Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7bb8b95d7b-cx56b Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-09-07T04:17:20.813Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

9 - Small Miracles in Tulsa: The Effects of Universal Pre-K on Cognitive Development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Arthur J. Reynolds
Affiliation:
University of Minnesota
Arthur J. Rolnick
Affiliation:
Federal Reserve Bank of Minneapolis
Michelle M. Englund
Affiliation:
University of Minnesota
Judy A. Temple
Affiliation:
University of Minnesota
Get access

Summary

Parents and public officials are increasingly concerned about the school readiness of young children. In response, in recent years, state governments have boosted their support for pre-K programs. Several states, including Oklahoma, have opted for universal pre-K, making it available, on a voluntary basis, to all 4-year-olds. At the same time, the federal government, through the No Child Left Behind Act, has imposed new testing requirements on public schools to determine whether students as a whole and particular subgroups of students are making good academic progress. These trends have heightened interest in the effectiveness of pre-K programs.

The Oklahoma pre-K program is of special interest because it enrolls a higher percentage of 4-year-olds than any pre-K program in the United States (Barnett, Epstein, Friedman, Sansanelli, & Hustedt, 2009). It is also of particular interest because it is based in the public schools and because it places strong emphasis on high quality: All lead teachers must have a college degree and be early-childhood certified; to facilitate the recruitment and retention of outstanding individuals, lead teachers are paid at the same rate as other public school teachers.

Many studies have demonstrated that considerable benefits flow from a high-quality targeted pre-K program. But can a large-scale universal pre-K program also produce substantial benefits by enhancing the school readiness of young children? Do all children benefit from such a program? Do some children benefit more? And how large are the impacts of such a program, in absolute or relative terms?

Type
Chapter
Information
Childhood Programs and Practices in the First Decade of Life
A Human Capital Integration
, pp. 188 - 198
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Barnett, W. S., Epstein, D., Friedman, A., Sansanelli, R., & Hustedt, J. (2009). The state of preschool 2006. New Brunswick, NJ: National Institute for Early Education Research, Rutgers University.Google Scholar
Currie, J., & Thomas, D. (1995, June). Does Head Start make a difference?American Economic Review, 85, 341–364.Google Scholar
Currie, J., & Thomas, D. (1999, November). Does Head Start help Hispanic children?Journal of Public Economics, 74, 235–262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuller, B. (2007). Standardized childhood: The political and cultural struggle over early education. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Garces, E., Thomas, D., & Currie, J. (2002, September). Longer term effects of Head Start. American Economic Review, 92, 999–1012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gormley, W., & Gayer, T. (2005, Summer). Promoting school readiness in Oklahoma: An evaluation of Tulsa's pre-K program. Journal of Human Resources, 40, 533–559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gormley, W., Gayer, T., Phillips, D., & Dawson, B. (2005, November). The effects of universal pre-K on cognitive development. Developmental Psychology, 41, 872–884.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heckman, J. (2000). Policies to foster human capital. Research in Economics, 54, 3–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, V., & Loeb, S. (1995, Spring). Where do Head Start attendees end up? One reason why preschool effects fade out. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 17, 62–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ludwig, J., & Miller, D. (2007). Does Head Start improve children's life chances? Evidence from a regression discontinuity design. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 122, 159–208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Magnuson, K., Ruhm, C., & Waldfogel, J. (2007). Does pre-kindergarten improve school preparation and performance?Economics of Education Review, 78, 115–157.Google Scholar
McKey, R., Condelli, L., Ganson, H., Barrett, B., McConkey, C., & Plantz, M. (1985, June). The impact of Head Start on children, families and communities (Final Report of the Head Start Evaluation, Synthesis and Utilization Project). Washington, DC: CSR.Google Scholar
,National Institute of Child Health and Human Development & Duncan, G. (2003). Modeling the impacts of child care quality on children's preschool cognitive development. Child Development, 74, 1454–1475.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peisner-Feinberg, E., Burchinal, M., Clifford, R., Culkin, M., Howes, C., Kagan, S. L.. (2001, September/October). The relation of preschool child-care quality to children's cognitive and social developmental trajectories through second grade. Child Development, 72, 1534–1553.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phillips, D., Gormley, W., & Lowenstein, A. (2009). Inside the pre-kindergarten door: Classroom climate and instructional time allocation in Tulsa's pre-k programs. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 24(3), 213–228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramey, C., Bryant, D., & Suarez, T. (1985). Preschool compensatory education and the modifiability of intelligence: A critical review. In Detterman, D. K. (Ed.), Current topics in human intelligence: Vol. 1. Research methodology (pp. 247–296). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Ramey, C., Campbell, F., Burchinal, M., Skinner, M., Gardner, D., & Ramey, S. (2000). Persistent effects of early childhood education on high-risk children and their mothers. Applied Developmental Science, 4, 2–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schanzenbach, D. W. (2006, August). What have researchers learned from Project Star? (Harris School Working Paper Series 06.06). Chicago: Harris School of Public Policy.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×