Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-9q27g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-22T00:20:28.715Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 August 2017

David Beswick
Affiliation:
University of Melbourne
Get access
Type
Chapter
Information
Cognitive Motivation
From Curiosity to Identity, Purpose and Meaning
, pp. 317 - 368
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2017

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abelson, R. P., Aronson, E., McGuire, W. J., Newcombe, T. M., Rosenberg, M. J. and Tannenbaum, P. H., Eds. (1968). Theories of cognitive consistency: A source book. Chicago, Rand McNally.Google Scholar
Adolphs, R. (2009). ‘The social brain: Neural basis of social knowledge.’ Annual Review of Psychology 60: 693716.Google Scholar
Adorno, T. W., Frenkel-Brunswick, E., Levinson, D. J. and Sanford, R. N. (1950). The authoritarian personality. New York, Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Ainley, M. D. (1987). ‘The factor structure of curiosity measures: Breadth and depth of interest curiosity styles.’ Australian Journal of Psychology 39: 5359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ainley, M. (2006). ‘Connecting with learning: Motivation, affect and cognition in interest processes.’ Educational Psychology Review 18: 391405.Google Scholar
Ainley, M., Hidi, S. and Berndorff, D. (2002). ‘Interest, learning and the psychological processes that mediate their relationship.’ Journal of Educational Psychology 94(3): 545561.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alkire, M. T., Hudetz, A. G. and Tononi, G. (2008). ‘Consciousness and anesthesia.’ Science 322(5903): 876880.Google Scholar
Allport, A. (1988). What concept of consciousness? In Marcel, A. J. and Brisiach, E. (Eds.), Consciousness in contemporary science (pp. 159182). New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Allport, F. H. (1937). ‘Teleonomic description in the study of personality.’ Character and Personality 5: 202214.Google Scholar
Allport, G. W. (1937a). Personality: A psychological interpretation. London, Constable.Google Scholar
Allport, G. W. (1937b). ‘The functional autonomy of motives.’ American Journal of Psychology, 50: 141156.Google Scholar
Allport, G. W. (1943). ‘The ego in contemporary psychology.’ Psychological Review 50: 451478.Google Scholar
Allport, G. W. (1950). The individual and his religion: A psychological interpretation. New York, MacMillan Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Allport, G. W. (1955). Becoming: Basic considerations in the psychology of personality. New Haven, Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Allport, G. W. (1961b). Pattern and growth in personality. New York, Holt Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Amabile, T. M. (1983a). The social psychology of creativity. New York, Springer- Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amabile, T. M. (1993). ‘Motivational synergy: Towards new conceptions of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation in the workplace.’ Human Resource Management Review 3: 185201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in context. Boulder, Westview Press.Google Scholar
Amabile, T. M. (1998). ‘How to kill creativity.’ Harvard Business Review 76(5): 7687.Google Scholar
Amabile, T. M. and Kramer, S. J. (2010). ‘What really motivates workers.’ Harvard Business Review 88(1): 4445.Google Scholar
Ames, C. (1984). Competitive, cooperative, and individualistic goal structures: A cognitive-motivational analysis. In Ames, R. and Ames, C. (Eds.), Research on motivation in education, Vol 1: Student motivation (pp. 177207). New York, Academic.Google Scholar
Ames, C. (1992). ‘Classrooms: Goals, structures, and student motivation.’ Journal of Educational Psychology 84: 261271.Google Scholar
Anselme, P. (2010). ‘The uncertainty processing theory of motivation.’ Behavioural Brain Research 208(2): 291310.Google Scholar
Antonovsky, A. (1987). Unravelling the mystery of health: How people manage stress and stay well. San Francisco, Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Antonovsky, A. (1998). The sense of coherence: An historical and future perspective. In McCubbin, H. I., Thompson, E. A., Thompson, A. I. and From, J. E. (Eds.), Stress, coping and health in families: Sense of coherence and resiliency (pp. 3–20). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Apperly, I. A. and Butterfill, S. A. (2009). ‘Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states?Psychological Review 116(4): 953970.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Apperly, I. A., Samson, D. and Humphreys, G. W. (2009). ‘Studies of adults can inform accounts of theory of mind development.’ Developmental Psychology 45: 190201.Google Scholar
Appley, M. H. (1991). Motivation, equilibrium, and stress. In Dienster, R. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation 1990 (Vol. 38, pp. 167). Lincoln and London, University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Arnheim, R. (1969). Visual thinking. Berkeley and Los Angeles, University of California Press.Google Scholar
Arnheim, R. (1971). Entropy and art: An essay on disorder and order. Berkeley, University of California Press.Google Scholar
Arnheim, R. (1974). Art and visual perception: A psychology of the creative eye. Berkeley, University of California Press.Google Scholar
Arnheim, R. (1982). The power of the center: A study of composition in the visual arts. Berkeley, University of California Press.Google Scholar
Arnheim, R. (1996). The split and the structure: Twenty-eight essays. Berkeley, University of California Press.Google Scholar
Arnheim, R. (1998). ‘Wolfgang Köhler and Gestalt theory: An English translation of Köhler’s introduction to Diephysischen Gestalten for philosophers and biologists.’ History of Psychology 1: 2126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aronoff, J. (1962). ‘Freud’s conception of the origin of curiosity.’ The Journal of Psychology 54: 3945.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aronson, E. (1958). The need for achievement as measured by graphic expression. In Atkinson, J. W. (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action, and society: A method of assessment and study (pp. 249265). Princeton, NJ, Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Aronson, E. (1968). Dissonance theory: Progress and problems. In Abelson, R. P., Aronson, E., McGuire, W. J. et al. (Eds.), Theories of cognitive consistency: A source book (pp. 5–27). Chicago, Rand McNally.Google Scholar
Aronson, E. (1992). ‘The return of the repressed: Dissonance theory makes a comeback.’ Psychological Inquiry 3: 303311.Google Scholar
Aronson, E. (1999). Dissonance, hypocracy and self-concept. In Harmon-Jones, E. and Mills, J. (Eds.), Cognitive dissonance: Progress on a pivotal theory in social psychology (pp. 103–126). Washington, DC, APA Books.Google Scholar
Aronson, E. and Carlsmith, J. M. (1962). ‘Performance expectancy as a determinant of actual performance.’ Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 65: 178182.Google Scholar
Aronson, E. and Carlsmith, J. M. (1963). ‘Effect of severity of threat on devaluation of forbidden behavior.’ Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 66: 584588.Google Scholar
Aronson, E., Fried, C. and Stone, J. (1991). ‘Overcoming denial and increasing the intention to use condoms through the induction of hypocrisy.’ American Journal of Public Health 81: 16361638.Google Scholar
Aronson, E. and Mills, J. (1959). ‘The effect of severity of initiation on liking for a group.’ Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 59: 177181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aronson, J., Cohen, G. and Nail, P. R. (1999). Self-affirmation theory: An update and appraisal. In Harmon-Jones, E. and Mills, J. (Eds.), Cognitive dissonance: Progress on a pivotal theory in social psychology (pp. 127–145). Washington, DC, APA Books.Google Scholar
Asch, S. E. (1946a). ‘Forming impressions of personality.’ Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 41(3): 258290.Google Scholar
Asch, S. E. (1946b). ‘Max Wertheimer’s contribution to psychology.’ Social Research 13: 81102.Google Scholar
Ash, M. (1995). Gestalt psychology in German culture, 1890–1967: Holism and the quest for objectivity. Cambridge and New York, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W. (1953). ‘The achievement motive and the recall of interrupted and completed tasks.’ Journal of Experimental Psychology 46: 381390.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W. (1957). ‘Motivational determinants of risktaking behavior.’ Psychological Review 64: 359372.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W. (Ed.) (1958a). Motives in fantasy, action, and society: A method of assessment and study. Princeton, NJ, Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W. (1958b). Towards experimental analysis of human motivation in terms of motives, expectancies, and incentives. In Atkinson, J. W. E. (Ed.), Motives in fantasy action and society (pp. 288305). Princeton, NJ, Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W. (1964). An introduction to motivation. New York, Van Nostrand Reinhold.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W. (1982). Old and new conceptions of how expected consequences influence actions. In Feather, N. T. (Ed.), Expectations and actions (pp. 395–420). Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W., Bastian, J. R., Earl, R. W. and Litwin, G. H. (1960). ‘The achievement motive, goalsetting, and probability preferences.’ Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 60: 2736.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W. and Birch, D. (1970). The dynamics of action. New York, Wiley.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W. and Birch, D. (1974). The dynamics of achievement-oriented activity. In Atkinson, J. W. and Raynor, J. O. (Eds.), Motivation and achievement (pp. 271325). Washington, DC, Winston.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W. and Feather, N. T. (1966a). A theory of achievement motivation. New York, Wiley.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W. and Feather, N. T. (1966b). Review and appraisal. In Atkinson, J. W. and Feather, N. T. (Eds.), A theory of achievement motivation (pp. 327–370). New York, Wiley.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W. and Litwin, G. H. (1960). ‘Achievement motive and test anxiety conceived as motive to approach success and motive to avoid failure.’ Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 60: 5263.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. W. and McClelland, D. C. (1948). ‘The projective expression of needs: II. The effect of different intensities of the hunger drive on thematic apperception.’ Journal of Experimental Psychology 38: 643658.Google Scholar
Atkinson, R. C. and Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In Spence, K. W. and Spence, J. T. (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 2, pp. 98195). New York, Academic Press.Google Scholar
Austin, J. T. and Vancouver, J. B. (1996). ‘Goal constructs in psychology: Structure, process, and content.’ Psychological Bulletin 120(3): 338375.Google Scholar
Averill, J. R. (1991). Intellectual emotions. In Spielberger, C. D. and Sarason, I. G. (Eds.), Stress and emotion: Anxiety, anger and curiosity (Vol. 14, pp. 316). New York, Hemisphere Publishing.Google Scholar
Averill, J. R. (1994). Emotions are many splendored things. In Ekman, P. and Davidson, R. J. (Eds.), The nature of emotions: Fundamental questions (pp. 99102). New York and Oxford, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baars, B. J. (1986). The cognitive revolution in psychology. New York, Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Baars, B. J. (1988). A cognitive theory of consciousness. Cambridge and New York, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Baars, B. J. (1997). In the theater of consciousness: The workspace of the mind. New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baars, B. J. (2002a). ‘The conscious access hypothesis: Origins and recent evidence.’ Trends in Cognitive Science 6(1): 4752.Google Scholar
Baars, B. J. (2002b). ‘Behaviorism redux: Review of The illusion of conscious will by Daniel M. Wegner, MIT Press 2002.’ Trends in Cognitive Science 6(6): 268269.Google Scholar
Baars, B. J. (2003). Working memory requires conscious processes, not vice versa: A global workspace account. In Osaka, N. (Ed.), Neural basis of consciousness (pp. 11–26). Amsterdam, John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Baars, B. J. (2005a). ‘Global workspace theory of consciousness: Toward a cognitive neuroscience of human experience.’ Progress in Brain Research 150 (Boundaries of Consciousness: Neurobiology and Neurolopathology): 4553.Google Scholar
Baars, B. J. (2005b). ‘Subjective experience is probably not limited to humans: The evidence from neurobiology and behavior.’ Consciousness & Cognition 14(1): 721.Google Scholar
Baars, B. J. and Gage, N. M. (2007). Cognition, brain, and consciousness: Introduction to cognitive neuroscience. Amsterdam and London, Academic.Google Scholar
Baars, B. J., Ramsøy, T. Z. and Laureys, S. (2003). ‘Brain, conscious experience and the observing self.’ Trends in Neurosciences 26(12): 671675.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford and New York, Clarendon Press and Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (1996). ‘Exploring the central executive.’ Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 49: 528.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (2000). ‘The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory.’ Trends in Cognitive Science 4(11): 417423.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (2007). Working memory, thought, and action. Oxford and New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (2012). ‘Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies.’ Annual Review of Psychology 63: 129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. D. and Della Sala, S. (1998). Working memory and executive control. In Roberts, A. C., Robbins, T. W. and Weiskrantz, L. E. (Eds.), The prefrontal cortex: Executive and cognitive functions: Royal Society (Great Britain). Discussion Meeting (pp. 921). New York and Oxford, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. and Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In Bower, G. H. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8). New York, Academic.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. and Logie, R. H. (1999). Working memory: The multiple component model. In Miyake, A. and Shah, P. (Eds.), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control (pp. 2861). Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1969). Principles of behavior modification. New York, Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1977a). ‘Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change.’ Psychological Review 84: 191215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandura, A. (1977b). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ, Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1982). ‘Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency.’ American Psychologist 37(2): 122147.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ, Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1989a). ‘Human agency in social cognitive theory.’ American Psychologist 44: 11751184.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1989b). Self-regulation of motivation and action through internal standards and goal systems. In Pervin, L. A. (Ed.), Goal concepts in personality and social psychology (pp. 1988). Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1991). Self-regulation of motivation through anticipatory and self-reactive mechanisms. In Diestbier, R. (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation 1990: Perspectives on motivation (Vol. 38, pp. 69164). Lincoln and London, University of Nebraska.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York, W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1999). A social cognitive theory of personality. In Pervin, L. and John, O. (Eds.), Handbook of personality (2nd ed., pp. 154196). New York, Guilford.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (2001). ‘Social cognitive theory: An agentic perspective.’ Annual Review of Psychology 52: 126.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (2008). Reconstrual of ‘free will’ from the agentic perspective or social cognitive. In Baer, J., Kaufman, J. C. and Baumeister, R. F. (Eds.), Are we free? Psychology and free will (pp. 86123). Oxford and New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. and Walters, R. H. (1959). Adolescent aggression. New York, Ronald Press.Google Scholar
Barnett, S. A. (1958). ‘Exploratory behavior.’ British Journal of Psychology 9: 289310.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F. (2006a). ‘Emotions as natural kinds?Perspectives on Psychological Science 1: 2858.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F. (2006b). ‘Solving the emotion paradox: Categorization and the experience of emotion.’ Personality and Social Psychology Review 10: 2046.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F. (2012). ‘Emotions are real.’ Emotion 12(3): 413429.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F., Lindquist, K. A., Bliss-Moreau, E., Duncan, S., Gendron, M., Mize, J. and Brennan, L. (2007). ‘Of mice and men: Natural kinds of emotions in the mammalian brain? A response to Panksepp and Izard.’ Perspectives on Psychological Science 2: 297312.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. W., Niedenthal, P. M., Barbey, A. K. and Ruppert, J. A. (2003). Social embodiment. In Ross, B. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation. (Vol. 43, pp. 4392). New York, Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bartels, J. M., Magun-Jackson, S. and Ryan, J. J. (2010). ‘Dispositional approach-avoidance achievement motivation and cognitive self-regulated learning: The mediation of achievement goals.’ Individual Differences Research 8(2): 97110.Google Scholar
