Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-vsgnj Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-22T14:32:53.750Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

35 - Anosognosia and denial of illness

from Part V - Other poststroke disorders

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 October 2009

Robert G. Robinson
Affiliation:
College of Medicine, University of Iowa
Get access

Summary

In 1914, Babinski (1914) reported that some patients with severe hemiplegia appeared to be unaware of their deficit. He called this absence of awareness “anosognosia,” a term derived from the Greek words “gnosis” (knowledge), “a” (without), and “noso” (disease). The medical literature of the late 19th and 20th centuries reported a variety of failures of awareness of one's own deficits or diseases including Anton's syndrome of cortical blindness (Anton 1896), unawareness of hemianopsia, amnesia, dementia, aphasia (particularly fluent aphasia), and mental illnesses like schizophrenia or dementia.

As they are unaware of their illness, patients with anosognosia may be less likely to seek treatment and remain compliant with prescribed medications. They may also engage in potentially dangerous activities based on their belief that they are able to do things that they are not, such as believing themselves capable of driving an automobile when they are not (Heilman and Valenstein 1993). Clinicians, therefore, should monitor their patients' awareness of their limitations.

Denial of illness is similar to anosognosia because patients fail to acknowledge their illness or deficits associated with it. Denial of illness has been reported among patients with brain injury as well as patients without brain injury (Weinstein and Kahn 1955). When stroke patients deny hemiplegia or sensory deficit, the distinction between denial and anosognosia is negligible.

Type
Chapter
Information
The Clinical Neuropsychiatry of Stroke
Cognitive, Behavioral and Emotional Disorders following Vascular Brain Injury
, pp. 366 - 382
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×