Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-k7p5g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-08T13:24:53.570Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

12 - Reasoning Abilities

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 November 2009

Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Jean E. Pretz
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Get access

Summary

The topic of human intelligence exceeds the span of any one discipline or method of inquiry. Different aspects of intelligence are best understood from disciplines as diverse as evolutionary biology, neuropsychology, cognitive psychology, anthropology, and education. At its core, however, intelligence is defined by differences between individuals or species. To say that one individual is more intelligent than another is to make a value judgment. Theories of human intelligence must therefore be able to explain those behaviors or accomplishments that societies value as indicants of intelligence (Sternberg, 1985). Such explanations may, at one extreme, invoke the action of neural mechanisms (Garlick, 2002) or, at the other extreme, the importance of social processes (Vygotsky, 1978). Ultimately, however, the theory must explain individual differences in those complex human behaviors that are most commonly understood as indicants of intelligence. Thus, the central facts to be explained by a theory of intelligence must go beyond faster or more efficient processing of elementary tasks, for example, or the efficiency of biological processes and inherited structures, or the influence of schools, environments, or even cultures. Rather, a theory of intelligence must explain the writing of novels, the solving of complex mathematical problems, the designing of skyscrapers and microchips, and the myriad other forms of complex cognition valued by society. In short, an understanding of how individuals solve complex tasks and an explanation of why they differ so markedly in their ability to do so are central facts for any theory of intelligence.

Type
Chapter
Information
Cognition and Intelligence
Identifying the Mechanisms of the Mind
, pp. 225 - 250
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2004