Batson, C. D., Schoenrade, P. and Ventis, W. L. (1992). Religion and the individual: A social-psychological perspective. New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Batthyany, A. (2009). Reclaiming conscious agency: Mental causation after Libet. In Batthyany, A. and Elitzur, A. (Eds.), Irreducibly conscious: Selected papers on consciousness (pp. 1–27). Heidelberg, Winter.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Meanings of life. New York, Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F. (1998). The self. In Gilbert, D. T., Fiske, Susan T., and Lindzey, Gardener (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 680740). Boston, McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F. (2002). ‘Religion and psychology: Introduction to the special issue.’ Psychological Inquiry 13(3): 165167.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F. (2005). The cultural animal: Human nature, meaning, and social life. Oxford and New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F. (2008). Free will, consciousness, and cultural animals. In Baer, J., Kaufman, J. C. and Baumeister, R. F. (Eds.), Are we free? Psychology and free will (pp. 39). Oxford and New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F. (2016). ‘Toward a general theory of motivation: Problems, challenges, opportunities, and the big picture.’ Motivation and Emotion 40: 110.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M. and Tice, D. M. (1998). ‘Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74: 12521265.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F. and Masicampo, E. J. (2010). ‘Conscious thought is for facilitating social and cultural interactions: How mental simulations serve the animal-culture interface.’ Psychological Review 117(3): 945971.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., Masicampo, E. J. and Vohs, K. D. (2011). ‘Do conscious thoughts cause behavior?Annual Review of Psychology 62: 331361.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., Mele, A. R. and Vohs, K. D., Eds. (2010). Free will and consciousness: How might they work? New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F. and Monroe, A. E. (2014). ‘Recent research on free will: Conceptualizations, beliefs, and processes.’ Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 50: 152.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F. and Tierney, J. (2011). Willpower: Rediscovering the greatest human strength. New York, Penguin Press.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F. and Vohs, K. D., Eds. (2004). Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications. New York, Guilford.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., DeWall, C. N. and Zhang, L. (2007). ‘How emotion shapes behavior: Feedback, anticipation, and reflection, rather than direct causation.’ Personality and Social Psychology Review 11: 167203.Google Scholar
Bayley, J. (1998). Iris: A memoir of Iris Murdoch. London, Duckworth.Google Scholar
Beauvois, J.-L. and Joule, R.-V. (1996). A radical dissonance theory. London, Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Beauvois, J.-L. and Joule, R.-V. (1999). A radical point of view on dissonance theory. In Harmon-Jones, E. and Mills, J. (Eds.), Cognitive dissonance: Progress on a pivotal theory in social psychology (pp. 4370). Washington, DC, APA Books.Google Scholar
Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H. and Anderson, S. (1994). ‘Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex.’ Cognition 50: 715.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D. and Damasio, A. R. (1997). ‘Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy.’ Science 275: 12931295.Google Scholar
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D. and Damasio, A. R. (2005). ‘The Iowa Gambling Task and the somatic marker hypothesis: Some questions and answers.’ Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9: 159162.Google Scholar
Bennett, M. R., Dennett, D. C., Hacker, P. and Searle, J. (2007). Neuroscience and philosophy: Brain, mind, and language. New York, Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Bennett, M. R. and Hacker, P. M. S. (2003). Philosophical foundations of neuroscience. Malden, MA, Blackwell Publishers.Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1949). ‘Interest as a psychological concept.’ British Journal of Psychology 39: 184185.Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1950). ‘Novelty and curiosity as determinants of exploratory behavior.’ British Journal of Psychology 41: 6880.Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1954). ‘A theory of human curiosity.’ British Journal of Psychology 45: 180191.Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1955). ‘The arousal and satiation of perceptual curiosity in the rat.’ Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 45: 180191.Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1957). ‘Conflict and information theory variables as determinants of human perceptual curiosity.’ Journal of Experimental Psychology 53: 399404.Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1958). ‘The influence of complexity and novelty in visual figures on orienting responses.’ Journal of Experimental Psychology 55: 289296.Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1960). Conflict, arousal, and curiosity. New York, McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1963). Motivational problems raised by exploratory and epistemic behavior. In Koch, S. (Ed.), Psychology: The study of a science (Vol. 5, pp. 284364). New York, McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1965). Structure and direction in thinking. New York, Wiley.Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1966). ‘Curiosity and exploration.’ Science 153(July): 2533.Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1971a). What next? Concluding summary. In Day, H. I., Berlyne, D. E. and Hunt, D. E. E. (Eds.), Intrinsic motivation: A new direction in education (pp. 186196). Toronto, Holt, Rinehart and Winston of Canada.Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1975). ‘Behaviourism? Cognitive theory? Humanistic psychology? – To Hull with them all.’ Canadian Psychological Review 16: 6980.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1978). ‘Curiosity and learning.’ Motivation and Emotion 2(2): 97175.Google Scholar
Berridge, K. C. (1996). ‘Food reward: Brain substrates of wanting and liking.’ Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Review 20: 125.Google Scholar
Berridge, K. C. (1999). Pleasure, pain, desire, and dread: Hidden core processes of emotion. In Kahneman, D., Diener, E. and Schwarz, N. (Eds.), Well-being: Foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 527559). New York, Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Berridge, K. C. and Robinson, T. E. (1998). ‘The role of dopamine in reward: Hedonics, learning, or incentive salience?Brain Research Reviews 28: 308367.Google Scholar
Beswick, B. (2012). A solution to the paradox of taste. PhD dissertation. School of Historical and Philosophical studies. The University of Melbourne.Google Scholar
Beswick, D. G. (1964). Theory and measurement of human curiosity. PhD dissertation. Social Relations (Social Psychology). Harvard University, Cambridge, MA. Available at http://www.beswick.info/psychres/BeswickCuriosity1964.html.Google Scholar
Beswick, D. G. (1971). Cognitive process theory of individual differences in curiosity. In Day, H. I., Berlyne, D. E. and Hunt, D. E. (Eds.), Intrinsic Motivation: A new direction in education (pp. 156170). Toronto, Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Beswick, D. G. (1974). ‘Intrinsic motivation in senior secondary school students.’ Education Research and Perspectives 1: 1525.Google Scholar
Beswick, D. G. (1983). ‘The changing student population in Australia from the seventies to the eighties.’ Journal of Tertiary Educational Administration: 519.Google Scholar
Beswick, D. G. (2004). ‘From curiosity to identity: Wonder, curiosity, purpose, and identity; the function of identity in the psychology of intrinsic motivation.’ Available at http://www.beswick.info/psychres/CuriosityIdentity.htm.Google Scholar
Beswick, D. G. (2009). Management implications of the interaction between intrinsic motivation and extrinsic rewards. In Raj, R. (Ed.), Intrinsic motivation (pp. 130159). Punagutta, Hyderabad, India, Icfai University Press.Google Scholar
Beswick, D. G. and Boreham, J. (1986). Development of scientific interests and competence, Research Working Paper 86.1. From a paper presented at the 53rd ANZAAS Congress, Perth, May 1983. Melbourne, Centre for the Study of Higher Education, University of Melbourne.Google Scholar
Beswick, D. G. and Ramsden, P. (1987). ‘Curiosity and learning with understanding.’ Journal of Higher Education (India) 13: 2643.Google Scholar
Beswick, D. G. and Tallmadge, G. K. (1971). ‘Re-examination of two learning style experiments in the light of the cognitive process theory of curiosity.’ Journal of Educational Psychology 62: 456462.Google Scholar
Birch, D., Atkinson, J. W. and Bongard, K. (1974). Cognitive control of action. In Weiner, B. (Ed.), Cognitive views of human motivation (pp. 7184). New York, Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bless, H. (2002). ‘Where has the feeling gone? The signal function of affective states.’ Psychological Inquiry 13(1): 2931.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. (2012). ‘Religion, morality, evolution.’ Annual Review of Psychology 63: 179199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bolles, R. C. (1972). ‘Reinforcement, expectancy, and learning.’ Psychological Review 79(5): 394409.Google Scholar
Bolles, R. C. (1974). Cognition and motivation: Some historic trends. In Weiner, B. (Ed.), Cognitive views of human motivation. (pp. 120). New York, Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bolles, R. C. (1975). Theory of motivation (2nd ed.). New York, Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss, Vol. 1: Attachment. New York, Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1979). The making and breaking of affectional bonds. London, Tavistock.Google Scholar
Boyer, P. (2003). ‘Religious thought and behaviour as by-products of brain function.’ Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7: 119124.Google Scholar
Brass, M. and Haggard, P. (2007). ‘To do or not to do: The neural signature of self-control.’ Journal of Neuroscience 27: 91419145.Google Scholar
Bratman, M. E. (1999). Faces of intention: Selected essays on intention and agency. New York, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Brehm, J. (1956). ‘Post-decision changes in the desirability of alternatives.’ Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 52: 384389.Google Scholar
Brehm, J. W. (1962). Motivation effects of cognitive dissonance. In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (pp. 51–77). Lincoln, University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Brehm, J. W. and Cohen, A. R. (1962). Explorations in cognitive dissonance. New York, Wiley.Google Scholar
Broadbent, D. E. (1954). ‘The role of auditory localization in attention and memory span.’ Journal of Experimental Psychology 47: 191196.Google Scholar
Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and communication. New York, Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1957). ‘On perceptual readiness.’ Psychological Review 64: 123152.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1962). On knowing; essays for the left hand. Cambridge, MA, Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1966). Toward a theory of instruction. Cambridge, MA, Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1996). The culture of education. Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (2003). Making stories: Law, literature, life. Cambridge, MA and London, Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Buber, M. (1937). I and thou. New York, Scribner.Google Scholar
Buck, R. (1985). ‘Prime theory: An integrated view of motivation and emotion.’ Psychological Review 92(3): 389413.Google Scholar
Burkart, J. M. (2009). Socio-cognitive abilities in cooperative breeding. In Roska-Hardy, L. S. and Neumann, E. M. (Eds.), Learning from animals? Examining the nature of human uniqueness (pp. 123141). New York, Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Butler, R. A. (1957). ‘The effect of deprivation of visual incentives on visual exploration motivation in monkeys.’ Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 48: 247249.Google Scholar
Butler, R. A. (1958). ‘Exploratory and related behavior: A new trend in animal research.’ Journal of Individual Psychology 14: 111120.Google Scholar
Butterfield, H. (1949). The origins of modern science. London, G. Bell.Google Scholar
Byman, R. (2005). ‘Curiosity and sensation seeking: A conceptual and empirical examination.’ Personality and Individual Differences 38: 13651379.Google Scholar
Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., Feinstein, J., A. and Jarvis, W. B. G. (1996). ‘Dispositional differences in cognitive motivation: The life and times of individuals varying in need for cognition.’ American Psychologist 119(2): 197253.Google Scholar
Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E. and Kao, C. F. (1984). ‘The efficient assessment of need for cognition.’ Journal of Personality Assessment 48: 306307.Google Scholar
Call, J. and Tomasello, M. (1998). ‘Distinguishing intentional from accidental actions in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens).’ Journal of Comparative Psychology 112: 192206.Google Scholar
Call, J. and Tomasello, M. (1999). ‘A nonverbal false belief task: The performance of children and great apes.’ Child Development 70: 381395.Google Scholar
Call, J. and Tomasello, M. (2008). ‘Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later.’ Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12: 187192.Google Scholar
Camazine, S. (2003). ‘Patterns in nature.’ Natural History 112(5): 3441.Google Scholar
Camazine, S., Deneubourg, S.-L., Franks, N., Sneyd, J., Theraulag, G. and Banabeau, E. (2001). Self-organization in biological systems. Princeton, NJ, Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Cameron, J. and Pierce, W. D. (1994). ‘Reinforcement, reward, and intrinsic motivation. A meta-analysis.’ Review of Educational Research 64: 363423.Google Scholar
Cannon, W. B. (1927). ‘The James-Lange theory of emotions: A critical examination and an alternative theory.’ American Journal of Psychology 39: 106124.Google Scholar
Cannon, W. B. (1929). Bodily changes in pain, hunger, fear and rage. New York, Appleton.Google Scholar
Carbonneau, N., Vallerand, R. J. and Lafrenière, M. K. (2012). ‘Towards a tripartite model of intrinsic motivation.’ Journal of Personality 80, 1147–1178.Google Scholar
Carson, R., Kelsh, N. and Pratt, C. (1998/1964). The sense of wonder. New York, HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Cartwright, D. and Harary, F. (1956). ‘Structural balance: A generalization of Heider’s theory.’ Psychological Review 63: 277293.Google Scholar
Carver, C. S. and Scheier, M. F. (1990). ‘Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: A control-process view.’ Psychological Review 97: 1935.Google Scholar
Carver, C. S. and Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of behavior. New York, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Carver, C. S. and Scheier, M. F. (2000). On the structure of behavioral self-regulation. In Boekaerts, M., Pintrich, P. R. and Zeidner, M. H. (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 42–62). San Diego, CA, Academic Press.Google Scholar
Carver, C. S. and Scheier, M. F. (2009). Action, affect, multitasking, and layers of control. In Forgas, J. P., Baumeister, R. and Tice, D. M. (Eds.), Psychology of self-regulation (pp. 109126). New York, London, Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Cavalier, R. J. (2002). Personal motivation: A model for decision making. Westport, Praeger.Google Scholar
Cave, K. R. and Wolfe, J. M. (1990). ‘Modeling the role of parallel processing in visual search.’ Cognitive Psychology 22: 225271.Google Scholar
Chan, R. C. K., Shum, D., Toulopoulou, T. and Chen, E. Y. H. (2008). ‘Assessment of executive functions: Review of instruments and identification of critical issues.’ Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 23(2): 201216.Google Scholar
Chaucer, G. (1387/1969). The Prologue and Three Tales: Edited with notes and commentary by Francis King and Bruce Steele. Melbourne, Cheshire.Google Scholar
Cherry, C. (1956). Information theory: Papers read at a symposium on information theory held at the Royal Institution, London, September 12th to 16th, 1955. London, Butterworths Scientific Publications.Google Scholar
