Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-2pzkn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-07T07:25:26.233Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

14 - The Parasite-Stress Theory of Cultural Values and Sociality

from Part IV - Group Living

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 March 2020

Lance Workman
Affiliation:
University of South Wales
Will Reader
Affiliation:
Sheffield Hallam University
Jerome H. Barkow
Affiliation:
Dalhousie University, Nova Scotia
Get access

Summary

The parasite-stress theory of values or sociality is a recent, encompassing perspective in human social psychology and behavior. As an ecological and evolutionary theory of peoples’ cultural values/core preferences, it applies widely across many domains of human social life and human affairs. It is a general theory of human culture and sociality. Fundamental to the theory is the behavioral immune system. The human behavioral immune system includes: psychological traits and manifest behaviors for avoiding contact with infectious diseases; behaviors of in-group social preference, altruism, alliance, and conformity that manage the negative effects of infectious diseases; mate choice to increase personal and offspring defense against parasites; culinary behavior; and components of personality. The contagion-avoidance aspect of behavioral immunity is much more than out-group avoidance and dislike (xenophobia). It also includes the preference for the natal or local region (philopatry) and hence avoidance of foreignness in people and places where novel parasites may occur. The parasite-stress theory has produced a cornucopia of newly discovered patterns and informed and reinterpreted previously described patterns in the behavior of individuals and at the level of cultures/societies and regions. In novel ways, it informs and synthesizes knowledge of major features of the social lives and societal-level affairs of people, ranging from prejudice and egalitarianism to personality, economic patterns, core values, interpersonal and intergroup violence, governmental systems, gender relations, family structure, and the genesis and maintenance of cultural diversity across the world.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson, R. M., & May, R. M. (1991). Infectious Disease of Humans: Dynamics and Control. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Billing, J., & Sherman, P. W. (1998). Antimicrobial functions of spices: Why some like it hot. Quarterly Review of Biology, 73, 349.Google Scholar
Blute, M. (2010). Darwinian Sociocultural Evolution: Solutions to Dilemmas in Cultural and Social Theory. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Brown, G. D. A., Fincher, C. L., & Walasek, L. (2016). Personality, parasites, political attitudes, and cooperation: A model of how infection prevalence influences openness and social group formation. Topics in Cognitive Science, 8, 98117.Google Scholar
Cashdan, E. (2001). Ethnic diversity and its environmental determinants: Effects on climate, pathogens, and habitat diversity. American Anthropology, 103, 968991.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cashdan, E., & Steele, M. (2013). Pathogen prevalence, group bias, and collectivism in the standard cross-cultural sample. Human Nature, 24, 5975.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiao, J. Y., & Blizinsky, K. D. (2010). Culture–gene coevolution of individualism–collectivism and the serotonin transporter gene. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 277, 529537.Google Scholar
Clay, R., Terrizzi, J. A. Jr., & Shook, N. J. (2012). Individual differences in the behavioral immune system and the emergence of cultural systems. Journal of Social Psychology, 43, 174184.Google Scholar
Corby-Harris, V., & Promislow, D. E. L. (2008). Host ecology shapes geographical variation for resistance to bacterial infection in Drosophila melanogaster. Journal of Animal Ecology, 77, 768776.Google Scholar
Curtis, V. A. (2007). Dirt, disgust and disease: A natural history of hygiene. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 61, 660664.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curtis, V., Aunger, R., & Rabie, T. (2004). Evidence that disgust evolved to protect from risk of disease. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 271, 1731.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curtis, V. A., de Barra, M., & Aunger, H. (2011). Disgust as an adaptive system for disease avoidance behaviour. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 389401.Google Scholar
de Barra, M., DeBruine, L., Jones, B., & Curtis, V. A. (2013). Illness in childhood predicts face preferences in adulthood. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34, 384389.Google Scholar
