Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-gq7q9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-18T18:20:30.028Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

8 - Surface structure, intonation, and discourse meaning

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 March 2010

Get access

Summary

Introduction

The structural units of phrasal intonation are frequently orthogonal to the syntactic constituent boundaries that are recognized by traditional grammar and embodied in most current theories of syntax. As a result, much recent work on the relation of intonation to discourse context and information structure has either eschewed syntax entirely (cf. Bolinger, 1972; Cutler and Isard, 1980; Gussenhoven, 1983; Brown and Yule, 1983), or has supplemented traditional syntax with entirely nonsyntactic string-related principles (cf. Cooper and Paccia-Cooper, 1980). Recently, Selkirk (1984) and others have postulated an autonomous level of “intonational structure” for spoken language, distinct from syntactic structure. Structures at this level are plausibly claimed to be related to discourse-related notions, such as “focus”. However, the involvement of two apparently uncoupled levels of structure in Natural Language grammar appears to complicate the path from speech to interpretation unreasonably, and thereby to threaten the feasibility of computational speech recognition and speech synthesis.

In Steedman (1991a), I argue that the notion of intonational structure formalized by Pierrehumbert, Selkirk, and others, can be subsumed under a rather different notion of syntactic surface structure, which emerges from the “Combinatory Categorial” theory of grammar (Steedman, 1987, 1990). This theory engenders surface structure constituents corresponding directly to phonological phrase structure. Moreover, the grammar assigns to these constituents interpretations that directly correspond to what is here called “information structure” – that is, the aspects of discourse-meaning that have variously been termed “topic” and “comment”, “theme” and “rheme”, “given” and “new” information, and/or “presupposition” and “focus”.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 1993

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×