Cherry, C. (1957). On human communication: A review, a survey, and a criticism. Cambridge, MA, Technology Press of Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Cherry, E. C. (1953). ‘Some experiments on the recognition of speech, with one and with two ears.’ Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 25: 975979.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1959). ‘A review of B. F. Skinner’s “Verbal Behavior”.’ Language 35(1): 2658.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Church, R. M. (1963). ‘The varied effects of punishment.’ Psychological Review 70: 369402.Google Scholar
Clore, G. (1992). Cognitive phenomenology: Feelings and the construction of judgement. In Martin, L. L. and Tesser, A. (Eds.), The construction of social judgments (pp. 133163). Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Clore, G. and Ortony, A. (2000). Cognition in emotion: Always, sometimes, or never? In Lane, R. D. and Nadel, L. (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of emotion (pp. 2461). New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Clore, G. L. (1994). Why emotions are felt. In Ekman, P. and Davidson, R. J. (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 103111). New York and Oxford, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Clore, G. L., Ortony, A. and Foss, M. A. 1987. ‘The psychological foundations of the affective lexicon’. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(4), 751–766.Google Scholar
Clore, G. L., Wyer, R. S., Dienes, B., Gasper, K., Gohm, C. and Isbell, L. (2001). Affective feelings as feedback: Some cognitive consequences. In Martin, L. L. and Clore, G. L. (Eds.), Theories of mood and cognition: A user’s handbook (pp. 2762). Mahwah, NJ, Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Coleridge, S. T. (1825). Aids to reflection in the formation of a manly character on the several grounds of prudence, morality, and religion: illustrated by select passages from our elder divines, especially from Archbishop Leighton. London, Printed for Taylor and Hessey.Google Scholar
Coleridge, S. T. and Coleridge, H. N. (1852). On the constitution of the church and state according to the idea of each. London, Edward Moxon.Google Scholar
Collins, R. P., Litman, J. A. and Spielberger, C. D. (2004). ‘The measurement of perceptual curiosity.’ Personality and Individual Differences 36: 11271141.Google Scholar
Conradi, P. J. (2001). Iris Murdoch: A life. London, Harper Collins.Google Scholar
Conway, M. A. and Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). ‘The construction of autobiographical memories in the self memory system.’ Psychological Review 107: 261288.Google Scholar
Cooper, J. (1999). Unwanted consequences and the self: In search of the motivation for dissonance reduction. In Harmon-Jones, E. and Mills, J. (Eds.), Cognitive dissonance: Progress on a pivotal theory in social psychology (pp. 149173). Washington, DC, APA Books.Google Scholar
Cooper, J. (2010). Riding the D train with Elliot: The Aronsonian legacy of cognitive dissonance. In Gonzales, M. H., Tarvis, C. and Aronson, J. (Eds.), The scientist and the humanist: A festschrift in honor of Elliot Aronson (pp. 159174). New York, Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Cooper, J. and Fazio, R. H. (1984). A new look at dissonance theory. In Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 229266). Orlando, FL, Academic Press.Google Scholar
Cooper, M., O’Hara, M., Schmid, P. and Wyatt, G. (2007). The handbook of person-centered psychotherapy and counseling. London, Palgrave MacMillan.Google Scholar
Corballis, M. C. (2004). ‘The origins of modernity: Was autonomous speech the critical factor?Psychological Review 111: 543552.Google Scholar
Corballis, M. C. (2009). ‘The evolution of language.’ Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1156: 1943.Google Scholar
Corlis, C. and Wiess, J. (1973). Curiosity and openness: Empirical testing of a basic assumption. New Orleans, Annual Conference of the American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Cornwell, J. F. M. and Higgins, E. T. (2016). ‘Eager feelings and vigilant reasons: Regulatory focus differences in judging moral wrongs.’ Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 145(3): 338355.Google Scholar
Corteen, R. S. and Wood, B. (1972). ‘Autonomic responses to shockassociated words in an unattended channel.’ Journal of Experimental Psychology 94: 308313.Google Scholar
Couch, A. and Keniston, K. (1960). ‘Yeasayers and naysayers: Agreeing response set as a personality variable.’ Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 60: 151174.Google Scholar
Cowan, N. (1988). ‘Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, and their mutual constraints within the human information processing system.’ Psychological Bulletin 104: 163191.Google Scholar
Cowan, N. (1993). ‘Activation, attention and short term memory.’ Memory & Cognition 21: 162167.Google Scholar
Cowan, N. (1995). Attention and memory: An integrated framework. Oxford, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cowan, N. (1999). An embedded processes model of working memory. In Miyake, A. and Shah, P. (Eds.), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control (pp. 62–101). Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cowan, N. (2005). Working memory capacity. Hove, Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Crane, R. (2009). Mindfulness based cognitive therapy: Distinctive features. New York, Routledge.Google Scholar
Crick, F. (1984). ‘Function of the thalamic reticular complex: The searchlight hypothesis.’ Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 81: 45864590.Google Scholar
Crick, F. and Koch, C. (1990). ‘Towards a neurobiological theory of consciousness.’ Seminars in Neurosciences 2: 263275.Google Scholar
Crick, F. and Koch, C. (1995). ‘Are we aware of neural activity in primary visual cortex?Nature 375: 121123.Google Scholar
Crick, F. and Koch, C. (2003). ‘A framework for consciousness.’ Nature: Neuroscience 6: 119126.Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1988). Society, culture, and person: A systems view of creativity. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), The nature of creativity (pp. 325339). Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York, Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. and Nakamura, J. (2011). Positive psychology: Where did it come from, where is it going? In Sheldon, K. M., Kashdan, T. B. and Steger, M. F. (Eds.), Designing positive psychology (pp. 38). Oxford and New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dalgleish, T. and Power, M. J. (1999). Handbook of cognition and emotion. Chichester, New York, Weinheim, Brisbane, Singapore and Toronto, John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York, G. P. Putnam.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (1996). ‘The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex.’ Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London (series B) 351: 14131420.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (1999). The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. New York, Harcourt Brace.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (2000a). A second chance for emotion. In Lane, R. D. and Nadel, L. (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of emotion (pp. 1223). New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (2000b). The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. New York, Harcourt Brace.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (2003). Looking for Spinoza: Joy, sorrow, and the feeling brain. New York, Harcourt.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (2004). William James and the modern neurobiology of emotion. In Evans, D. and Cruse, P. (Eds.), Emotion, evolution and rationality (pp. 3–14). Oxford, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (2010). Self comes to mind. New York, Pantheon Books.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. and Anderson, S. W. (2003). The frontal lobes. In Heilman, K. M. and Valenstein, E. (Eds.), Clinical neuropsychology (4th ed., pp. 404–446). New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R., Tranel, D. and Damasio, H. (1990). ‘Individuals with sociopathic behavior caused by frontal damage fail to respond autonomically to social stimuli.’ Behavioural Brain Research 41: 8194.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R., Tranel, D. and Damasio, H. (1991). Somatic markers and the guidance of behaviour: Theory and preliminary testing. In Levin, H. S., Eisenberg, H. M. and Benton, A. L. (Eds.), Frontal lobe function and dysfunction (pp. 217229). New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Damasio, H., Grabowski, T., Frank, R., Galaburda, A. M. and Damasio, A. R. (1994). ‘The return of Phineas Gage: Clues about the brain from the skull of a famous patient.’ Science 264: 11021105.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1872/1998). Expression of the emotions in man and animals. Chicago, University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1988). Beagle diary, edited by Keynes, R. D.. London, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. and Barlow, N. (1958). The autobiography of Charles Darwin, 1809–1882: With original omissions restored. London, Collins.Google Scholar
Darwin, C., Ekman, P. and Prodger, P. (1998). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. London, HarperCollins Publishers.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. R. (1876–1882). Recollections of the development of my mind & character: Autobiography [1876-4.1882] CUL-DAR26.1–121 Transcribed by Kees Rookmaaker. Darwin Online. Available at http://darwin-online.org.uk/.Google Scholar
Davies, P. (1999a). The fifth miracle: The search for the origin and meaning of life. New, London, Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1989). The selfish gene (2nd ed.). Oxford, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Day, H. (1969). An instrument for the measurement of intrinsic motivation: An interim report to the Department of Manpower and Immigration. Toronto, Canada, Ontario Institute for Studies in Education.Google Scholar
Day, H. (1971). The measure of specific curiosity. In Day, H., Berlyne, D. E. and Hunt, D. E. (Eds.), Intrinsic motivation: A new direction in education (pp. 99112). Toronto, Holt, Rinehart & Winston of Canada.Google Scholar
Day, H. I. (1968). ‘The role of specific curiosity in school achievement.’ Journal of Educational Psychology 59: 3743.Google Scholar
Day, H. I., Berlyne, D. E. and Hunt, D. E., Eds. (1971). Intrinsic motivation: A new direction in education. Toronto, Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
deCharms, R. (1968). Personal causation: The internal affective determinants of behavior. New York, Academic Press.Google Scholar
deCharms, R. (1987). Personal causation, agency, and the self. In Young-Eisendrath, P. and Hall, J. A. E. (Eds.), The book of the self (pp. 7741). New York, New York University Press.Google Scholar
deCharms, R. (1992). Personal causation and the origin concept. In Smith, C. P. E. (Ed.), Motivation and personality: Handbook of thematic content analysis (pp. 325333). Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
deCharms, R. and Plimpton, F. (1992). The origin scoring system. In Smith, C. P. E. (Ed.), Motivation and personality: Handbook of thematic content analysis (pp. 334375). Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Deci, E. L. (1971). ‘Effects of externally mediated rewards on intrinsic motivation.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 18: 105155.Google Scholar
Deci, E. L. (1972). ‘The effects of contingent and non-contingent rewards and controls on intrinsic motivation.’ Organizational Behavior and Human Performance 8: 217229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deci, E. L. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New York, Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Deci, E. L. (1980). The psychology of self-determination. Lexington, MA, Lexington Books.Google Scholar
Deci, E. L. and Flaste, R. (1995). Why we do what we do: The dynamics of personal autonomy. New York, Putnam’s Sons.Google Scholar
Deci, E. L., Koestner, L. R. and Ryan, R. M. (1999a). ‘The undermining effect is a reality after all – extrinsic rewards, task interest, and self-determination: Reply to Eisenberger, Pierce and Cameron (1999) and Lepper, Henderlong, and Griingras (1999).’ Psychological Bulletin 125, 692700.Google Scholar
Deci, E. L., Koestner, R. and Ryan, R. M. (1999b). ‘A meta-analytic review of experiments examining the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation.’ Psychological Bulletin 125: 627668.Google Scholar
Deci, E. L., Koestner, R. and Ryan, R. M. (2001). ‘Extrinsic rewards and intrinsic motivation in education: Reconsidered once again.’ Review of Educational Research 71(1): 127.Google Scholar
Deci, E. L. and Moller, A. C. (2005). The concept of competence: A starting place for understanding intrinsic motivation and self-determined extrinsic motivation. In Elliot, A. J. and Dweck, C. S. (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 579597). New York, Guilford.Google Scholar
Deci, E. L. and Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York, Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Deci, E. L. and Ryan, R. M. (1987). ‘The support of autonomy and the control of behavior.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 53: 10241037.Google Scholar
Deci, E. L. and Ryan, R. M. (1991). ‘A motivational approach to self: Integration in personality.’ Nebraska Symposium on Motivation 1990: 237288.Google Scholar
Deci, E. L. and Ryan, R. M. (2000). ‘The ‘what’ and ‘why’ of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior.’ Psychological Inquiry 11: 227268.Google Scholar
Deci, E. L. and Ryan, R. M. (2002a). Handbook of self-determination research. Rochester, NY, University of Rochester Press.Google Scholar
Deci, E. L. and Ryan, R. M. (2002b). Overview of self-determination theory: An organismic-dialectical perspective. In Deci, E. L. and Ryan, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of self-determination research (pp. 333). Rochester, NY, University of Rochester Press.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S., Duhamel, J.-R., Hauser, M. D. and Rizzolatti, G., Eds. (2005). From monkey brain to human brain. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S., Kerszberg, M. and Changeux, J. P. (1998). ‘A neuronal model of a global workspace in effortful cognitive tasks.’ Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 95: 1452914534.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S. and Naccache, L. (2001). ‘Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: Basic evidence and a workspace framework.’ Cognition 79: 137.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Cohen, L., Le Bilan, D., Mangin, J.-R., Poline, J.-B. and Rivire, D. (2001). ‘Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming.’ Nature Neuroscience 4 (678–680): 137.Google Scholar
Dember, W. N. and Earl, R. W. (1957). ‘Analysis of exploratory, manipulatory, and curiosity behaviors.’ Psychological Review 64: 9196.Google Scholar
Dembo, T. (1931). ‘Der Arger als dynamisches problem. (Untersuchungen zur Handlungs-und Affektpsychologie. X. Ed. by Kurt Lewin.) Psychologiche Forschung 15: 1144.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1971). ‘Intentional systems.’ Journal of Philosophy 8: 87106.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1978). Brainstorms: Philosophical essays on mind and psychology. Montgomery, VT, Bradford.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1984). Elbow room: The varieties of free will worth wanting. Oxford and New York, Clarendon Press and Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1987). The intentional stance. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1991). Consciousness explained. Boston, Little Brown and Co.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1996). Kinds of minds: Toward an understanding of consciousness. New York, Basic Books.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1998). Brainchildren: Essays on designing minds. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (2001). ‘Are we explaining consciousness yet?Cognition 79: 221237.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (2003). Freedom evolves. New York, Viking.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (2004). ‘Calling in the Cartesian loans: Commentary on Wegner’s The illusion of conscious will.’ Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27: 661.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (2006). Breaking the spell: Religion as a natural phenomenon. New York, Viking.Google Scholar
Desmond, A. and Moore, J. (2010). Darwin’s sacred cause: Race, slavery and the quest for human origins. London, Penguin.Google Scholar
Desmond, A. J. and Moore, J. R. (1991). Darwin. London, Michael Joseph.Google Scholar
Deutsch, J. A. and Deutsch, D. (1963). ‘Attention: Some theoretical considerations.’ Psychological Review 70: 8090.Google Scholar
DeWall, C. N., Baumeister, R. F. and Masicampo, E. J. (2008). ‘Evidence that logical reasoning depends on conscious processing.’ Consciousness and Cognition 17: 628645.Google Scholar
Diamond, M. C. (1988). Enriching heredity. New York, Free Press.Google Scholar
Dickerson, C. A., Thibodeau, R., Aronson, E. and Miller, D. (1992). ‘Using cognitive dissonace to encourage water conservation.’ Journal of Applied Social Psychology 22: 841854.Google Scholar
Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E. and Smith, H. L. (1999). ‘Subjective well-being: Three decades of progress.’ Psychological Bulletin 125: 276302.Google Scholar