DeBruine, L. M., Little, A. C., & Jones, B. C. (2012). Extending parasite-stress theory to variation in human mate preferences. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 8687.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denic, S., & Nicholls, M. G. (2007). Genetic benefits of consanguinity through selection of genotypes protective against malaria. Human Biology, 79, 145158.Google Scholar
Denic, S., Nagelkerke, N., & Agarwal, M. M. (2008a). Consanguineous marriages and endemic malaria: Can inbreeding increase population fitness? Malaria Journal, 7, 150.Google Scholar
Denic, S., Nagelkerke, N., & Agarwal, M. M. (2008b). Consanguineous marriages: Do genetic benefits outweigh its costs in populations with alpha(+)-thalassemia, hemoglobin S, and malaria? Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 364369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diamond, J. (1998). Guns, Germs and Steel: The Fates of Human Societies. New York: W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
Dionne, M., Miller, K. M., Dodson, J. J., Caron, F., & Bernatchez, L. (2007). Clinical variation in MHC diversity with temperature: Evidence for the role of host–pathogen interaction on local adaptation in Atlantic salmon. Evolution, 61, 21542164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dobson, A. P., & Carper, E. R. (1996). Infectious diseases and human population history. BioScience, 46, 115126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duncan, L. A., Schaller, M., & Park, J. H. (2009). Perceived vulnerability to disease: Development and validation of a 15-item self-report instrument. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 541546.Google Scholar
Durham, W. H. (1991). Coevolution: Genes, Culture and Human Diversity. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Ewald, P. W. (1994). Evolution of Infectious Disease. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Faulkner, J., Schaller, M., Park, J. H., & Duncan, L.A. (2004). Evolved disease-avoidance mechanisms and contemporary xenophobic attitudes. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 7, 333353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fessler, D. M. T., Eng, S. J., & Navarrete, C. D. (2005). Elevated disgust sensitivity in the first trimester of pregnancy: Evidence supporting the compensatory prophylaxis hypothesis. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 344351.Google Scholar
Fincher, C. L., & Thornhill, R. (2008a). A parasite-driven wedge: Infectious diseases may explain language and other biodiversity. Oikos, 117, 12891297.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fincher, C. L., & Thornhill, R. (2008b). Assortative sociality, limited dispersal, infectious disease and the genesis of the global pattern of religion diversity. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Biological Sciences, 275, 25872594.Google Scholar
Fincher, C. L., & Thornhill, R. (2012a). Parasite-stress promotes in-group assortative sociality: The cases of strong family ties and heightened religiosity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 6179.Google Scholar
Fincher, C. L., & Thornhill, R. (2012b). The parasite-stress theory may be a general theory of culture and sociality response. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 99119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fincher, C. L., Thornhill, R., Murray, D. R., & Schaller, M. (2008). Pathogen prevalence predicts human cross-cultural variability in individualism/collectivism. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Biological Sciences, 275, 12791285.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fumagalli, M., Sironi, M., Pozzoli, U., et al. (2011). Signatures of environmental genetic adaptation pinpoint pathogens as the main selective pressure through human evolution. PLoS Genetics, 7, e1002355.Google Scholar
Gangestad, S. W., & Buss, D. M. (1993). Pathogen prevalence and human mate preference. Ethology and Sociobiology, 14, 8996.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gangestad, S. W., Haselton, M. G., & Buss, D. M. (2006). Evolutionary foundations of cultural variation: Evoked culture and mate preferences. Psychological Inquiry, 17, 7595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Good, C. M. (1972). Salt, trade, and disease: Aspects of development in Africa’s northern Great Lakes region. International Journal of African Historical Studies, 5, 543586.Google Scholar
Guernier, V., Hochberg, M. E., & Guegan, J. (2004). Ecology drives the worldwide distribution of human diseases. PLoS Biology, 2, e141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gurven, M., Allen-Arave, W., Hill, K., & Hurtado, M. (2000). “It’s a Wonderful Life”: Signaling generosity among the Ache of Paraguay. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21, 263282.Google Scholar
