Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-767nl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-08T13:32:08.397Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

18 - A New Look at “Age”: Young and Old L2 Learners

from Part IV - Individual Differences

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 June 2019

John W. Schwieter
Affiliation:
Wilfrid Laurier University
Alessandro Benati
Affiliation:
American University of Sharjah, United Arab Emirates
Get access

Summary

Learner age has for a long time featured prominently in the research agenda of the second language acquisition (SLA) field. Arguably, a few decades ago this agenda was almost exclusively dominated by the concern with constraints of age on post-puberty learners that originated in the Critical Period Hypothesis (CPH) as formulated by Lenneberg (1967) for first language (L1) acquisition. The CPH links age of onset or age of acquisition (AOA) to biological constraints on what can be attained beyond puberty, and it was extended to second language (L2) acquisition through the work of Johnson and Newport (1989). Participants in CPH studies typically came to be immigrants arriving to the L2 community at different ages and acquiring the community language to different degrees. In these studies, participants’ ultimate attainment is examined and compared on the basis of their age of arrival (usually equated with AOA), and a negative association is usually found between AOA and ultimate attainment. In Long’s (2005) words, the aim of this line of research is to provide “evidence for the existence, scope or timing of maturational constraints on the human capacity for learning second (including foreign) languages” (p. 288).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abrahamsson, N., & Hyltenstam, K. (2008). The robustness of aptitude effects in near-native second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 30, 481509.Google Scholar
Al-Thubaiti, K. A. (2010). Age effects in a minimal input setting on the acquisition of English morpho-syntactic and semantic properties by L1 speakers of Arabic. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Essex, Wivenhoe.Google Scholar
Andrew, P. (2012). The social construction of age: Adult foreign language learners. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Antoniou, M., Gunasekera, G. M., & Wong, P. C. M. (2013). Foreign language training as cognitive therapy for age-related cognitive decline: A hypothesis for future research. Neuroscience & Behavioural Reviews, 37(10), 26892698.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bahrick, H. P. (1984). Fifty years of second language attrition: Implications for programmatic research. The Modern Language Journal, 68, 105118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Craik, F. I. M. (2010). Cognitive and linguistic processing in the bilingual mind. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19, 1923.Google Scholar
Bigelow, M., del Mas, B., Hansen, K., & Tarone, E. (2006). Literacy and the processing of oral recasts in SLA. TESOL Quarterly, 40, 125.Google Scholar
Birdsong, D. (2005). Nativelikeness and non-nativelikeness in L2A research. International Review of Applied Linguistics, 43, 319328.Google Scholar
Birdsong, D., & Paik, J. (2008). Second language acquisition and ultimate attainment. In Spolsky, B. & Hult, F. (eds.), Handbook of educational linguistics (pp. 424436). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Burke, D., & MacKay, D. (1997). Memory, language, and ageing. Ageing: Science, Medicine, and Society, 352, 18451856.Google Scholar
Cook, V. (2002). Background to the L2 User. In Cook, V. (ed.), Portraits of the L2 User (pp. 128). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Council of the European Union. (1997). Council resolution on the early teaching of European Union languages. Retrieved from https://eur-lex.europa.eu/legal-content/EN/ALL/?uri=CELEX%3A31998Y0103%2801%29.Google Scholar
Cox, J., & Sanz, C. (2015). Deconstructing PI for the ages: Explicit instruction vs. practice in young and older adult bilinguals. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching (IRAL), 2, 225248.Google Scholar
Craik, F., & Bialystok, E. (2005). Intelligence and executive control: Evidence from aging and bilingualism. Cortex, 41, 222224.Google Scholar
Davies, A. (2003). The native speaker: Myth and reality. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
DeKeyser, R. (2000). The robustness of critical period effects in second language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 22(4), 499533.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeKeyser, R. (2013). Age effects in second language learning: Stepping stones toward better understanding. Language Learning, 63(1), 5267.Google Scholar