Digman, J. M. (1990). ‘Personality structure: Emergence of the Five-Factor Model.’ Annual Review of Psychology 41: 417440.Google Scholar
Dijksterhuis, A., Chartrand, T. L. and Aarts, H. (2007). Effects of priming and perception on social behavior and goal pursuit. In Bargh, J. A. (Ed.), Social psychology and the unconscious: The automaticity of higher mental processes (pp. 51132). Philadelphia, Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Dolan, R. J. (2002). ‘Emotion, cognition, and behavior.’ Science 298: 11911194.Google Scholar
Dollard, L. and Miller, N. E. (1950). Personality and psychotherapy. New York, McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Dweck, C. S. (1985). Intrinsic motivation, perceived control, and self-evaluation maintenance: An achievement goal analysis. In Ames, R. and Ames, C. (Eds.), Research on motivation in education (Vol. II, pp. 289–305). New York, Academic Press.Google Scholar
Dweck, C. S. (1996). ‘Capturing the dynamic nature of personality.’ Journal of Research in Personality 30: 348362.Google Scholar
Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Philadelphia, Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Dweck, C. S. (2006). Mindset: The new psychology of success. New York, Random House.Google Scholar
Dweck, C. S. and Elliott, E. S. (1983). Achievement motivation. In Mussen, P. and Hetherington, E. M. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology. Vol. IV. Social and personality development (pp. 643–691). New York, Wiley.Google Scholar
Dweck, C. S. and Leggett, E. S. (1988). ‘A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality.’ Psychological Review 95: 256273.Google Scholar
Dweck, C. S. and Molden, D. C. (2005). Self theories: Their impact on competence motivation and acquisition. In Elliot, A. J. and Dweck, C. S. (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 122140). New York and London, The Guildford Press.Google Scholar
Eco, U. (1999). Kant and the platypus: Essays on language and cognition. London, Secker & Warburg.Google Scholar
Edwards, J. A., Wichman, A. L. and Weary, G. (2009). ‘Causal uncertainty as a chronically accessible construct.’ European Journal of Social Psychology 39: 694706.Google Scholar
Ehrlich, D., Guttman, I., Schoenbach, P. and Mills, J. (1957). ‘Postdecision exposure to relevant information.’ Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 54: 98102.Google Scholar
Eisenberger, R. and Cameron, J. (1996). ‘Detrimental effects of reward: Reality or myth?American Psychologist 51(11): 11531166.Google Scholar
Eisenberger, R. and Selbst, M. (1994). ‘Does reward increase or decrease creativity?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 66: 11161127.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1972). Universals and cultural differences in facial expressions of emotion. In Cole, J. (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation 1971 (pp. 207283). Lincoln, University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1973). Darwin and facial expression: A century of research in review. New York, Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1977). Biological and cultural contributions to body and facial movement. In Blacking, J. (Ed.), A. S. A. Monograph 15, The anthropology of the body (pp. 3984). London, Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1992a). ‘Are there basic emotions.’ Psychological Review 99(3): 550553.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1992b). ‘Facial expressions of emotions: New findings, new questions.’ Psychological Science 3: 3438.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1992c). ‘An argument for basic emotions.’ Cognition and Emotion 6: 169200.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1994a). All emotions are basic. In Ekman, P. and Davidson, R. J. (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 1519). New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1994b). ‘Strong evidence for universals in facial expressions: A reply to Russell’s mistaken critique.’ Psychological Bulletin 115: 268287.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1998). Afterword: Universality of emotional expression? A personal history of the dispute. In Darwin, C. and Ekman, P. (Eds.), The expression of emotions in man and animals (pp. 363393). London, HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1999b). Facial expressions. In Dalgleish, T. and Power, M. J. (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 301320). New York, John Wiley & Sons Ltd.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. and Friesen, W. V. (1971). ‘Constants across cultures in the face and emotion.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 17: 124129.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., Sorenson, E. R. and Friesen, W. V. (1969). ‘Pan-cultural elements in facial displays of emotions.’ Science, 164: 8688.Google Scholar
Eliot, G. (1965). Middlemarch. Harmondsworth, UK, Penguin.Google Scholar
Elliot, A. J. (1994). Approach and avoidance achievement goals: An intrinsic motivation analysis. PhD dissertation, University of Wisconsin, Madison.Google Scholar
Elliot, A. J. (1997). Integrating ‘classic’ and ‘contemporary’ approaches to achievement motivation: A hierarchical model of approach and avoidance achievement motivation. In Pintrich, P. and Maehr, M. (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 10, pp. 143179). Greenwich, CT, JAI Press.Google Scholar
Elliot, A. J. (1999). ‘Approach and avoidance motivation and achievement goals.’ Educational Psychologist 34: 169189.Google Scholar
Elliot, A. J. (2005). A conceptual history of the achievement goal construct. In Elliot, A. J. and Dweck, C. S. (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 5272). New York and London, The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Elliot, A. J. (2006). ‘The hierarchical model of approach-avoidance motivation.’ Motivation and Emotion 30(2 Special Issue: Approach/Avoidance): 111116.Google Scholar
Elliot, A. J. (2008). Approach and avoidance motivation. In Elliot, A. J. (Ed.), Handbook of approach and avoidance motivation (pp. 314). New York, Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Elliot, A. J. and Church, M. (1997). ‘A hierarchical model of approach and avoidance achievement motivation.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 72: 218232.Google Scholar
Elliot, A. J. and Covington, M. V. (2001). ‘Approach and avoidance motivation.’ Educational Psychology Review 13(2): 7392.Google Scholar
Elliot, A. J. and Dweck, C. S., Eds. (2005a). Handbook of competence and motivation. New York and London, The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Elliot, A. J. and Dweck, C. S. (2005b). Competence and motivation: Competence as the core of achievement motivation. In Elliot, A. J. and Dweck, C. S. (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 312). New York and London, The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Elliot, A. J. and Harackiewicz, J. M. (1994). ‘Goal setting, achievement orientation, and intrinsic motivation: A mediational analysis.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 66: 968980.Google Scholar
Elliot, A. J. and Harackiewicz, J. M. (1996). ‘Approach and avoidance achievement goals and intrinsic motivation: A mediational analysis.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 70: 461475.Google Scholar
Elliot, A. J. and McGregor, H. A. (2001). ‘A 2 X 2 achievement goal framework.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 80(3): 501519.Google Scholar
Elliot, A. J. and Reis, H. T. (2003). ‘Attachment and adult exploration.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 85: 317331.Google Scholar
Elliott, E. S. and Dweck, C. S. (1988). ‘Goals: An approach to motivation and achievement.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54: 512.Google Scholar
Ellsworth, P. C. (1994). Some reasons to expect universal antecedents of emotion. In Ekman, P. and Davidson, R. J. (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 150154). New York and Oxford, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Emerson, R. W. (1850/1890). Representative men; seven lectures. New York and Boston, Thomas Y. Crowell.Google Scholar
Emmons, R. (1997). Motives and life goals. In Hogan, R., Johnson, J. and Briggs, S. (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 485512). San Diego, CA, Academic Press.Google Scholar
Emmons, R. A. (1986). ‘Personal strivings: An approach to personality and subjective well-being.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 51(5): 10581068.Google Scholar
Emmons, R. A. (1989). The personal striving approach to personality. In Pervin, L. A. (Ed.), Goal concepts in personality and social psychology (pp. 87126). Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Emmons, R. A. (1999a). The psychology of ultimate concerns: Motivation and spirituality in personality, New York, The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Emmons, R. A. (1999b). ‘Religion in the psychology of personality: An introduction.’ Journal of Personality 67(6): 873888.Google Scholar
Emmons, R. A. (2005). ‘Striving for the sacred: Personal goals, life meaning, and religion.’ Journal of Social Issues 61(4): 731745.Google Scholar
Emmons, R. A., Colby, P. M. and Kaiser, H. A. (1998). Personal goals and the recovery of meaning. In Wong, P. T. P. and Fry, P. S. (Eds.), The human quest for meaning: A handbook of psychological applications (pp. 163178). Mahwah, NJ and London, Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates.Google Scholar
Emmons, R. A. and Paloutzian, R. F. (2003). ‘The psychology of religion.’ Annual Review of Psychology 54: 377402.Google Scholar
Epstein, S. (1985). ‘The implications of cognitive-experiential self theory for research in social psychology and personality.’ Journal of the Theory of Social Behavior 15(3): 283310.Google Scholar
Escalona, S. K. (1940). ‘The effect of success and failure upon the level of aspiration and behavior in manic-depressive psychoses.’ University of Iowa studies in child welfare 16(199302).Google Scholar
Evans, D. (2004). The search hypothesis of emotion. In Evans, D. and Cruse, D. (Eds.), Emotion, evolution and rationality (pp. 179191). Oxford, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Feather, N. T. (1959). ‘Subjective probability and decision under uncertainty.’ Psychological Review 66: 150164.Google Scholar
Feather, N. T. (1961). ‘The relationship of persistence at a task to expectation of success and achievement related motives.’ Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 63: 552561.Google Scholar
Feather, N. T. (1962). ‘The study of persistence.’ Psychological Bulletin 59: 94115.Google Scholar
Feather, N. T. (1963). ‘Persistence at a difficult task with alternative task of intermediate difficulty.’ Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 66: 604609.Google Scholar
Feather, N. T. (1967). ‘Valence of outcome and expectation of success in relation to task difficulty and perceived locus of control.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 7: 372386.Google Scholar
Feather, N. T. (1971). ‘Organization and discrepancy in cognitive structures.’ Psychological Review 78(5): 355379.Google Scholar
Feather, N. T., Ed. (1982a). Expectations and actions. Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Feather, N. T. (1982b). Expectancy-value approaches: Present and future directions. In Feather, N. T. (Ed.), Expectations and actions (pp. 395–420). Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Feist, G. J. (1994). ‘Affective consequences of insight in art and science students.’ Cognition and Emotion 8: 489502.Google Scholar
Festinger, L. (1942). ‘A theoretical interpretation of shifts in level of aspiration.’ Psychological Review 49: 335350.Google Scholar
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA, Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Festinger, L. (1964). Conflict, decision, and dissonance. Stanford, CA, Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Festinger, L. (1987/1999). Reflections on cognitive dissonance: 30 years later. In Harmon-Jones, E. and Mills, J. (Eds.), Cognitive dissonance: Progress on a pivotal theory in social psychology (pp. 381385). Washington, DC, APA Books.Google Scholar
Festinger, L. and Carlsmith, J. M. (1959). ‘Cognitive consequences of forced compliance.’ Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 58: 203210.Google Scholar
Fisher, P. (1998). Wonder, the rainbow, and the aesthetics of rare experiences. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Fitzgerald, E. T. (1966). ‘Measurement of openness to experience: A study of regression in the service of the ego.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 4: 655663.Google Scholar
Fleming, J. and Horner, M. S. (1992). The motive to avoid success. In Smith, C. P. (Ed.), Motivation and personality: Handbook of thematic content analysis (pp. 179189). Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind: An essay on faculty psychology. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1987). Modules, frames, fridgeons, sleeping dogs, and the music of the spheres. In Phylyshyn, Z. W. (Ed.), The robot’s dilemma: The frame problem in artificial intelligence (pp. 139149). Cambridge, MA and London, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (2000). The mind doesn’t work that way: The scope and limits of computational psychology. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (2005). ‘Reply to Steven Pinker “So how does the mind work?”.’ Mind and Language 20(1): 2532.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (2006). ‘How the mind works; what we still don’t know.’ Daedalus 135: 8694.Google Scholar
Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E. and Target, M. (2002). Affect regulation, mentalization, and the development of self. New York, Other Press.Google Scholar
Ford, M. E. (1992). Motivating humans: Goals, emotions, and personal agency beliefs. Newbury Park, CA, Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Ford, M. E. and Nichols, C. W. (1987). A taxonomy of human goals and some possible applications. In Ford, M. E. and Ford, D. H. (Eds.), Humans as self-constructing systems: Putting the framework to work (pp. 289311). Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Forgas, J. P., Baumeister, R. and Tice, D. M., Eds. (2009). Psychology of self-regulation. New York and London, Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Forsyth, D. T. (1980). ‘The function of attributions.’ Social Psychology Quarterly 43: 184189.Google Scholar
Fowler, H. (1965). Curiosity and exploratory behavior. New York, Macmillan.Google Scholar
Fowler, J. W. (1981). Stages of faith: The psychology of human development and the quest for meaning. New York, HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Fox, M. D. and Raichle, M. E. (2007). ‘Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging.’ Nature Reviews Neuroscience 8: 700711.Google Scholar
Frank, J. D. (1941). ‘Recent studies of the level of aspiration.’ Psychological Bulletin 38: 218225.Google Scholar
Frankl, V. E. (1959). From death camp to existentialism. Boston, Beacon Press.Google Scholar
Frankl, V. E. (1962). Man’s search for meaning: An introduction to logotherapy: A newly revised and enlarged edition of from death-camp to existentialism. New York, Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
Frankl, V. E. and Batthyány, A. (2010). The feeling of meaninglessness: A challenge to psychotherapy and philosophy. Milwaukee, WI, Marquette University Press.Google Scholar
Frenkel-Brunswick, E. (1949). Intolerance of ambiguity as an emotional and perceptual personality variable. In Bruner, J. S. and Krech, D. (Eds.), Perception and personality: A symposium (pp. 108143). Durham, NC, Duke University Press.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1923/1962). The ego and the id. New York, Norton.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1936). The outline of psychoanalysis. London, Hogarth.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. (1994). Emotions are functional, most of the time. In Ekman, P. and Davidson, R. J. (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 112122). New York and Oxford, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge, New York and Paris, Cambridge University Press and Editions de la Maison des sciences de l’homme.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H., Manstead, A. S. R. and Bem, S. (2000). Emotions and beliefs: How feelings influence thoughts. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H., Ortony, A., Sonnemans, Clore. J. and Clore, G. L. (1992). The complexity of intensity: Issues concerning the structure of emotion intensity. In Clark, M. (Ed.), Emotion: Review of personality and social psychology (pp. 6089). Newbury Park, CA, Sage.Google Scholar
Frith, C. D. and Frith, U. (2012). ‘Mechanisms of social cognition.’ Annual Review of Psychology 63: 287313.Google Scholar
Frost, R. (1931/2007). ‘Education by poetry.’ Amherst Graduates’ Quarterly (February). In Richardson, M. (Ed.), The Collected Prose of Robert Frost (pp. 102–111). Cambridge, MA, The Belnap Press of Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Fu, G. and Lee, K. (2007). ‘Social grooming in the kindergarten: The emergence of flattery behavior.’ Developmental Science 10: 255265.Google Scholar