Hamilton, W. D., & Zuk, M. (1982). Heritable true fitness and bright birds: A role for parasites? Science, 218, 284387.Google Scholar
Hoben, A. D. (2011). An evolutionary investigation of consanguineous marriages. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Groningen.Google Scholar
Hoben, A. D., Buunk, A. P., Fincher, C. L., & Thornhill, R. (2010). On the adaptive origins and maladaptive consequences of human inbreeding: Parasite prevalence, immune functioning, and consanguineous marriage. Evolutionary Psychology, 8, 658676.Google Scholar
Inbar, Y., Pizarro, D. A., Iyer, R., & Raidt, J. (2012). Disgust sensitivity, political conservatism, and voting. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 5, 537544.Google Scholar
Jones, B. C., Feinberg, D. R., Watkins, C. D., et al. (2013a). Pathogen disgust predicts women’s preferences for masculinity in men’s voices, faces, and bodies. Behavioral Ecology, 24, 373379.Google Scholar
Jones, B. C., Fincher, C. L., Welling, L. L. M., et al. (2013b). Salivary cortisol and pathogen disgust predict men’s preferences for feminine shape cues in women’s faces. Biological Psychology, 92, 233240.Google Scholar
Kaltz, O., Gandon, S., Michalakis, Y., & Shykoff, J. A. (1999). Local maladaptation in the anther-smut fungus Microbotryum violaceum to its host plant Silene latifolia: Evidence from a cross-inoculation experiment. Evolution, 53, 395407.Google ScholarPubMed
Kouznetsova, D., Stevenson, R. J., Oaten, M. J., & Case, T. I. (2012). Disease-avoidant behaviour and its consequences. Psychology and Health, 27, 491506.Google Scholar
Lee, A. J., & Zietsch, B. P. (2011). Experimental evidence that women’s mate preferences are directly influenced by cues of pathogen prevalence and resource scarcity. Biology Letters, 7, 892895.Google Scholar
Letendre, K., Fincher, C. L., & Thornhill, R. (2010). Does infectious disease cause global variation in the frequency of intrastate armed conflict and civil war? Biological Reviews, 85, 669683.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Letendre, K., Fincher, C. L., & Thornhill, R. (2012). Infectious disease, collectivism, and warfare. In Shackelford, T. & Weekes-Shackelford, V., eds., The Oxford Handbook on Evolutionary Perspectives on Violence, Homicide, and Warfare. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 351371.Google Scholar
Little, A. C., DeBruine, L. M., & Jones, B. C. (2010). Exposure to visual cues of pathogen contagion changes preferences for masculinity and symmetry in opposite-sex faces. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 278, 20322039.Google ScholarPubMed
Loker, E. S. (2012). Macroevolutionary immunology: A role for immunity in the diversification of animal life. Frontiers in Immunology, 3, 25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lopez, A. D., Mathers, C. D., Ezzati, M., Jamieson, D. T., & Murray, C. J. (2006). Global and regional burden of disease and risk factors, 2001: Systematic analysis of population health data. Lancet, 367, 17471757.Google Scholar
Low, B. S. (1990). Marriage systems and pathogen stress in human societies. American Zoologist, 30, 325339.Google Scholar
MacMurray, J., Comings, D. E., & Napolioni, V. (2014). The gene–immune–behavioral pathway: Gamma-interferon (1FN-γ) simultaneously coordinates susceptibility to infectious disease and harm avoidance behaviors. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 35, 169175.Google Scholar
McNeill, W. H. (1998). Plagues and Peoples. Harpswell, ME: Anchor.Google Scholar
Miller, E. N., Fadl, M., Mohamed, H. S., et al. (2007). Y chromosome lineage- and village-specific genes on chromosomes 1p22 and 6q27 control visceral leishmaniasis in Sudan. PLoS Genetics, 3, 679688.Google Scholar
Miller, S. L., & Maner, J. K. (2011). Sick body, vigilant mind: The biological immune system activates the behavioral immune system. Psychological Science, 22, 14671471.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, F. R, Coetzee, V., Contreras-Garduño, J., et al. (2013). Cross-cultural variation in women’s preferences for cues to sex- and stress-hormones in the male face. Biology Letters, 9, 20130050.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mortensen, C. R., Becker, D. V., Ackerman, J. M., Neuberg, S. L., & Kenrick, D. T. (2010). Infection breeds reticence: The effects of disease salience on self-perceptions of personality and behavioral avoidance tendencies. Psychological Science, 21, 440447.Google Scholar
Murray, D. R., & Schaller, M. (2010). Historical prevalence of infectious diseases within 230 geopolitical regions: A tool for investigating origins of culture. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 41, 99108.Google Scholar