Dwaik, R., & Shehadeh, A. (2015). The starting age and ultimate attainment of English learning in the Palestinian context. English Language Teaching, 8(10), 9199.Google Scholar
Ellis, N. C. (2005). At the interface: Dynamic interactions of explicit and implicit language knowledge. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 305352.Google Scholar
European Commission (2011). Language learning at pre-primary school level: Making it efficient and sustainable: A policy handbook. Brussels: European Commission.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E. (2009). Give input a chance! In Piske, T. & Young-Scholten, M. (eds.), Input matters in SLA (pp. 175190). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E., & Liu, S. (2001). The effect of experience on adults’ acquisition of a second language. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 23, 527552.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E., Yeni-Komshian, G. H., & Liu, S. (1999). Age constraints on second-language acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 78104.Google Scholar
Forsberg Lundell, F., & Bartning, I. (eds.) (2015). Cultural migrants and optimal language acquisition. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gabryś-Barker, D. (ed.) (2017). Third age learners of foreign languages. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
García Mayo, M.P. (ed.) (2017). Learning foreign languages in primary school. Research insights. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
García Mayo, M. P., & García Lecumberri, M. L. (eds.) (2003). Age and the acquisition of English as a foreign language: Theoretical issues and field work. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Gómez Bedoya, M. (2008). El aprendizaje en la tercera edad. Una aproximación en la clase de ELE: Los aprendientes mayores japoneses en el Instituto Cervantes de Tokio. Unpublished master’s thesis, Universidad Antonio de Nebrija, Madrid.Google Scholar
Grady, C. L., & Craik, F. I. M. (2000). Changes in memory processing with age. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 10, 224231.Google Scholar
Granena, G., & Long, M. H. (2013). Age of onset, length of residence, language aptitude, and ultimate L2 attainment in three linguistic domains. Second Language Research, 29(3), 577600. doi:10.1177/0267658312461497.Google Scholar
Green, D. (2017). The interactional challenge: L2 learning and use in the third age. In Gabryś-Barker, D. (ed.), Third age learners of foreign languages. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Grosjean, F. (1998). Studying bilinguals: Methodological and conceptual issues. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 131149.Google Scholar
Hall, C. B., Lipton, R. B., Sliwinski, M., Katz, M. J., Derby, C. A., & Verghese, J. (2009). Cognitive activities delay onset of memory decline in persons who develop dementia. Neurology, 73(5), 356361.Google Scholar
Harada, T. (2014). Not age but length of learning matters in second language speech learning in a minimal input situation. Poster presented at the Conference of the American Association for Applied Linguistics (AAAL), Portland, OR, March.Google Scholar
Hopp, H., & Schmid, M. (2013). Perceived foreign accent in first language attrition and second language acquisition: The impact of age of acquisition and bilingualism. Applied Psycholinguistics, 34(2), 361394.Google Scholar
Huang, B., & Chang, S. (2015). Not all early birds get the worm: The effects of early instruction on long-term foreign language outcomes. Paper presented at the Conference of the American Association for Applied Linguistics (AAAL), Toronto, March.Google Scholar
Jaekel, N., Schurig, M., Florian, M., & Ritterb, M. (2017). From early starters to late finishers? A longitudinal study of early foreign language learning in school. Language Learning, 67(3), 631664.Google Scholar
Jia, G., Aaronson, D., & Wu, Y. (2002). Long-term language attainment of bilingual immigrants: Predictive variables and language group differences. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23, 599621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, J. S., & Newport, E. L. (1989). Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive Psychology, 21, 6099.Google Scholar
Kinsella, C., & Singleton, D. (2014). Much more than age. Applied Linguistics, 35(4), 441462.Google Scholar
Kliesch, M., Giroud, N., Pfenninger, S. E., & Meyer, M. (2018). Research on second language acquisition in old adulthood: What we have and what we need. In Gabryś-Barker, D. (ed.), Third age learners of foreign languages (pp. 4876). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Klingberg, T. (2010). Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(7), 317324.Google Scholar