Gailliot, M. T., Baumeister, R. F., DeWall, C. N., Maner, J. K., Plant, E. A., Tice, D. M., Brewer, L. E.; Schmeichel, B. J (2007). ‘Self-control relies on glucose as a limited energy source: Willpower is more than a metaphor.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 92: 325336.Google Scholar
Galton, F. (1894). ‘Arithmetic by smell.’ Psychological Review 1(1): 6162.Google Scholar
Gardner, H. (1985). The mind’s new science: A history of the cognitive revolution. New York, Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gardner, J. W. (1940). ‘The use of the term “level of aspiration”.’ Psychological Review 47: 5968.Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S. (1970). The bisected brain. New York, Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S. (1995). ‘Principles of human brain organization derived from split-brain studies.’ Neuron 14: 217228.Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S. (1997). Why can’t I control my brain? Aspects of conscious experience. In Ito, M., Miyashita, Y. and Rolls, E. E. (Eds.), Cognition, computation and consciousness (pp. 6979). Oxford, New York and Tokyo, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S. (1998). The mind’s past. Berkeley, University of California Press.Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S. (2000). ‘Cerebral specialization and interhemispheric communication: Does the corpus callosum enable the human condition?Brain 123: 12931326.Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S. (2005). The ethical brain. New York, Dana Press.Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S. (2008). Human: The science behind what makes your brain unique. New York, HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S. and Ledoux, J. E. (1978). The integrated mind. New York, Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Gergen, K. J. (1992). The saturated self: Dilemmas of identity in modern life. New York, Basic Books.Google Scholar
Giddens, A. (1991). Modernity and self-identity: Self and society in the late modern age. Stanford, CA, Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Goldberg, I. I., Harel, M. and Malach, R. (2006). ‘When the brain loses its self: Prefrontal inactivation during sensorimotor processing.’ Neuron 50: 329339.Google Scholar
Goldberg, L. R. (1990). ‘An alternative “description of personality”: The Big-Five factor structure.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 59: 12161229.Google Scholar
Goldberg, L. R. (1999). A broad-bandwidth, public domain, personality inventory measuring the lower-level facets of several five-factor models. In Mervielde, I., Deary, I., De Fruyt, F. and Ostendorf, F. (Eds.), Personality psychology in Europe (Vol. 7, pp. 728). Tilburg, The Netherlands, Tilburg University Press.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). ‘Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans.’ American Psychologist 54(7): 493503.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., Fujita, K. and Oettingen, G. (2004). Planning and the implementation of goals. In Baumeister, R. F. and Vohs, K. D. (Eds.), Hanbbook of self-regulation (pp. 211–228). New York and London, Guilford.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., Wicklund, R. A. and Hilton, J. L. (1982). ‘Admission of failure and symbolic self-completion: Extending Lewinian theory.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 43(2): 358371.Google Scholar
Gomes, G. (1999). Volition and the readiness potential. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6, pp. 59–76.Google Scholar
Gonzales, M. H., Tarvis, C. and Aronson, J., Eds. (2010). The scientist and the humanist: A festschrift in honor of Elliot Aronson. New York, Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. (1991). Bully for brontosaurus: reflections in natural history. London: Hutchinson Radius.Google Scholar
Gray, J. A. (2004). Consciousness: Creeping up on the hard problem. Oxford, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gray, J. R., Schaeffer, A. and Braver, S. T. (2005). Affect and the resolution of cognitive control dilemmas. In Barrett, L. F., Niedenthal, P. M. and Winkielman, P. (Eds.), Emotion and consciousness (pp. 6794). New York and London, The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Gruber, M. J., Gelman, B. D. and Ranganath, C. (2014). “States of curiosity modulate hippocampus-dependent learning via the dopaminergic circuit.” Neuron 84: 486496.Google Scholar
Guthrie, S. E. (1993). Faces in the clouds: A new theory of religion. New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Haggard, P. (2005). ‘Conscious intention and motor cognition.’ Trends in Cognitive Science 9: 290295.Google Scholar
Haggard, P. (2008). ‘Human volition: Towards a neuroscience of will.’ Nature Reviews Neuroscience 9: 934946.Google Scholar
Haggard, P. and Eimer, M. (1999). ‘On the relation between brain potentials and the awareness of voluntary movements.’ Experimental Brain Research 126: 128133.Google Scholar
Haggard, P. and Magno, E. (1999). ‘Localizing awareness of action with transcranial magnetic stimulation.’ Experimental Brain Research 127: 102107.Google Scholar
Hagger, M. S., Wood, C., Stiff, C. and Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2010). ‘Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: A meta-analysis.’ Psychological Bulletin 136: 495525.Google Scholar
Haier, R. J., Siegel, B. V., MacLachlan, A., Soderling, E., Lottenberg, S. and Buchsbaum, M. S. (1992). ‘Regional glucose metabolic changes after learning a complex visuospatial motor task: A positron emission tomographic study.’ Brain Research 570(1–2): 134143.Google Scholar
Harackiewicz, J. M. (2004). Optimal motivation in education: The importance of students’ goals and interests. Supporting Student Wellbeing, Adelaide, South Australia, ACER.Google Scholar
Harackiewicz, J. M. and Sansone, C. (1991). Goals and intrinsic motivation: You can’t get there from here. In Maehr, M. L. and Pintrich, P. R. (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement (pp. 21–49). Greenwich, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Harackiewicz, J. M. and Sansone, C. (2000). Rewarding competence: The importance of goals in the study of intrinsic motivation. In Sansone, C. a. J. M. and Harackiewicz, J. M. (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: The search for optimal motivation and perform (pp. 79103). San Diego, CA and London, Academic Press.Google Scholar
Harlow, H. F. (1950). ‘Learning and satiation of response in intrinsically motivated complex puzzle performance by monkeys.’ Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 43: 289294.Google Scholar
Harlow, H. F. (1953a). Motivation as a factor in the acquisition of new responses. In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), Current theory and research in motivation, symposium (pp. 2449). Lincoln, University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Harlow, H. F. (1953b). ‘Mice, monkeys, men, and motives.’ Psychological Review 60: 2332.Google Scholar
Harlow, H. F. (1954). Learning theory, personality theory, and clinical research. In Kentucky symposium (Vol. 9, pp. 3653). Oxford, Wiley.Google Scholar
Harmon-Jones, E. (1999). Towards an understanding of the motivation underlying dissonance effects: Is the production of aversive consequences necessary? In Harmon-Jones, E. and Mills, J. (Eds.), Cognitive dissonance: Progress on a pivotal theory in social psychology (pp. 71–99). Washington, DC, APA Books.Google Scholar
Harmon-Jones, E., Brehm, J. W., Greenberg, J., Simon, L. and Nelson, D. E. (1996). ‘Evidence that the production of aversive consequences is not necessary to create cognitive dissonance.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 70: 516.Google Scholar
Harmon-Jones, E. and Mills, J., Eds. (1999). Cognitive dissonance: Progress on a pivotal theory in social psychology. Washington, DC, APA Books.Google Scholar
Harris, P. (2000). The work of the imagination. Oxford, Blackwell.Google Scholar
Harris, R. (2007). The happiness trap: Stop struggling, start living. Wollombi, N.S.W, Exisle Publishing.Google Scholar
Hauser, M. D. (2009). ‘The possibility of impossible cultures.’ Nature 460: 190196.Google Scholar
Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N. W. and Fitch, T. (2002). ‘The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve?Science 298: 15691579.Google Scholar
Hayes-Roth, B. (1985). ‘A blackboard architecture for control.’ Artificial Intelligence 26: 251321.Google Scholar
Heaney, S. (2006). Crediting poetry, 1995 Nobel Lecture. In Nobel lectures: from the literature laureates, 1986 to 2005. Nobelstiftelsen. Carlton, Victoria, Melbourne University Publishing.Google Scholar
Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological theory. New York, Wiley.Google Scholar
Hebb, D. O. (1955). ‘Drives and the C.N.S. (conceptual nervous system).’ Psychological Review 62: 243254.Google Scholar
Hebb, D. O. (1958). ‘The motivating effects of exteroceptive stimulation.’ American Psychologist 13: 109113.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1977). ‘Achievement motivation and its constructs: A cognitive model.’ Motivation and Emotion 1: 283329.Google Scholar
Heckhausen, H. (1991). Motivation and action. Berlin, Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Heider, F. (1944). ‘Social perception and phenomenal causality.’ Psychological Review 51: 358374.Google Scholar
Heider, F. (1946). ‘Attitudes and cognitive organization.’ Journal of Psychology 21: 107112.Google Scholar
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York, Wiley.Google Scholar
Heider, F. (1960). ‘The Gestalt theory of motivation.’ Nebraska Symposium on Motivation 8: 145172.Google Scholar
Heider, K. G. (1991). Landscapes of emotion: Mapping three cultures of emotion in Indonesia. Cambridge, New York and Paris, Cambridge University Press and Editions de la maison des sciences de l’homme.Google Scholar
Heine, S. J. and Lehman, D. R. (1997). ‘Culture, dissonance and self-affirmation.’ Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 23: 389400.Google Scholar
Heine, S. J., Proulx, T. and Vohs, K. D. (2006). ‘The meaning maintenance model: On the coherence of social motivations.’ Personality and Social Psychology Review 10: 88110.Google Scholar
Henderlong, J. and Lepper, M. R. (2002). ‘The effects of praise on children’s intrinsic motivation: A review and synthesis.’ Psychological Bulletin 128: 774795.Google Scholar
Hendrick, I. (1942). ‘Instinct and the ego during infancy.’ Psychoanalitic Quarterly 11: 3358.Google Scholar
Henle, M. (1955). ‘Some effects of motivational processes on cognition.’ Psychological Review 62: 423432.Google Scholar
Hennessey, B. A. and Amabile, T. M. (1998). ‘Reward, intrinsic motivation, and creativity.’ American Psychologist 53(6): 674675.Google Scholar
Herrmann, E., Call, J., Hernandez-Lloreda, M. V., Hare, B. and Tomasello, M. (2007). ‘Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: The cultural intelligence hypothesis.’ Science 317(September 7): 13601366.Google Scholar
Hidi, S. (2000). An interest researcher’s perspective: The effects of extrinsic and intrinsic factors on motivation. In Sansone, C. and Harackiewicz, J. M. (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: The search for optimal motivation and performance (pp. 309339). San Diego, CA and London, Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hidi, S., Ainley, M., Berndorff, D. and Del Favero, L. (2007). The role of interest and self-efficacy in science-related expository writing. In Boscolo, S. H. P. (Ed.), Motivation and interest in writing (pp. 203–217). Oxford, Elsevier.Google Scholar
Hidi, S. and Berndorff, D. (1998). Situational interest and learning. In Hoffmann, L., Krapp, A., Renninger, K. A. and Baunert, J. (Eds.), Interest and learning: Proceedings of the Seeon Conference on Interest and Gender. (pp. 7490). Kiel, Germany, IPN.Google Scholar
Hidi, S. and Harackiewicz, J. M. (2000). ‘Motivating the academically unmotivated: A critical issue for the 21st century.’ Review of Educational Research 70: 151179.Google Scholar
Higgins, E. T. (1987). ‘Self-discrepancy: A theory of relating self and affect.’ Psychological Review 94: 319340.Google Scholar
Higgins, E. T. (1997). ‘Beyond pleasure and pain.’ American Psychologist 52(12): 12801300.Google Scholar
Higgins, E. T. (1998). ‘Promotion and prevention: Regulatory focus as a motivational principle.’ Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 30: 146.Google Scholar
Higgins, E. T. (2000). ‘Social cognition: Learning about what matters in the social world.’ European Journal of Social Psychology 30: 339.Google Scholar
Higgins, E. T. (2001). Promotion and prevention experiences: Relating emotions to non-emotional motivational states. In Forgas, J. P. (Ed.), Handbook of affect and social cognition (pp. 186211). Mahwah, NJ, Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Higgins, E. T., Klein, R. and Strauman, T. (1985). ‘Self-concept discrepancy theory: A psychological model for distinguishing among different aspects of depression and anxiety.’ Social Cognition 3: 5176.Google Scholar
Higgins, E. T., Roney, C., Crowe, E. and Hymes, C. (1994). ‘Ideal versus ought predilections for approach and avoidance: Distinct self-regulatory systems.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 66: 276286.Google Scholar
Higgins, E. T., Shah, J. and Friedman, R. (1997). ‘Emotional responses to goal attainment: Strength of regulatory focus as moderator.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 72: 515525.Google Scholar
Holmes, R. (2008). The age of wonder: How the romantic generation discovered the beauty and terror of science. London, Harper Press.Google Scholar
Hoppe, F. (1930). ‘Erfolg und Misserfolg (Untersuchaungen zur Handlungs – und affekpsychologie IV).’ Psychologiche Forschung 14: 162.Google Scholar
Horner, M. S. (1968). Sex differences in achievement motivation and performance in competitive and noncompetitive situations. PhD dissertation, Psychology. University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan.Google Scholar
Horner, M. S. (1971). The psychological significance of success in competitive achievement situations: A threat as well as a promise. In Day, H. I., Berlyne, D. E. and Hunt, D. E. (Eds.), Intrinsic motivation: A new direction in education (pp. 4660). Toronto, Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Horner, M. S. (1972). ‘Towards an understanding of achievement-related conflicts in women.’ Journal of Social Issues 28: 157175.Google Scholar
Horner, M. S. (1974). The measurement and behavioral implications of fear of success in women. In Atkinson, J. W. and Raynor, J. (Eds.), Motivation and achievement (pp. 91120). Washington, DC, V. W. Winston.Google Scholar
Horner, M. S. and Fleming, J. (1992). A revised scoring manual for the motive to avoid success. In Smith, C. P. (Ed.), Motivation and personality: Handbook of thematic content analysis (pp. 190204). Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hrdy, S. B. (2009). Mothers and others: The evolutionary origins of mutual understanding. Cambridge, MA, The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1943a). Principles of behavior: An introduction to behavior theory. New York and London, D. Appleton-Century Company, Inc.Google Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1943b). ‘The problem of intervening variables in molar behavior theory.’ Psychological Review 50(3): 273291.Google Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1951/1964). A behavior system: An introduction to behavior theory concerning the individual organism. New York, Wiley.Google Scholar
Hull, R., Martin, R. C., Beier, M. E., Lane, D. and Hamilton, A. C. (2008). ‘Executive function in older adults: A structural equation modeling approach.’ Neuropsychology 22(4): 508522.Google Scholar
Hume, D. (1997). Of the standard of taste. In Cooper, D. (Ed.), Aesthetics: The classical readings (pp. 76–94). Oxford, Blackwell.Google Scholar
Hunt, J. M. (1960). ‘Experience and the development of motivation: Some reinterpretations.’ Child Development 31: 489504.Google Scholar
Hunt, J. M. (1963). Motivation inherent in information processing and action. In Harvey, O. J. (Ed.), Motivation and social interaction (pp. 3594). New York, Ronald Press.Google Scholar
Hunt, J. M. (1965). ‘Intrinsic motivation and its role in psychological development.’ Nebraska Symposium on Motivation 13: 189282.Google Scholar
Hunt, J. M. (1971). Towards a history of intrinsic motivation. In Day, H. I., Berlyne, D. E. and Hunt, D. E. (Eds.), Intrinsic motivation: A new direction in education (pp. 132). Toronto, Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E. (1971). The face of emotion. New York, Meredity.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E. (1977). Human emotions. New York, Plenum.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E. (1993). ‘Four systems for emotion activation: Cognitive and non-cognitive processes.’ Psychological Review 100: 6990.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E. (2000). ‘Reinforcement, emotion, and consciousness.’ Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23(2): 202204.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E. (2007). ‘Basic emotions, natural kinds, emotion schemas, and a new paradigm.’ Perspectives on Psychological Science 2(3): 260280.Google Scholar
Jack, A. I. and Shallice, T. (2001). ‘Introspective physicalism as an approach to the science of consciousness.’ Cognition 79(1–2): 161196.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. (2008). Language, consciousness, culture: Essays on mental structure. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jackson, H. (1976). An assessment of long term effects of personal causation training. St. Louis, MO, Washington University Press.Google Scholar