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ackerman, P. L. (1988). Determinants of individual differences during skill acquisition: A theory of cognitive abilities and information processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 299–329Google Scholar
Ackerman, P. L., Beier, M., & Boyle, M. O. (2002). Individual differences in working memory within a nomological network of cognitive and perceptual speed abilities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 567–589CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alderton, D. L., & Larson, G. E. (1994). Cross-task consistency in strategy use and the relationship with intelligence. Intelligence, 18, 47–76CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bethell-Fox, C. E., Lohman, D. F., & Snow, R. E. (1984). Adaptive reasoning: Componential and eye movement analysis of geometric analogy performance. Intelligence, 8, 205–238CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boekaerts, M. (1995). The interface between intelligence and personality as determinants of classroom learning. In D. H. Saklofske & M. Zeidner (Eds.), International handbook of personality and intelligence (pp. 161–183). New York: Plenum PressCrossRef
Brody, N. (1992). Intelligence. San Diego: Academic Press
Bruner, J. (1957). Going beyond the information given. In J. S. Bruner (Ed.), Contemporary approaches to cognition (pp. 41–69). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Carlstedt, B., Gustafsson, J.-E., & Ullstadius, E. (2000). Item sequencing effects on the measurement of fluid intelligence. Intelligence, 28, 145–160CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, P. A., Just, M. A., & Schell, P. (1990). What one intelligence test measures: A theoretical account of the processing in the Raven Progressive Matrices test. Psychological Review, 97, 404–431CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities. A survey of factor-analytic studies. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University PressCrossRef
Chastain, R. L. (1992). Adaptive processing in complex learning and cognitive performance. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Stanford University, Stanford, CA.
Conway, A. R. A., Cowan, N., Bunting, M. F., Therriault, D. J., & Minkoff, S. R. B. (2002). A latent variable analysis of working memory capacity, short-term memory capacity, processing speed, and general fluid intelligence. Intelligence, 30, 163–183CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corno, L., Cronbach, L. J., Lohman, D. F., Kupermintz, H., & Talbert, J. (2002). Remaking the concept of aptitude: Extending the legacy of Richard E. Snow. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Crawford, J. (1988). Intelligence, task complexity and tests of sustained attention. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes' error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Putnam
Deary, I. J., & Stough, C. (1996). Intelligence and inspection time: Achievements, prospects, and problems. American Psychologist, 51, 599–608CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Detterman, D. K. (1986). Human intelligence is a complex system of separate processes. In R. J. Sternberg & D. K. Detterman (Eds.), What is intelligence? (pp. 57–61). Norwood, NJ: Ablex
Elshout, J. J. (1985). Problem solving and education. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Francisco, June 1985
Embretson, S. E. (1983). Construct validity: Construct representation versus nomothetic span. Psychological Bulletin, 93, 179–197Google Scholar
Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., & Conway, A. R. A. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128(3), 309–331CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Estes, W. K. (1974). Learning theory and intelligence. American Psychologist, 29, 740–749CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, J. S. B. T., & Over, D. E. (1996). Rationality and reasoning. Hove, UK: Psychology Press
Fry, A. F., & Hale, S. (1996). Processing speed, working memory, and fluid intelli-gence: Evidence for a developmental cascade. Psychological Science, 7, 237–241CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garlick, D. (2002). Understanding the nature of the general factor of intelligence: The role of individual differences in neural plasticity as an explanatory mechanism. Psychological Review, 109, 116–136CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldman, S. R., & Pellegrino, J. W. (1984). Deductions about induction: Analyses of developmental and individual differences. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (Vol. 2, pp. 149–197). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Greeno, J. G. (1978). A study of problem solving. In R. Glaser (Ed.), Advances in instructional psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 13–75). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Gustafsson, J.-E. (1988). Hierarchical models of individual differences in cognitive abilities. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (Vol. 4, pp. 35–71). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Gustafsson, J.-E., & Undheim, J. O. (1996). Individual differences in cognitive functions. In D. C. Berliner & R. C. Calfee (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 186–242.) New York: Simon & Schuster Macmillan
Holzman, T. G., Pellegrino, J. W., & Glaser, R. (1982). Cognitive dimensions of numerical rule induction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 360–373CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunt, E. B., Frost, N., & Lunneborg, C. (1973). Individual differences in cognition: A new approach to intelligence. In G. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 7, pp. 87–122). New York: Academic PressCrossRef
Hunt, E., & Lansman, M. (1982). Individual differences in attention. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human abilities (Vol. 1, pp. 207–254). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Hunter, J. E., & Schmidt, F. L. (1996). Intelligence and job performance: Economic and social implications. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 3/4, 447–472CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jensen, A. R. (1982). The chronometry of intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (Vol. 1, pp. 255–310). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g factor: The science of mental ability. Westport, CT: Praeger