Kozar, O., & Yates, L. (2017). Factors in language learning after 40: Insights from a longitudinal study. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching (IRAL).Google Scholar
Krashen, S. D., Long, M. H., & Scarcella, R. C. (1979). Age, rate and eventual attainment in second language acquisition. TESOL Quarterly, 9, 573582. Reprinted in Krashen, S. D., Scarcella, R. C., & Long, M. H. (eds.) (1982), Child–adult differences in second language acquisition (pp. 161172). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Kuppens, A. H. (2010). Incidental foreign language acquisition from media exposure. Learning, Media and Technology, 35(1), 6585.Google Scholar
Larson-Hall, J. (2008). Weighing the benefits of studying a foreign language at a younger starting age in a minimal input situation. Second Language Research, 24(1), 3563.Google Scholar
Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). Biological foundations of language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Llanes, À., & Muñoz, C. (2013). Age effects in a study abroad context: Children and adults studying abroad and at home. Language Learning, 63, 6390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Long, M. (1990). Maturational constraints on language development. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 12, 251285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Long, M. (2005). Problems with supposed counter-evidence to the Critical Period Hypothesis. International Review of Applied Linguistics, 43(4), 287317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Long, M. (2013). Maturational constraints on child and adult SLA. In Granena, G. & Long, M. (eds.), Sensitive periods, language aptitude, and ultimate L2 attainment (pp. 341). Amsterdam/Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Mackey, A., Adams, R., Stafford, C., & Winke, P. (2010). Exploring the relationship between modified output and working memory capacity. Language Learning, 60, 501533.Google Scholar
Mackey, A., & Sachs, R. (2012). Older learners in SLA research: A first look at working memory, feedback, and L2 development. Language Learning, 62(3), 704740.Google Scholar
Moyer, A. (2004). Age, accent and experience in second language acquisition. An integrated approach to critical period inquiry. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Moyer, A. (2013). Foreign accent: The phenomenon of non-native speech. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Moyer, A. (2014). Exceptional outcomes in L2 phonology: The critical factors of learner engagement and self-regulation. Applied Linguistics, 35(4), 418440.Google Scholar
Muñoz, C. (ed.) (2006a). Age and the rate of foreign language learning. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Muñoz, C. (2006b). The effects of age on foreign language learning: The BAF Project. In Muñoz, C. (ed.), Age and the rate of foreign language learning (pp. 140). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Muñoz, C. (2008a). Symmetries and asymmetries of age effects in naturalistic and instructed L2 learning. Applied Linguistics, 24(4), 578596.Google Scholar
Muñoz, C. (2008b). Age-related differences in foreign language learning. Revisiting the empirical evidence. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching (IRAL), 46(3), 197220.Google Scholar
Muñoz, C. (2011). Is input more significant than starting age in foreign language acquisition? International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching (IRAL), 49, 113133.Google Scholar
Muñoz, C. (2014a). Complexities and interactions of age in second language learning: Broadening the research agenda. Applied Linguistics, 35(4), 369373.Google Scholar
Muñoz, C. (2014b). Contrasting effects of starting age and input on the oral performance of foreign language learners. Applied Linguistics, 35(4), 463482.Google Scholar
Muñoz, C., Cadierno, T., & Casas, I. (2018). Different starting points for early foreign language learning: A comparative study with Danish and Spanish young learners of English. Language Learning, 68(4), 1076–1109.Google Scholar
Muñoz, C., & Llanes, À. (2014). Study abroad and changes in degree of foreign accent in children and adults. The Modern Language Journal, 98(1), 432449.Google Scholar
Muñoz, C., & Singleton, D. (2011). A critical review of age-related research on L2 ultimate attainment. Language Teaching, 44(1), 135.Google Scholar
Murphy, V. (2014). Second language learning in the early school years. Trends and contexts. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nikolov, M., & Mihaljevíc Djigunovic, J. M. (2011). All shades of every color: An overview of early teaching and learning of foreign languages. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 31, 95119.Google Scholar