Jaki, S. L. (1986). Science and creation. Edinburgh, Scottish Academic Press.Google Scholar
James, W. (1884). ‘What is an emotion?Mind 4: 188204.Google Scholar
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York, Holt.Google Scholar
James, W. (1902). The varieties of religious experience. London, Longmans, Green.Google Scholar
Johnson, P. E. (1959). Psychology of religion. Nashville, TN, Abingdon.Google Scholar
Johnson, S. (1751/1969). The Yale edition of the works of Samuel Johnson Vol V. New Haven, CT, Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. and Oatley, K. (1989). ‘The language of emotions: An analysis of a semantic field.’ Cognition and Emotion 3: 81123.Google Scholar
Jones, D. M. (1993). Objects, streams and threads of auditory attention. In Baddeley, A. D. and Weiskrantz, L. (Eds.), Attention: Selection, awareness and control (pp. 87104). Oxford, Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Jones, D. M., Hughes, R. W. and Macken, W. J. (2006). ‘Perceptual organization masquerading as phonological storage: Further support for a perceptual-gestural view of short-term memory.’ Journal of Memory and Language 54: 265281.Google Scholar
Jones, D. M., Macken, W. J. and Nicholls, A. P. (2004). ‘The phonological store of working memory: Is it phonological and is it a store?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 30(3): 656674.Google Scholar
Jonides, J. (1998). ‘The role of parietal cortex in verbal working memory.’ Journal of Neuroscience 18(13): 50265034.Google Scholar
Joske, W. D. (1981). Philosophy and the meaning of life. In Klemke, E. D. (Eds.), The meaning of life (pp. 248261). New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Judd, C. H. (1908). ‘The relation of special training to general intelligence.’ Educational Review 36: 2842.Google Scholar
Jung, C. G. (1951/1959). Collected works. New York, Pantheon.Google Scholar
Kagan, J. (1972). ‘Motives and development.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 22(1): 5166.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. and Frederick, S. (2002). Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. In Gilovich, D. G. T and Kahneman, D (Eds.), Heuristics and biases (pp. 4981). New York, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kaiser, C. B. (1991). Creation and the history of science. Grand Rapids, MI, Eerdmans.Google Scholar
Kang, M. J., Hsu, M., Krajbich, I. M., Loewenstein, G., McClure, S. M., Wang, J. T. et al. (2009). ‘The wick in the candle of learning: Epistemic curiosity activates reward circuitry and enhances memory.’ Psychological Science 20(8): 963973.Google Scholar
Karoly, P. (1993). ‘Goal systems: An organizational framework for clinical assessment and treatment planning.’ Psychological Assessment 3: 273280.Google Scholar
Kashdan, T. (2009). Curious? Discover the missing ingredient to a fulfilling life. New York, William Morrow; Harper.Google Scholar
Kashdan, T. and Ciarrochi, J., Eds. (2013). Mindfulness, acceptance, and positive psychology: The seven foundations of well-being. Oakland, CA, Context Press.Google Scholar
Kashdan, T. B. (2004). Curiosity. In Peterson, C. and Seligman, M. (Eds.), Character strengths and virtues (pp. 125141). Washington and New York, American Psychological Association and Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kashdan, T. B. and Breen, W. E. (2007). ‘Materialism and diminished well-being: Experiential avoidance as a mediating mechanism.’ Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 26: 521539.Google Scholar
Kashdan, T. B., Dewal, C. N., Pond, R. S., Silvia, P. J., Lambert, N. M, Fincham, F. D., et al. (2013a). ‘Curiosity protects against interpersonal aggression: Cross-sectional, daily process, and behavioral evidence.’ Journal of Personality 81: 87102.Google Scholar
Kashdan, T. B., Gallagher, M. W., Silvia, P. J., Winterstein, B. P., Breen, W. E., Terhar, D., et al. (2009). ‘The Curiosity and Exploration Inventory-II: Development, factor structure, and initial psychometrics.’ Journal of Research in Personality 43: 987998.Google Scholar
Kashdan, T. B., McKnight, P. E., Fincham, P. P. and Rose, P. (2011). ‘When curiosity breeds intimacy: Taking advantage of intimacy opportunities and transforming boring conversations.’ Journal of Personality 79: 13691401.Google Scholar
Kashdan, T. B., Rose, P. and Fincham, F. D. (2004). ‘Curiosity and exploration: Facilitating positive subjective experiences and personal growth opportunities.’ Journal of Personality Assessment 82(3): 291305.Google Scholar
Kashdan, T. B., Sherman, R. A., Yarbro, J. and Funder, D. C. (2013b). ‘How are curious people viewed and how do they behave in social situations? From the perspectives of self, friends, parents, and unacquainted observers.’ Journal of Personality 81: 142154.Google Scholar
Kashdan, T. B. and Silvia, P. J. (2009). Curiosity and interest: The benefits of thriving on novelty and challenge. In Lopez, S. J. S. (Ed.), Oxford handbook of positive psychology (2nd ed., pp. 367374). New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kashdan, T. B. and Steger, M. F. (2011). Challenges, pitfalls, and aspirations for positive psychology. In Sheldon, K. M., Kashdan, T. B. and Steger, M. F. (Eds.), Designing positive psychology (pp. 921). Oxford and New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kashdan, T. B., Young, K. C. and Machell, K. A. (2015). ‘Positive emotion regulation: Addressing two myths.’ Current Opinion in Psychology 3: 117121.Google Scholar
Kashdan, T. B. and Yuen, M. (2007). ‘Whether highly curious students thrive academically depends on the learning environment of their school: A study of Hong Kong adolescents.’ Motivation and Emotion 31: 260270.Google Scholar
Kasser, T. (2002). The high price of materialism. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kasser, T. and Ryan, R. M. (1993). ‘A dark side of the American dream: Correlates of financial success as a central life aspiration.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 65: 410422.Google Scholar
Kasser, T. and Ryan, R. M. (1996). ‘Further examining the American dream: Differential correlates of intrinsic and extrinsic goals.’ Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 22: 280287.Google Scholar
Katz, L. D. (2000). ‘Emotion, representation, and consciousness.’ Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23(2): 204205.Google Scholar
Kauffman, S. A. (1993). The origins of order: Self-organization and selection in evolution. New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kauffman, S. A. (2008). Reinventing the sacred: A new view of science, reason and religion. New York, Basic Books.Google Scholar
Kay, A., Laurin, C., K., Fitzsimons, G. M. and Landau, M. J. (2014). ‘A functional basis for structure-seeking: Exposure to structure promotes willingness to engage in motivated action.’ Journal of Experimental Psychology 143(2): 486491.Google Scholar
Kelly, G. (1955). The psychology of personal constructs. New York, Norton.Google Scholar
Kelly, H. H. (1967). Attribution theory in social psychology. In Levine, D. E. (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 15, pp. 192240). Lincoln, University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Kelly, H. H. (1971). Attributions in social interactions. Morristown, NJ, General Learning Press.Google Scholar
Kelly, H. H. (1973). ‘The processes of causal attribution.’ American Psychologist 28: 107128.Google Scholar
Keyes, C. L. M. and Haidt, J., Eds. (2003). Flourishing: Positive psychology and the life well-lived. Washington, DC, American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Kidd, C. and Hayden, B. Y. (2015). ‘The psychology and neuroscience of curiosity.’ Neuron 88(3): 449460.Google Scholar
Kierkegaard, S. (1986). Either/or. New York, Perennial Library.Google Scholar
Kiesler, C. A. (1971). The psychology of commitment. New York, Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kimble, G. A., Wertheimer, M. and White, C. L. (1991). Portraits of Pioneers in Psychology. Washington, DC, American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
King, L. A., Hicks, J. A., Krull, J. and Baker, A. G. (2006). ‘Positive affect and the experience of meaning in life.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 90: 179196.Google Scholar
Kirsch, A. (2014). ‘Extracting the Woodchuck: Robert Frost’s “doubleness,” revealed in his letters – and poems.’ Harvard Magazine, 116(3 January–February), 44–48.Google Scholar
Kjaer, T. W., Nowak, M., Kjaer, K. W., Lou, A. R. and Lou, H. C. (2001). ‘Precuneus prefrontal activity during awareness of visual verbal stimuli.’ Consciousness and Cognition 10(3): 356365.Google Scholar
Klauer, K. C. and Zhao, Z. M. (2004). ‘Double dissociations in visual and spatial short-term memory.’ Journal of Educational Psychology – General 133(3): 355381.Google Scholar
Klinger, E. (1977). Meaning and void: Inner experience and the incentives in people’s lives. Minneapolis, University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Klinger, E. (1998). The search for meaning in evolutionary perspective and its clinical implications. In Wong, P. T. P. and Fry, P. S. (Eds.), The human quest for meaning: A handbook of psychological applications (pp. 2750). Mahwah, NJ and London, Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates.Google Scholar
Koch, C. (2004). The quest for consciousness: A neurobiological approach. Denver, CO, Roberts and Co.Google Scholar
Koch, C. (2012). Consciousness: Confessions of a romantic reductionist. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Koch, C. and Tsuchiya, N. (2007). ‘Attention and consciousness: Two distinct brain processes.’ Trends in Cognitive Science 11: 1622.Google Scholar
Koffka, K. (1935). Principles of gestalt psychology. London Routledge & K. Paul.Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1920/1924). Die physischen Gestalten in Ruhe und im stanionaren Zustand. Erlangen, Germany, Verlag der Philosophischen Akademie.Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1929/1947). Gestalt psychology: An introduction to new concepts in modern psychology. New York, New American Library.Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1938). The place of value in a world of facts. New York, Liveright.Google Scholar
Kolb, B. and Whishaw, I. Q. (1998). ‘Brain plasticity and behavior.’ Annual Review of Psychology 49: 4364.Google Scholar
Kornhuber, H. H. and Deecke, L. (1965). ‘Hirnpotentialanderungen bei Wilkurbewegungen und passiven Bewegungen des Menschen: Bereitschaftspotential und reafferente Potentiale.’ Pflügers Archiv: European Journal of Physiology 284: 117.Google Scholar
Korotkov, D. (1998). The sense of coherence: Making sense out of chaos. In Wong, P. T. P. and Fry, P. S. (Eds.), The human quest for meaning: A handbook of psychological research and clinical applications (pp. 51–70). Mahwah, NJ and London, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Korsgaard, C. (1983). The normativity of instrumental reasoning. In Cullity, G. and Gaut, B. (Eds.), Ethics and practical reason (pp. 215254). Oxford, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Krapp, A., Hidi, S. and Renninger, K. A. (1992). Interest, learning and development. In Renninger, K. A., Hidi, S. and Krapp, A. (Eds.), The role of interest in learning and development (pp. 325). Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kross, E. and Mischel, W. (2010). From stimulus control to self-control: Towards an integrative understanding of the processes underlying willpower. In Hassin, R., Ochsner, K. and Trope, Y. (Eds.), Self control in society, mind, and brain (pp. 428446). New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kruglanski, A. W. (1989). Lay epistemics and human knowledge: Cognitive and motivational bases. New York, Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Kruglanski, A. W. (2004). The psychology of closed mindedness. New York, Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Kruglanski, A. W., Friedman, I. and Zeevi, G. (1971). ‘The effects of extrinsic incentives on some qualitative aspects of task performance.’ Journal of Personality 39: 606617.Google Scholar
Kruglanski, A. W. and Webster, D. M. (1996). ‘Motivated closing of the mind: “Seizing” and “freezing”’ Psychological Review 103: 263283.Google Scholar
Lane, R. D. and Nadel, L., Eds. (2000). Cognitive neuroscience of emotion. Series in affective science. New York and Oxford, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Langer, E. J. (1975). ‘The illusion of control.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 32: 311328.Google Scholar
Langevin, R. (1971). ‘Is curiosity a unitary construct?Canadian Journal of Psychology 25: 361374.Google Scholar
Lashley, K., Chow, K. and Semmes, J. (1951). ‘An examination of the electric field theory of cerebral integration.’ Psychological Review 58: 123136.Google Scholar
Laurin, K., Kay, A. and Moscovitch, D. A. (2008). ‘On the belief in God: Towards an understanding of the emotional substrates of compensatory control.’ Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 44: 15591562.Google Scholar
Lauriola, M., Litman, J. A., Mussel, P., De Santis, R., Crowson, H. M. and Hoffman, R. R. (2015). ‘Epistemic curiosity and self-regulation.’ Personality and Individual Differences 83: 202207.Google Scholar
Lawrence, A. D. and Calder, A. J. (2004). Homologizing human emotions. In Evans, D. and Cruse, P. (Eds.), Emotion, evolution and rationality (pp. 1547). Oxford, Oxford University Pres.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R. (1984). ‘On the primacy of cognition.’ American Psychologist 39: 124126.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R. S. (1974). Cognition and coping processes in emotion. In Weiner, B. (Ed.), Cognitive views of human motivation (pp. 2132). New York, Academic Press.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R. S. (1994a). Universal antecedents of the emotions. In Ekman, P. and Davidson, R. J. (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 163171). New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R. S. (1994b). The stable and the unstable in emotion. In Ekman, P. and Davidson, R. J. (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 7985). New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R. S. (1999). The cognition-emotion debate: A bit of history. In Dalgleish, T. and Power, M. J. (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and motivation (pp. 319). Chichester and New York, Wiley.Google Scholar
LeDoux, J. E., Ed. (1996). The emotional brain. New York, Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
LeDoux, J. E. (2000). ‘Emotion circuits in the brain.’ Annual Review of Neuroscience 23: 155184.Google Scholar
Lepper, M. R. (1998). ‘A whole much less than the sum of its parts.’ American Psychologist 53(6): 675676.Google Scholar
Lepper, M. R., Greene, D. and Nisbett, R. E. (1973). ‘Undermining children’s intrinsic interest with extrinsic rewards: A test of the “overjustification” hypothesis.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 28: 129137.Google Scholar
Leslie, A. M. (2005). ‘Developmental parallels in understanding minds and bodies.’ Trends in Cognitive Science 9: 459462.Google Scholar
Lewin, K. (1926). ‘Vorsatz, Wille, und Bedürfnis.’ Psychologische Forschung (Psychological Research) 7: 330385.Google Scholar
Lewin, K. (1935). A dynamic theory of personality. New York and London, McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Lewin, K. (1938). The conceptual representation and the measurement of psychological forces. Durham, NC, Duke University Press.Google Scholar
Lewin, K. (1951). Field theory in social science. New York, Harper.Google Scholar
Lewin, K., Dembo, T., Festinger, L. and Sears, P. S. (1944). Level of aspiration. In McV Hunt, J. (Ed.), Personality and the behavior disorders (pp. 333378). New York, The Ronald Press.Google Scholar
Lewin, K., Heider, F. and Heider, G. M. (1936). Principles of topological psychology. New York and London, McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Libet, B. (1965). ‘Cortical activation in conscious and unconscious experience.’ Perspectives in Biology and Medicine 9: 7786.Google Scholar
Libet, B. (1985). ‘Unconscious cerebral initiative and the role of conscious will in voluntary action.’ Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8: 529566.Google Scholar
Libet, B. (1999). ‘Do we have free will?Journal of Consciousness Studies 6(8–9): 4757.Google Scholar
Libet, B. (2004). Mind time: The temporal factor in consciousness. Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Libet, B., Gleason, C. A., Wright, T. and Pearl, D. K. (1983). ‘Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential): The unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act.’ Brain 106: 623642.Google Scholar
Libet, B., Wright, E. W. J., Feinstein, B. and Pearl, D. K. (1979). ‘Subjective referral of the timing for a conscious sensory experience: A functional role for the somatosensory specific projection system in man.’ Brain 102: 193224.Google Scholar
Libet, B., Wright, E. W. Jr. and Gleason, C. A. (1982). ‘Readiness-potentials preceding unrestricted “spontaneous” vs pre-planned voluntary acts.’ Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 54: 322335.Google Scholar
Litman, J. A. (2005). ‘Curiosity and the pleasure of learning: Wanting and liking new information.’ Cognition and Emotion 19(6): 793814.Google Scholar