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1999). Deductive reasoning. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 109–135CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kyllonen, P. C., & Christal, R. E. (1990). Reasoning ability is (little more than) working-memory capacity?!Intelligence, 14, 389–433CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kyllonen, P. C., Lohman, D. F., & Woltz, D. J. (1984). Componential modeling of alternative strategies for performing spatial tasks. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 1325–1345CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lohman, D. F. (1988). Spatial abilities as traits, processes, and knowledge. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (Vol. 4, pp. 181–248). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Lohman, D. F. (1994). Component scores as residual variation (or why the intercept correlates best). Intelligence, 19, 1–11CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lohman, D. F. (2000). Complex information processing and intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence (pp. 285–340). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University PressCrossRef
Lohman, D. F. (2004). Aptitude for college: The importance of reasoning tests for minority admissions. In R. Zwick (Ed.), Rethinking the SAT: The future of standardized testing in university admissions (pp. 41–56). New York: Routledge Falmer. Web site: http://faculty.education.uiowa.edu/dlohman/
Lohman, D. F., & Hagen, E. (2001a). Cognitive Abilities Test (Form 6): Interpretive guide for teachers and counselors. Itasca, IL: Riverside
Lohman, D. F., & Hagen, E. (2001b). Cognitive Abilities Test (Form 6): Interpretive guide for school administrators. Itasca, IL: Riverside
Lohman, D. F., & Hagen, E. (2002). Cognitive Abilities Test (Form 6): Research handbook. Itasca, IL: Riverside
Markman, A. B., & Genter, D. (2001). Thinking. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 223–247CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshalek, B., Lohman, D. F., & Snow, R. E. (1983). The complexity continuum in the radex and hierarchical models of intelligence. Intelligence, 7, 107–128CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martinez, M. E. (2000). Education as the cultivation of intelligence. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Miyake, A., & Shah, P. (1999). Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press
Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall
Pellegrino, J. W. (1985). Inductive reasoning ability. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Human abilities: An information-processing approach (pp. 195–225). New York: W.H. Freeman
Pellegrino, J. W., & Glaser, R. (1980). Components of inductive reasoning. In R. E. Snow, P.-A. Federico, & W. E. Montague (Eds.), Aptitude, learning, and instruction: Vol. 1. Cognitive process analyses of aptitude (pp. 177–218). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Primi, R. (2001). Complexity of geometric inductive reasoning tasks contribution to the understanding of fluid intelligence. Intelligence, 30, 41–70CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raaheim, K. (1988). Intelligence and task novelty. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (Vol. 4, pp. 73–97). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. (1977). Raven's Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. New York: Psychological Corporation
Roberts, R. D., & Stankov, L. (1999). Individual differences in speed of mental processing and human cognitive abilities: Toward a taxonomic model. Learning and Individual Differences, 11, 1–120CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salthouse, T. A., Babcock, R. L, Mitchell, D. R. D., Palmon, R., & Skovronek, E. (1990). Sources of individual differences in spatial visualization ability. Intelligence, 14, 187–230CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, R. E. (1980). Aptitude and achievement. New Directions for Testing and Measurement, 5, 39–59Google Scholar
Snow, R. E. (1994). Abilities in academic tasks. In R. J. Sternberg & R. K. Wagner (Eds.), Mind in context: Interactionist perspectives on human intelligence (pp. 3–37). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press
Snow, R. E. (1996). Aptitude development and education. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 2, 536–560CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, R. E., & Lohman, D. F. (1989). Implications of cognitive psychology for educational measurement. In R. Linn (Ed.), Educational measurement (3rd ed., pp. 263–331). New York: Macmillan
Snow, R. E., Corno, L., & Jackson, D., III. (1996). Individual differences in affective and conative functions. In D. C. Berliner & R. C. Calfee (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 243–310). New York: Simon & Schuster Macmillan
Snow, R. E., Kyllonen, P. C., & Marshalek, B. (1984). The topography of ability and learning correlations. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (Vol. 2, pp. 47–104). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Spearman, C. E. (1927). The abilities of man. London: Macmillan
Spilsbury, G. (1992). Complexity as a reflection of the dimensionality of a task. Intelligence, 16, 31–45CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stankov, L. (1988). Single tests, competing tasks and their relationship to broad factors of intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 9, 25–33CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (1999). Who is rational? Studies of individual differences in reasoning. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Sternberg, R. J. (1977). Intelligence, information processing, and analogical reasoning: The componential analysis of human abilities. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Beyond IQ: A triarchic theory of human intelligence. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press
Sternberg, R. J. (1986). Toward a unified theory of human reasoning. Intelligence, 10, 281–314CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., & Gardner, M. K. (1983). Unities in inductive reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112, 80–116CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Süß, H.-M., Oberauer, K., Wittmann, W. W., Wilhelm, O., & Schulze, R. (2002). Working memory capacity explains reasoning ability — and a little bit more. Intelligence, 30, 261–288CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swiney, J. F. (1985). A study of executive processes in intelligence. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Stanford University, Stanford, CA.
Toulmin, S., Rieke, R., & Janik, A. (1984). An introduction to reasoning (2nd ed). New York: Macmillan
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Yanng, Y., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2001). Mental models and logical reasoning problems in the GRE. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 7(4), 308–316Google Scholar
Zimmerman, W. S. (1954). The influence of item complexity upon the factor composition of a spatial visualization test. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 14, 106–119CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×