Murray, D. R., & Schaller, M. (2012). Threat(s) and conformity deconstructed: Perceived threat of infectious disease and its implications for conformist attitudes and behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology, 42, 180188.Google Scholar
Murray, D. R., Trudeau, R., & Schaller, M. (2011). On the origins of cultural differences in conformity: Four tests of the pathogen prevalence hypothesis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 318329.Google Scholar
Murray, D. R., Schaller, M., & Suedfeld, P. (2013). Pathogens and politics: Further evidence that parasite prevalence predicts authoritarianism. PLoS ONE, 8, e62275.Google Scholar
Napolioni, V., Murray, D. R., Cominngs, D. E., et al. (2014). Interaction between infectious diseases and personality traits: ACP1* C as a potential mediator. Infection, Genetics and Evolution, 26, 267273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Navarrete, C. D., & Fessler, D. M. T. (2006). Disease avoidance and ethnocentrism: The effects of disease vulnerability and disgust sensitivity on intergroup attitudes. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27, 270282.Google Scholar
Navarrete, C. D., Fessler, D. M. T., & Eng, S. J. (2007). Elevated ethnocentrism in the first trimester of pregnancy. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 6065.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norenzayan, A., & Shariff, A. F. (2008). The origin and evolution of religious prosociality. Science, 322, 5862.Google Scholar
Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J., & Case, T. I. (2009). Disgust as a disease-avoidance mechanism. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 303321.Google Scholar
Park, J. H., & Schaller, M. (2005). Does attitude similarity serve as a heuristic cue for kinship? Evidence of an implicit cognitive association. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 158170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pitchappan, R. M. (2002). Castes, migration, immunogenetics and infectious diseases in south India. Community Genetics, 5, 157161.Google Scholar
Prokop, P., & Fačovičová, J. (2011). Preferences for spicy foods and disgust of ectoparasites are associated with reported health in humans. Psihologija, 44, 281293.Google Scholar
Prugnolle, F., Manica, A., Charpentier, M., et al. (2005). Pathogen-driven selection and worldwide HLA class I diversity. Current Biology, 15, 10221027.Google Scholar
Reid, S. A., Zhang, J., Anderson, G. L., et al. (2012). Parasite primes make foreign-accented English sound more distant to people who are disgusted by pathogens (but not by sex or morality). Evolution and Human Behavior, 33, 471478.Google Scholar
Ridley, M. (1993). The Red Queen: Sex and the Evolution of Human Nature. New York: Macmillan Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Rougeron, V., De Meeus, T., Hide, M., et al. (2009). Extreme inbreeding in Leishmania braziliensis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 1022410229.Google Scholar
Ryan, S., Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J., & Case, T. I. (2012). Facial disfigurement is treated like an infectious disease. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33, 639646.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaller, M. (2006). Parasites, behavioral defenses, and the social psychological mechanisms through which cultures are evoked. Psychological Inquiry, 17, 96101.Google Scholar
Schaller, M., & Duncan, L. (2007). The behavioral immune system: Its evolution and social psychological implications. In Forges, J. P., Haselton, M. G., & Von Hippel, W., eds., Evolution and the Social Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and Social Cognition. New York: Psychology Press, pp. 293307.Google Scholar
Schaller, M., & Murray, D. (2008). Pathogens, personality, and culture: Disease prevalence predicts worldwide variability in sociosexuality, extraversion, and openness to experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 212221.Google Scholar
Schaller, M., & Neuberg, S. L. (2008). Intergroup prejudices and intergroup conflicts. In Crawford, C. & Krebs, D. L., eds., Foundations of Evolutionary Psychology. New York: Erlbaum, pp. 399412.Google Scholar
Schaller, M., Miller, G. E., Gervais, W. M., Yager, S., & Chen, E. (2010). Mere visual perception of other people’s disease symptoms facilitates a more aggressive immune response. Psychological Science, 21, 649652.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schnall, S., Haidt, J., Clore, G.L., & Jordan, A. H. (2008). Disgust as embodied moral judgment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 10961109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sherman, P. W., & Billing, J. (1999). Darwinian gastronomy: Why we use spices. BioScience, 49, 453463.Google Scholar