Ojima, S., Matsuba-Kurita, H., Nakamura, N., Hoshino, T., & Hagiwara, H. (2011). Age and amount of exposure to a foreign language during childhood: Behavioral and ERP data on the semantic comprehension of spoken English by Japanese children. Neuroscience Research, 70(2), 197205.Google Scholar
Ortega, L. (2005). For what and for whom is our research? The ethical as transformative lens in instructed SLA. The Modern Language Journal, 89, 427443.Google Scholar
Paradis, J. (2011). Individual differences in child English second language acquisition: Comparing child-internal and child-external factors. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1(3), 213237.Google Scholar
Paradis, M. (2009). Declarative and procedural determinants of second languages. Amsterdam/Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Park, D., Lautenschlager, G., Hedden, T., Davidson, N., Smith, A., & Smith, P. (2002). Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. Psychology and Aging, 17, 299320.Google Scholar
Park, D., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. (2009). The adaptive brain: Aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 173196.Google Scholar
Pfenninger, S., & Singleton, D. (2016). Affect trumps age: A person in-context relational view of age and motivation in SLA. Second Language Research, 32(3), 311345.Google Scholar
Pfenninger, S., & Singleton, D. (2017). Beyond age effects in instructional L2 learning. Revisiting the age factor. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Ramírez Gómez, D. (2014). Older-adult foreign language learning: Instructors’ beliefs and some recommendations. In Sonda, N. & Krause, A. (eds.), JALT 2013 Conference Proceedings (pp. 229239). Tokyo: Japan Association for Language Teaching.Google Scholar
Ramírez Gómez, D. (2016). Language teaching and the older adult. The significance of experience. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Rastelli, S. (2014). Discontinuity in second language acquisition. The switch between statistical and grammatical learning. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Ryan, S., & Dörnyei, Z. (2013). The long-term evolution of language motivation and the L2 self. In Berndt, A. (ed.), Fremdsprachen in der Perspektive lebenslangen Lernens (pp. 89100). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T. A. (2004). What and when of cognitive aging. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 140144.Google Scholar
Singleton, D. (2017). Really late learners: Some research context and some practical hints. In Gabryś-Barker, D. (ed.), Third age learners of foreign languages. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Singleton, D., & Pfenninger, S. (2018). Bilingualism in midlife. In Ortega, L., & De Houwer, A. (eds.), The Cambridge handbook of bilingualism (pp. 76–100). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Singleton, D., & Ryan, L. (2004). Language acquisition: The age factor (2nd edn.). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Sun, H. (2015). Predictors and stages of very young child EFL learners’ English development in China. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Groningen.Google Scholar
Sun, H., Steinkrauss, R., Tendeiro, J., & de Bot, K. (2016). Individual differences in very young children’s English acquisition in China: Internal and external factors. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(3), 550566.Google Scholar
Unsworth, S., de Bot, K., Persson, L., & Prins, T. (2012). Final Report: Foreign languages in primary school project. Proceedings of the foreign languages in primary schools project: Presentation of results of the FLiPP-research, Leerhotel Het Klooster, Amersfoort.Google Scholar
Unsworth, S., Persson, L., Prins, T., & de Bot, K. (2015). An investigation of factors affecting early foreign language learning in the Netherlands. Applied Linguistics, 36, 527548.Google Scholar
Van der Hoeven, N., & de Bot, K. (2012). Relearning in the elderly: Age-related effects on the size of savings. Language Learning, 62(1), 4267.Google Scholar
Verghese, J., Lipton, R. B., Katz, M. J., Hall, C. B., Derby, C. A., Kuslansky, G., Ambrose, A. F., Sliwinski, M., & Buschke, H. (2003). Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly. New England Journal of Medicine, 348(25), 25082516.Google Scholar
Wilkinson, A. J., & Yang, L. (2016). Inhibition plasticity in older adults: Practice and transfer effects using multiple task approach. Neural Plasticity, 2016, 112.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×