? Psychol Rep 2010; 106: 832–44.Google Scholar
Mazza, M, Giusti, L, Albanese, A et al. Social cognition disorders in military police officers affected by posttraumatic stress disorder after the attack of An-Nasiriyah in Iraq 2006. Psychiatry Res 2012; 198: 248–52.Google Scholar
Nazarov, A, Frewen, P, Oremus, C et al. Comprehension of affective prosody in women with post-traumatic stress disorder related to childhood abuse. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2015; 131: 342–9.Google Scholar
Passardi, S, Peyk, P, Rufer, M et al. Impaired recognition of positive emotions in individuals with posttraumatic stress disorder, cumulative traumatic exposure, and dissociation. Psychother Psychosom 2018; 87: 118–20.Google Scholar
Bell, CJ, Colhoun, HC, Frampton, CM et al. Earthquake brain: altered recognition and misclassification of facial expressions are related to trauma exposure but not posttraumatic stress disorder. Front Psychiatry 2017; 8: 278.Google Scholar
Freud, A, Burlingham, DT. The Writings of Anna Freud. Vol. 3. Infants Without Families: Reports on the Hampstead Nurseries, 1939-1945. New York, NY: International Universities Press, 1973.Google Scholar
Klinnert, MD, Campos, JJ, Sorce, JF et al. Emotions as behavior regulations: social referencing in infancy. In: Plutchhik, R, Kellerman, H, eds. Emotion: Theory, Research, and Experience. New York, NY: Academic Press, 1983; 5786.Google Scholar
Sorce, JF, Emde, RN, Campos, J, Klinnert, MD. Maternal emotional signaling: its effect on the visual cliff behavior of 1-year-olds. Dev Psychol 1985; 21: 195200.Google Scholar
Askew, C, Field, AP. Vicarious learning and the development of fears in childhood. Behav Res Ther 2007; 45: 2616–27.Google Scholar
Debiec, J, Olsson, A. Social fear learning: from animal models to human function. Trends Cogn Sci 2017; 21: 546–55.Google Scholar
Olsson, A, Knapska, E, Lindstrom, B. The neural and computational systems of social learning. Nat Rev Neurosci 2020; 21: 197212.Google Scholar
Golkar, A, Haaker, J, Selbing, I, Olsson, A. Neural signals of vicarious extinction learning. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2016; 11: 1541–9.Google Scholar
Bateman, A, Campbell, C, Luyten, P, Fonagy, P. A mentalization-based approach to common factors in the treatment of borderline personality disorder. Curr Opin Psychol 2018; 21: 44–9.Google Scholar
Storebø, OJ, Stoffers-Winterling, JM, Völlm, BA et al. Psychological therapies for people with borderline personality disorder. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2020; 5: CD012955.Google Scholar
Cristea, IA, Gentili, C, Cotet, CD et al. Efficacy of psychotherapies for borderline personality disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry 2017; 74: 319–28.Google Scholar
Rameckers, SA, Verhoef, REJ, Grasman, R et al. Effectiveness of psychological treatments for borderline personality disorder and predictors of treatment outcomes: a multivariate multilevel meta-analysis of data from all design types. J Clin Med 2021; 10: 5622.Google Scholar
Volkert, J, Hauschild, S, Taubner, S. Mentalization-based treatment for personality disorders: efficacy, effectiveness, and new developments. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2019; 21: 25.Google Scholar
Vogt, KS, Norman, P. Is mentalization-based therapy effective in treating the symptoms of borderline personality disorder? A systematic review. Psychol Psychother 2019; 92: 441–64.Google Scholar
Bateman, A, Fonagy, P. 8-year follow-up of patients treated for borderline personality disorder: mentalization-based treatment versus treatment as usual. Am J Psychiatry 2008; 165: 631–8.Google Scholar
Bateman, A, Fonagy, P. Randomized controlled trial of outpatient mentalization-based treatment versus structured clinical management for borderline personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2009; 166: 1355–64.Google Scholar
Bateman, A, Fonagy, P. Impact of clinical severity on outcomes of mentalisation-based treatment for borderline personality disorder. Br J Psychiatry 2013; 203: 221–7.Google Scholar
Bateman, A, Constantinou, MP, Fonagy, P, Holzer, S. Eight-year prospective follow-up of mentalization-based treatment versus structured clinical management for people with borderline personality disorder. Personal Disord 2021; 12: 291–9.Google Scholar
Bateman, A, O’Connell, J, Lorenzini, N et al. A randomised controlled trial of mentalization-based treatment versus structured clinical management for patients with comorbid borderline personality disorder and antisocial personality disorder. BMC Psychiatry 2016; 16: 304.Google Scholar
Jørgensen, CR, Freund, C, Boye, R et al. Outcome of mentalization-based and supportive psychotherapy in patients with borderline personality disorder: a randomized trial. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2013; 127: 305–17.Google Scholar
Jørgensen, CR, Bøye, R, Andersen, D et al. Eighteen months post-treatment naturalistic follow-up study of mentalization-based therapy and supportive group treatment of borderline personality disorder: clinical outcomes and functioning. Nord Psychol 2014; 66: 254–73.Google Scholar
Petersen, B, Toft, J, Christensen, NB et al. A 2-year follow-up of mentalization-oriented group therapy following day hospital treatment for patients with personality disorders. Personal Ment Health 2010; 4: 294301.Google Scholar
Laurenssen, EMP, Luyten, P, Kikkert, MJ et al. Day hospital mentalization-based treatment v. specialist treatment as usual in patients with borderline personality disorder: randomized controlled trial. Psychol Med 2018; 48: 2522–9.Google Scholar
Bales, D, Bateman, A. Partial hospitalization settings. In: Bateman, A, Fonagy, P, eds. Handbook of Mentalizing in Mental Health Practice. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing, 2012; 197226.Google Scholar
Kvarstein, EH, Pedersen, G, Folmo, E et al. Mentalization-based treatment or psychodynamic treatment programmes for patients with borderline personality disorder – the impact of clinical severity. Psychol Psychother 2019; 92: 91111.Google Scholar
Robinson, P, Hellier, J, Barrett, B et al. The NOURISHED randomised controlled trial comparing mentalisation-based treatment for eating disorders (MBT-ED) with specialist supportive clinical management (SSCM-ED) for patients with eating disorders and symptoms of borderline personality disorder. Trials 2016; 17: 549.Google Scholar
Rossouw, TI, Fonagy, P. Mentalization-based treatment for self-harm in adolescents: a randomized controlled trial. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2012; 51: 1304–13.Google Scholar
Witt, KG, Hetrick, SE, Rajaram, G et al. Interventions for self-harm in children and adolescents. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2021; 3: CD013667.Google Scholar
Bo, S, Sharp, C, Beck, E et al. First empirical evaluation of outcomes for mentalization-based group therapy for adolescents with BPD. Personal Disord 2017; 8: 396401.Google Scholar
Beck, E, Bo, S, Jorgensen, MS et al. Mentalization-based treatment in groups for adolescents with borderline personality disorder: a randomized controlled trial. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2020; 61: 594604.Google Scholar
Jørgensen, MS, Storebø, OJ, Bo, S et al. Mentalization-based treatment in groups for adolescents with borderline personality disorder: 3- and 12-month follow-up of a randomized controlled trial. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2021; 30: 699710.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×