Smith, K. F., Sax, D. F., Gaines, S. D., Guernier, V., & Guégan, J. F. (2007). Globalization of human infectious disease. Ecology, 88, 19031910.Google Scholar
Stevenson, R. J., Case, T. I., & Oaten, M. J. (2009). Frequency and recency of infection and their relationship with disgust and contamination sensitivity. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30, 363368.Google Scholar
Stevenson, R. J., Hodgson, D., Oaten, M. J., Barouei, J., & Case, T. I. (2011). The effect of disgust on oral immune function. Psychophysiology, 48, 900907.Google Scholar
Sugiyama, L. S. (2004). Illness, injury, and disability among Shiwiar forager–horticulturalists: Implications of human life history. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 123, 371389.Google Scholar
Sugiyama, L. S., & Sugiyama, M. S. (2003). Social roles, prestige, and health risk: Social niche specialization as a risk-buffering strategy. Human Nature, 14, 165190.Google Scholar
Terrizzi, J. A. Jr., Shook, N. J., & Ventis, W. L. (2010). Disgust: A predictor of social conservatism and prejudicial attitudes toward homosexuals. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 587592.Google Scholar
Terrizzi, J. A. Jr., Shook, N. J., & Ventis, W. L. (2012). Religious conservatism: an evolutionarily evoked disease-avoidance strategy. Religion, Brain and Behavior, 2, 105120.Google Scholar
Terrizzi, J. A. Jr., Shook, N. J., & McDaniel, M. A. (2013). The behavioral immune system and social conservatism: A meta-analysis. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34, 99108.Google Scholar
Thompson, J. N. (2005). The Geographic Mosaic of Coevolution. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Thornhill, R., & Fincher, C. L. (2011). Parasite stress promotes homicide and child maltreatment. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society: Biological Sciences, 366, 34663477.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thornhill, R., & Fincher, C. L. (2013a). Commentary on Hackman, J., & Hruschka, D. (2013). Fast life histories, not pathogens, account for state-level variation in homicide, child maltreatment, and family ties in the U.S. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34, 314315.Google Scholar
Thornhill, R., & Fincher, C. L. (2013b). The parasite-driven-wedge model of parapatric speciation. Journal of Zoology, 291, 2333.Google Scholar
Thornhill, R., & Fincher, C. L. (2014). The Parasite-Stress Theory of Values and Sociality: Infectious Disease, History and Human Values Worldwide. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Thornhill, R., & Gangestad, S. W. (2008). The Evolutionary Biology of Human Female Sexuality. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Thornhill, R., Fincher, C. L., & Aran, D. (2009). Parasites, democratization, and the liberalization of values across contemporary countries. Biological Reviews, 84, 113131.Google Scholar
Thornhill, R., Fincher, C. L., Murray, D. R., & Schaller, M. (2010). Zoonotic and non-zoonotic diseases in relation to human personality and societal values: Support for the parasite-stress model. Evolutionary Psychology, 8, 151169.Google Scholar
Tinsley, M. C., Blanford, S., & Jiggins, F. M. (2006). Genetic variation in Drosophila melanogaster pathogen susceptibility. Parasitology, 132, 767773.Google Scholar
Van Valen, L. (1973). A new evolutionary law. Evolutionary Theory, 1, 130.Google Scholar
Volk, A. A., & Atkinson, J. A. (2013). Infant and child death in the human environment of evolutionary adaptation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34, 182192.Google Scholar
Way, B. M., & Lieberman, M. D. (2010). Is there a genetic contribution to cultural differences? Collectivism, individualism and genetic markers of social sensitivity. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5, 203211.Google Scholar
Welling, L. L. M., Conway, C. A., DeBruine, L. M., & Jones, B. C. (2007). Perceived vulnerability to disease is positively related to the strength of preferences for apparent health in faces. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 5, 131139.Google Scholar
Wolfe, N. D., Dunavan, C. P., & Diamond, J. (2007). Origins of major human infectious diseases. Nature, 447, 279283.Google Scholar
Wu, B., & Chang, L. (2012). The social impact of pathogen threat: How disease salience influences conformity. Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 5054.Google Scholar
Young, S. G., Savvo, D. F., & Hugenberg, K. (2011). Vulnerability to disease is associated with a domain-specific preference for symmetrical faces relative to symmetrical non-face stimuli. European Journal of Social Psychology, 41, 558563.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×