Litman, J. A. (2008). ‘Interest and deprivation factors of epistemic curiosity.’ Personality and Individual Differences 44: 15851595.Google Scholar
Litman, J. A. (2010). ‘Relationships between measures of I- and D-type curiosity, ambiguity tolerance, and need for closure: An initial test of the wanting-liking model of information seeking.’ Personality and Individual Differences 48: 397402.Google Scholar
Litman, J. A., Collins, R. P. and Spielberger, C. D. (2005). ‘The nature and measurement of sensory curiosity.’ Personality and Individual Differences 39: 11231133.Google Scholar
Litman, J. A., Hutchins, T. L. and Russon, R. K. (2005). ‘Epistemic curiosity, feeling-of-knowing, and exploratory behavior.’ Cognition and Emotion 19(4): 559582.Google Scholar
Litman, J. A. and Jimerson, T. L. (2004). ‘The measurement of curiosity as a feeling of deprivation.’ Journal of Personality Assessment 82(2): 147157.Google Scholar
Litman, J. A. and Mussel, P. (2013). ‘Validity of the interest-and deprivation-type epistemic curiosity model in Germany.’ Journal of Individual Differences 34(2): 5968.Google Scholar
Litman, J. A. and Silvia, P. J. (2006). ‘The latent structure of trait curiosity: Evidence of interest and deprivation curiosity dimensions.’ Journal of Personality Assessment 86(3): 318328.Google Scholar
Litman, J. A. and Spielberger, C. D. (2003). ‘Measuring epistemic curiosity and its diversive and specific components.’ Journal of Personality Assessment 80: 7586.Google Scholar
Litwin, G. H. and Stringer, R. A. (1968). Motivation and organizational climate. Boston, Graduate School of Business Administration, Harvard University.Google Scholar
Locke, E. A. and Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting & task performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ, Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Loewenstein, G. (1994). ‘The psychology of curiosity: A review and reinterpretation.’ Psychological Bulletin 116(1): 7598.Google Scholar
Loewenstein, G., Adler, D., Behrens, D. and Gillis, J. (1992). Why Pandora opened the box: Cuiosity as a desire for missing information. Pittsburgh, PA, Department of Social and Decision Sciences, Carnegie Mellon University.Google Scholar
MacDonald, A. P. (1970). ‘Revised scale for ambiguity tolerance: Reliability and validity.’ Psychological Reports 26: 791798.Google Scholar
Mach, E. (1886). Beiträge zur Analyse der Empfindungen. Jena, Fischer.Google Scholar
Mach, E. (1897). Contributions to the analysis of sensations, with new additions by the author. Translated by C. M. Williams. Chicago, The Open Court Publishing Company.Google Scholar
MacIntyre, A. C. (1984). After virtue: A study in moral theory. South Bend, IN, University of Notre Dame Press.Google Scholar
Maddi, S. R. (1970). The search for meaning. In M. Page (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (pp. 137186). Lincoln, University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Maddi, S. R. (1971). Comments on Nunnally’s and Suchman’s papers. In Day, H. I., Berlyne, D. E. and Hunt, D. E. E. (Eds.), Intrinsic motivation: A new direction in education (pp. 83–90). Toronto, Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Maddi, S. R. (1998). Creating meaning through making decisions. In Wong, P. T. P. and Fry, P. S. (Eds.), The human quest for meaning: A handbook of psychological applications (pp. 326). Mahwah, NJ and London, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Maehr, M. (1974). ‘Culture and achievement motivation.’ American Psychologist 29: 887896.Google Scholar
Maehr, M. L. (1983). On doing well in science: Why Jonny no longer excels; why Sarah never did. In Paris, S. G., Olson, G. M. and Stevenson, H. W. (Eds.), Learning and motivation in the classroom (pp. 179210). Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Maehr, M. L. (1989). Thoughts about motivation. In Ames, C. and Ames, R. (Eds.), Research on motivation in education, Vol. 3: Goals and cognitions (pp. 299315). New York, Academic.Google Scholar
Maehr, M. L. and Meyer, H. A. (1997). ‘Understanding motivation and schooling: Where we’ve been, where we are, and where we need to go.’ Educational Psychology Review 9: 371409.Google Scholar
Maehr, M. L. and Nicholls, A. P. (1980). Culture and achievement motivation: A second look. In Warren, N. (Ed.), Studies in cross-cultural psychology. (Vol. 2, pp. 221267). New York, Academic.Google Scholar
Maia, T. V. and McClelland, J. L. (2004). ‘A reexamination of the evidence for the somatic marker hypothesis: What participants really know in the Iowa gambling task.’ Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 101(45): 1607516080.Google Scholar
Manczak, E., Zapata-Gietl, C. and McAdams, D. P. (2014). ‘Regulatory focus in the life story: Prevention and promotion as expressed in three layers of personality.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 106: 169181.Google Scholar
Mandler, G. (1975). Mind and emotion. New York, Wiley.Google Scholar
Mandler, G. (1984). Mind and body. New York, Norton.Google Scholar
Mandler, G. (1985). Cognitive psychology: An essay in cognitive science. Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Mandler, G. (1997). Human nature explored. New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mandler, G. (2002). Consciousness recovered: Psychological functions and origins of conscious thought. Amsterdam and Philadelphia, John Benjamins Pub.Google Scholar
Mandler, G. (2004). ‘Free will for everyone – with flaws.’ Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27: 669.Google Scholar
Mandler, G. (2007). A history of modern experimental psychology: From James and Wundt to cognitive science. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mandler, G. and Cowen, J. E. (1958). ‘Test anxiety questionnaires.’ Journal of Consulting Psychology 22: 228229.Google Scholar
Mandler, G. and Sarason, S. B. (1952). ‘A study of anxiety and learning.’ Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 47: 166173.Google Scholar
Mandler, J. M. and Mandler, G. (1964). Thinking: From association to gestalt. New York, Wiley.Google Scholar
Maslow, A. H. (1954). ‘The instinctoid nature of basic needs.’ Journal of Personality 22(3): 326347.Google Scholar
Maslow, A. H. (1954). Motivation and personality. New York, Harper.Google Scholar
Maw, W. H. (1971). Difference in the personality of children differing in curiosity. In Day, H. I., Berlyne, D. E. and Hunt, D. E. (Eds.), Intrinsic motivation: A new direction in education (pp. 91–98). Toronto, Holt Rinehart and Winston of Canada.Google Scholar
Maw, W. H. and Maw, E. W. (1964). Differences between children with high and low curiosity: A summary. Moravia, NY, Chronicle Guidance Publications.Google Scholar
Maw, W. H. and Maw, E. W. (1968). ‘Self appraisal of curiosity.’ The Journal of Educational Research 61: 462465.Google Scholar
McAdams, D. P. (1985). Power, intimacy, and the life story: Personological inquiries into identity. Homewood, IL, Dorsey Press.Google Scholar
McAdams, D. P. (1990). The person: An introduction to personality psychology. New York, Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
McAdams, D. P. (1995). ‘What do we know when we know a person?Journal of Personality 63: 365396.Google Scholar
McAdams, D. P. (1996a). ‘Personality, modernity, and the storied self: A contemporary framework for studying persons.’ Psychological Inquiry 7(4): 295321.Google Scholar
McAdams, D. P. (1996b). The stories we live by: Personal myths and the making of the self. New York, Guilford Press.Google Scholar
McAdams, D. P. (2000). The person: An integrated introduction to personality psychology. Fort Worth, TX, Harcourt College Publishers.Google Scholar
McAdams, D. P. (2006). The redemptive self: Stories Americans live by. Oxford and New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
McAdams, D. P. (2008). Personal narratives and the life story. In John, O. P., Robins, R. W. and Pervin, L. A. (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (3rd ed., pp. 241261). New York, Guilford.Google Scholar
McAdams, D. P. (2015). The art and science of personality development. New York, Guilford Press.Google Scholar
McAdams, D. P. and Guo, J. (2015). ‘Narrating the generative life.’ Psychological Science 26: 475483.Google Scholar
McAdams, D. P. and Olson, B. D. (2010). ‘Personality development: Continuity and change over the life course.’ Annual Review of Psychology 61: 517542.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1958a). Methods of measuring human motivation. In Atkinson, J. W. (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action, and society: A method of assessment and study (pp. 742). Princeton, NJ, Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1958b). Risk-taking in children with high and low need for achievement. In Atkinson, J. W. (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action, and society: A method of assessment and study (pp. 306–321). Princeton, NJ, Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1958c). The importance of early learning in the formation of motives. In Atkinson, J. W. (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action, and society: A method of assessment and study (pp. 437–452). Princeton, NJ, Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1961). The achieving society. Princeton, NJ, Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1965a). ‘Toward a theory of motive acquisition.’ American Psychologist 20: 321333.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1965b). “Achievement and entrepreneurship: A longitudinal study.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 1: 389–392.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1976). The achieving society: With a new introduction. New York, Irvington Publishers: distributed by Halsted Press.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1978). ‘Managing motivation to expand human freedom.’ American Psychologist 33: 201210.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1984). Motives, personality, and society: Selected papers. New York, Praeger.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1985). Human motivation. Glenview, IL, Scott, Foresman.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. (1989). ‘Motivational factors in health and disease.’ American Psychologist 44: 675683.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C., Atkinson, J. W., Clark, R. A. and Lowell, E. L. (1953). The achievement motive. New York, Appleton Century Crofts.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. and Boyatzis, R. E. (1982). ‘The leadership motive pattern and long-term success in management.’ Journal of Applied Psychology 67: 737743.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C., Clark, R. A., Roby, T. B. and Atkinson, J. W. (1949). ‘The projective expression of needs: IV. The effect of the need for achievement on thematic apperception.’ Journal of Experimental Psychology 39: 242255.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C., Koestner, R. and Weinberger, J. (1989). ‘How do self-attributed and implicit motives differ?Psychological Review 96: 690702.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C., Rhindlisbacher, S. and deCharms, R. (1955). Religious and other sources of parental attitudes towards independence training. In McClelland, D. C. (Ed.), Studies in motivation (pp. 389–397). New York, Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
McClelland, D. C. and Winter, D. G. (1969). Motivating economic achievement. New York, The Free Press.Google Scholar
McClure, S. M., Daw, N. D. and Montague, P. R. (2003). ‘A computational substrate for incentive salience.’ Trends in Neurosciences 26: 423428.Google Scholar
McCullough, M. E., Hoyt, W. T., Larson, D. B., Koenig, H. G. and Thoresen, C. E. (2000). ‘Religious involvement and mortality: A meta-analytic review.’ Health Psychology 19: 211222.Google Scholar
McCullough, M. E. and Willoughby, B. L. B. (2009). ‘Religion, self-regulation, and self-control: Associations, explanations, and implications.’ Psychological Bulletin 135: 6993.Google Scholar
McDougall, W. (1908). An introduction to social psychology. London, Methuen.Google Scholar
McDougall, W. (1920). ‘Motives in the light of recent discussion.’ Mind 29: 277293.Google Scholar
McDougall, W. (1923). Outline of psychology. New York, Scribner.Google Scholar
McDougall, W. (1945). An introduction to social psychology. London, Methuen.Google Scholar
McGeogh, J. A. (1932). ‘Forgetting and the law of disuse.’ Psychological Review 39: 352370.Google Scholar
McGeogh, J. A. and Irion, A. L. (1952). The psychology of human learning. New York, Longmans.Google Scholar
McLean, K. C., Pasupathi, M. and Pals, J. L. (2007). ‘Selves creating stories creating selves: A process model of selfdevelopment.’ Personality and Social Psychology Review 11: 262278.Google Scholar
McReynolds, P. (1962). ‘Exploratory behavior: A theoretical interpretation.’ Psychological Reports 11(2): 311318.Google Scholar
McReynolds, P. (1971). The three faces of cognitive motivation. In Day, H. I., Berlyne, D. E. and Hunt, D. E. E. (Eds.), Intrinsic motivation: A new direction in education (pp. 3345). Toronto, Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
McReynolds, P. (1987). Self-theory, anxiety and intrapsychic conflicts. In Cheshire, N. and Thomas, H. (Eds.), Self, symptoms and psychotherapy (pp. 197223). New York, Wiley.Google Scholar
Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self & society from the standpoint of a social behaviorist. Ed. by Morris, Charles W.. Chicago, University of Chicago press.Google Scholar
Mead, M. (1975). ‘Review of Darwin and Facial Expression.’ Journal of Communication 25: 210.Google Scholar
Mele, A. R. (2010). Conscious deciding and the science of free will. In Baumeister, R. F., Mele, A. R. and Vohs, K. D. (Eds.), Free will and consciousness: How might they work? (pp. 4365). New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Metcalfe, J. (2008). Evolution of metacognition. In Dunlosky, J. and Bjork, R. (Eds.), Handbook of metamemory and memory (pp. 2946). New York, Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Miller, G. A. (1956). ‘The magic number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information.’ Psychological Review 63: 8197.Google Scholar
Miller, G. A. (1962). Psychology: The science of mental life. London, Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Miller, G. A., Galanter, E. and Pribram, K. (1960). Plans and the structure of behavior. New York, Holt.Google Scholar
Miller, N. E. (1951). Learnable drives and rewards. In Stevens, S. S. (Ed.), Handbook of experimental psychology (pp. 436–472). New York, Wiley.Google Scholar
Miller, N. E. and Dollard, J. (1941). Social learning and imitation. New Haven, CT, Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Mills, J. (1999). Improving the 1957 version of dissonance theory. In Harmon-Jones, E. and Mills, J. (Eds.), Cognitive dissonance: Progress on a pivotal theory in social psychology (pp. 2542). Washington, DC, APA Books.Google Scholar
Mischel, W. (1968). Personality and assessment. New York, Wiley.Google Scholar
Mischel, W. (1973). ‘Towards a cognitive social learning reconceptualization of personality.’ Psychological Review 80: 252283.Google Scholar
Mischel, W. (1974). Cognitive appraisals and transformations in self-control. In Weiner, B. (Ed.), Cognitive views of human motivation (pp. 3349). New York, Academic Press.Google Scholar
Mischel, W. (1996). From good intentions to willpower. In Gollwitzer, P. M. and Bargh, J. A. (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 197218). New York, Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Mischel, W. (2004). ‘Toward an integrative science of the person (prefatory chapter).’ Annual Review of Psychology 55: 122.Google Scholar
Mischel, W. (2009). ‘From Personality and Assessment (1968) to personality science, 2009.’ Journal of Research in Personality (Special Issue: Personality and Assessment 40 years later) 43: 282290.Google Scholar
Mischel, W. (2012). Self-control theory. In van Lange, P. A. M., Kruglanski, A. and Higgins, E. T. (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 122). Washington, DC, Sage.Google Scholar
Mischel, W. and Ayduk, O. (2002). ‘Self-regulation in a cognitive-affective personality system: Attentional control in the service of the self.’ Self and Identity 1: 113120.Google Scholar
Mischel, W. and Ayduk, O. (2004). Willpower in a cognitive-affective processing system: The dynamics of delay of gratification. In Baumeister, R. F. and Vohs, K. D. (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, Theory, and Applications (pp. 99129). New York, Guilford.Google Scholar
Mischel, W. and Ayduk, O. (2010). Willpower in a cognitive-affective processing system: The dynamics of delay of gratification. In Vohs, K. D. and Baumeister, R. F. (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (2nd ed., pp. 83105). New York, Guilford.Google Scholar
Mischel, W. and Metzner, R. (1962). ‘Preference for delayed reward as a function of age, intelligence, and length of delay interval.’ Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 64: 425431.Google Scholar
Mischel, W. and Shoda, Y. (1995). ‘A cognitive-affective system theory of personality: Reconceptualizing situations, dispositions, dynamics, and invariance in personality structure.’ Psychological Review 102: 246268.Google Scholar
Mischel, W. and Shoda, Y. (2008). Toward a unified theory of personality: Integrating dispositions and processing dynamics within the Cognitive-Affective Processing System (CAPS). In John, O. P., Robins, R. W. and Pervin, L. A. (Eds.), Handbook of personality (3rd ed., pp. 208241). New York, Guilford.Google Scholar
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A. and Wager, T. D. (2000). ‘The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis.’ Cognitive Psychology 41: 49100.Google Scholar
Miyake, A. and Shah, P. (1999a). Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Moller, A. C., Deci, E. L. and Ryan, R. M. (2006). ‘Choice and ego-depletion: The moderating role of autonomy.’ Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 32: 10241036.Google Scholar
Montgomery, K. C. (1954). ‘The role of exploratory drive in learning.’ Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 47: 6064.Google Scholar
Montgomery, K. C. and Segall, M. (1955). ‘Discrimination learning based upon the exploratory drive.’ Journal Comparative and Physiological Psychology 48: 225228.Google Scholar
Moray, N. (1959). ‘Attention in dichotic listening: Affective cues and the influence of instructions.’ Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 11: 5660.Google Scholar
Morsella, E. (2005). ‘The function of phenomenal states: Supramodular interaction theory.’ Psychological Review 112: 10001021.Google Scholar
Mowrer, O. H. (1960). Learning theory and behavior. New York, Wiley.Google Scholar
Mowrer, O. H. (1961). The crisis in psychiatry and religion. Princeton, NJ, Van Nostrand.Google Scholar
Muraven, M., Shmueli, D. and Burkley, E. (2006). ‘Conserving self-control strength.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 91: 524537.Google Scholar
Murdoch, I. (1967). The sovereignty of good over other concepts: The Leslie Stephen Lecture. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Murdoch, I. (1992). Metaphysics as a guide to morals. London, Chatto & Windus.Google Scholar
Murphy, N. (2007a). Introduction. In Murphy, N. C. and Stoeger, W. R. (Eds.), Evolution and emergence: Systems, organisms, persons (pp. 115). Oxford and New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Murphy, N. (2007b). Reductionism: How did we fall into it? And can we emerge from it? In Murphy, N. C. and Stoeger, W. R. (Eds.), Evolution and emergence: Systems, organisms, person (pp. 2439). Oxford and New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Murphy, N. C. and Stoeger, W. R., Eds. (2007). Evolution and emergence: Systems, organisms, persons. Oxford and New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Murphy, P. K. and Alexander, P. A. (2000). ‘A motivated exploration of motivation terminology.’ Contemporary Educational Psychology 25(1): 353.Google Scholar
Murray, D. J. (1995). Gestalt psychology and the cognitive revolution. Hemel Hempstead, Harvester Wheatsheaf.Google Scholar
Murray, H. A. (1936). ‘Facts which support the concept of need or drive.’ Journal of Personality 3: 2742.Google Scholar
Murray, H. A. (1938). Explorations in personality: A clinical and experimental study of fifty men of college age. New York and London, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Murray, H. A. (1953). Thematic Apperception Test manual. Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Mussel, P. (2010). ‘Epistemic curiosity and related constructs: Lacking evidence of discriminant validity.’ Personality and Individual Differences 49: 506510.Google Scholar
Nairne, J. S. (2002). ‘Remembering over the short-term: The case against the standard model.’ Annual Review of Psychology 53: 5381.Google Scholar
Neisser, U. (2002). ‘Wolfgang Köhler.’ National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, Biographical Memoirs, Volume 81. Retrieved 22 February 2016 from http://www.nap.edu/readingroom.php?book=biomems&page=wkohler.htmlGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. and Bruner, J. S. (2006). Narratives from the crib. Cambridge, MA and London, Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Newcombe, T. M. (1968a). Interpersonal balance. In Abelson, R. P., Aronson, E., McGuire, W. J. et al. (Eds.), Theories of cognitive consistency: A source book (pp. 2851). Chicago, Rand McNally.Google Scholar
Newcombe, T. M. (1968b). Introduction. In Abelson, R. P., Aronson, E., McGuire, W. J. et al. (Eds.), Theories of cognitive consistency: A source book (pp. xvxvii). Chicago, Rand McNally.Google Scholar
Newell, A. (1990). Unified theories of cognition. Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Newell, A., Laird, J. and Rosenbloom, P. S. (1993). The Soar papers: Research on integrated intelligence. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Newell, A. and Simon, H. A. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ, Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Nicholls, J. G. (1984). ‘Achievement motivation: Conceptions of ability, subjective experience, task choice, and performance.’ Psychological Review 91: 328346.Google Scholar
Nicholls, J. G. (1989). The competitive ethos and democratic education. Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Norman, D. A. (1968). ‘Towards a theory of memory and attention.’ Psychological Review 75: 522536.Google Scholar
Norman, D. A. and Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. In Davidson, R. J., Schwartz, E. and Shapiro, D. (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation (pp. 1–18). New York, Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Norretranders, T. (1999). The user illusion, cutting consciousness down to size. Harmondsworth, Penguin.Google Scholar
Oatley, K. and Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1987). ‘Towards a cognitive theory of emotions.’ Cognition and Emotion 1: 2950.Google Scholar
Oatley, K. and Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1996). The communicative theory of emotions: Empirical tests, mental models, and implications for social interaction. In Martin, L. and Tesser, A. (Eds.), Striving and feeling: Interactions among goals, affect, and self-regulation (pp. 363–393). Mahwah, NJ, Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Oatley, K., Keltner, D. and Jenkins, J. M. (2006). Understanding emotions. Malden, MA, Blackwell Pub.Google Scholar
Oatley, K. (1987). ‘Cognitive science and the understanding of emotion.’ Cognition and Emotion 1, 209–216.Google Scholar
Oettingen, G. and Gollwitzer, P. M. (2009). Making goal pursuit effective: Expectancy-dependent goal setting and planned goal striving. In Forgas, J. P., Baumeister, R. and Tice, D. M. (Eds.), Psychology of self-regulation (pp. 127146). New York and London, Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Oishi, S., Diener, E. and Lucas, R. (2007). ‘The optimum level of well-being: Can people be too happy?Perspectives on Psychological Science 2: 346360.Google Scholar
Onishi, K. H. and Baillargeon, R. (2005). ‘Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs?Science 308: 255258.Google Scholar
Ortony, A., Clore, G. L. and Collins, A. (1988). The cognitive structure of emotions. New York, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Oster, J. (1991). Toward Robert Frost: The reader and the poet. Athens and London, University of Georgia.Google Scholar
Ovsiankina, M. (1928). ‘Die Wiederanfnahme unterbrochener Handlugen.’ Psychologische Forschung 11: 302379.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (2000). The neurodynamics of emotions: An evolutionary-neurodevelopmental view. In Lewis, M. D. and Granic, I. (Eds.), Emotion, development, and self-organization: Dynamic systems approaches to emotional development (pp. 236264). Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (2005). ‘Affective consciousness: Core emotional feelings in animals and humans.’ Consciousness and Cognition 14: 3080.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (2007). ‘Neurologizing the psychology of affects: How appraisal-based constructivism and basic emotion theory can coexist.’ Perspectives on Psychological Science 2(3): 281296.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (2011a). The primary process affects in human development, happiness, and thriving. In Sheldon, K. M., Kashdan, T. B. and Steger, M. F. (Eds.), Designing positive psychology (pp. 51–85).Oxford and New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (2011b). ‘Toward a cross-species neuroscientific understanding of the affective mind: Do animals have emotional feelings?American Journal of Primatology 73(6): 545561.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. and Biven, L. (2012). The archaeology of mind: Neuroevolutionary origins of human emotions. New York, W. W. Norton & Co.Google Scholar
Pargament, K. I. (1997). The psychology of religion and coping: Theory, research, practice. New York, Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Pargament, K. I. (2002a). ‘The bitter and the sweet: An evaluation of the costs and benefits of religiousness.’ Psychological Inquiry 13(3): 168181.Google Scholar
Pargament, K. I., Magyar-Russell, G. M. and Murray-Swank, N. A. (2005). ‘The sacred and the search for significance: Religion as a unique process.’ Journal of Social Issues 61(4): 665687.Google Scholar
Parr, A. (2010). Emotion and social cognition in primates. In Breed, M. D. M. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of animal behavior (pp. 621627). Janice, Elsevier.Google Scholar
Pashler, H. E. (1998). The psychology of attention. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Patmore, C. (1856). The angel in the house, book II. London, J. W. Parker.Google Scholar
Patnoe, S. (1988). A narrative history of experimental social psychology. New York, Springer.Google Scholar
Paulesu, E., Frith, C. D. and Franckowiak, R. S. J. (1993). ‘The neural correlates of the verbal components of working memory.’ Nature 362(6418): 342345.Google Scholar
Peacocke, A. (2007). Emergent realities with causal efficacy: Some philosophical and theological applications. In Murphy, N. C. and Stoeger, W. R. (Eds.), Evolution and emergence: Systems, organisms, persons (pp. 267283). Oxford and New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Penfield, W. (1958). The excitable cortex in conscious man. Liverpool, Liverpool University Press.Google Scholar
Penfield, W. (1975). The mystery of the mind: A critical study of consciousness and the human brain. Princeton, NJ, Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Peplau, L. A. (1976). ‘Impact of fear of success and sex-role attitudes on women’s competitive achievement.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 34(4): 561568.Google Scholar
Perls, F. S. (1951/1972). Gestalt therapy: Excitement and growth in the human personality. London, Souvenir Press.Google Scholar
Perls, F. S. (1976). The Gestalt approach: An eye witness to therapy. New York, Bantam Books.Google Scholar
Perls, F. S., Hefferline, R. F. and Goodman, P. (1951). Gestalt therapy: Excitement and growth in the human personality. New York, Dell.Google Scholar
Perner, J. and Ruffman, T. (2005). ‘Infant’s insight into the mind: How deep?Science 308: 214216.Google Scholar
Perner, J., Ruffman, T. and Leekam, S. R. (1994). ‘Theory of mind is contagious: You catch it from your sibs.’ Child Development 65: 12281235.Google Scholar
Pervin, L. A. (1983). The stasis and flow of behavior: Towards a general theory of goals. In Page, M. M. (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation: Vol. 30 Personality: Current theory and research (pp. 153). Lincoln, University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Pervin, L. A., Ed. (1989). Goal concepts in personality and social psychology. Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Peters, R. A. (1978). ‘Effects of anxiety, curiosity, and perceived-instructor threat on student verbal behavior in the college classroom.’ Journal of Educational Psychology 70: 388395.Google Scholar
Peterson, C. and Seligman, M. E. P., Eds. (2004). Character strengths and virtues: A handbook and classification. New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Petri, H. L. and Govern, J. M. (2004). Motivation: Theory, research and applications (5th ed.). Belmont, CA, Thomson/Wadsworth.Google Scholar
Petri, H. L. and Govern, J. M. (2013). Motivation: Theory, research and applications (6th ed.). Belmont, CA, Wadsworth, Cengage Learning.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1950). The psychology of intelligence. London, Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1953). The origin of intelligence in the child. London, Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1971). Biology and knowledge. Chicago, University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J., Gattegno, C. and Hodgson, F. M. (1951). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. Melbourne, Heinemann in association with the New Education Fellowship.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. New York, Norton.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (2005a). ‘So how does the mind work?Mind and Language 20: 124.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (2005b). ‘A reply to Jerry Fodor on how the mind works.’ Mind and Language 20(1): 3338.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (2007). The stuff of thought: Language as a window into human nature. New York, Viking.Google Scholar
Pintrich, P. R. (2000b). ‘An achievement goal theory perspective on issues in motivation terminology, theory, and research.’ Contemporary Educational Psychology 25: 92104.Google Scholar
Pintrich, P. R., Conley, A. M. and Kempler, T. M. (2003). ‘Current issues in achievement goal theory and research.’ International Journal of Educational Research 39: 319337.Google Scholar
Piotrowski, J. T., Litman, J. A. and Valkenburg, P. (2014). ‘Measuring epistemic curiosity in young children.’ Infant and Child Development 23: 542553.Google Scholar
Polger, T. W. (2001). ‘Review of Dennett, Daniel C. Brainchildren: Essays on designing minds.’ Philosophical Psychology 14: 245250.Google Scholar
Pomerantz, E. M., Grolnick, W. S. and Price, C. E. (2005). The role of parents in how children approach achievement In Elliot, A. J. and Dweck, C. S. (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 259278). New York and London, The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. (1978). Chronometric explorations of mind. Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. (1994). ‘Attention: The mechanisms of consciousness.’ Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 91: 73987403.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. and Dehaene, S. (1994). ‘Attentional networks.’ Trends in Neurosciences 17: 7579.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. and Raichle, M. E. (1994). Images of mind. New York, Scientific American Library.Google Scholar
Powers, W. T. (1973). Behavior: The control of perception. Chicago, Aldine.Google Scholar
Powers, W. T., Clark, R. and McFarland, R. (1960). ‘A general feedback theory of human behavior: Part 2.’ Perceptual and Motor Skills Monograph 11(3, Serial No. 7).Google Scholar
Premack, D. and Woodruff, G. (1978). ‘Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind?Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1(4): 515526.Google Scholar
Pribram, K. (1971). Languages of the brain. Englewood Cliffs, NJ, Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Pribram, K. (1984). ‘What is iso and what is morphic in isomorphism?Psychological Research 46: 329332.Google Scholar
Prinz, J. (2004). Which emotions are basic? In Evans, D. and Cruse, P. (Eds.), Emotion, evolution and rationality (pp. 6987). Oxford, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Prinzmetal, W., Ha, R. and Khani, A. (2010). ‘The mechanisms of involuntary attention.’ Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 36(2): 255267.Google Scholar
Proulx, T. and Heine, S. J. (2006). ‘Death and black diamonds: Meaning, mortality, and the meaning maintenance model.’ Psychological Inquiry 17(4): 309318.Google Scholar
Proulx, T. and Heine, S. J. (2010). ‘The frog in Kierkegaard’s beer: Finding meaning in the threat-compensation literature.’ Social and Personality Psychology Compass 4(10): 889890.Google Scholar
Ramsden, P., Beswick, D. and Bowden, J. A. (1986). ‘Effects of learning skills interventions on first year students’ learning.’ Human Learning 5(3): 151164.Google Scholar
Rausch, E. (1966). Das Eigenschaftsproblem in der Gestalttheorie der Wahrnehmung. In Metzger, W. and Erke, H. (Eds.), Handbuch der Psychologie. Bd 1/1. Wahrnehmung und Bewußstein (pp. 866953). Göttingen, Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Raynor, J. (1969). ‘Future orientation and motivation of immediate activity: An elaboration of the theory of achievement motivation.’ Psychological Review 76: 606610.Google Scholar
Raynor, J. (1970). ‘Relationships between achievement-related motives, future orientation, and academic performance.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 15: 2833.Google Scholar
Raynor, J. (1974a). Future orientation: The cognitive elaboration of a theory of achievement motivation. In Atkinson, J. W. and Raynor, J. (Eds.), Motivation and achievement (pp. 121154). Washington, DC, V. W. Winston.Google Scholar
Raynor, J. O. (1974b). Motivation and career striving. In Atkinson, J. W. and Raynor, J. O. (Eds.), Motivation and achievement (pp. 369387). Washington, DC, V. W. Winston.Google Scholar
Raynor, J. O. (1981). Self-possession of attributes, self-evaluation, and future orientation: A theory of adult competence motivation. In Raynor, J. O. and Entin, E. E. (Eds.), Motivation, career striving, and aging (pp. 207–226). Washington DC, Hemisphere Publishing Corporation.Google Scholar
Raynor, J. O. (1982). Future orientation, self-evaluation, and achievement motivation: Use of an expectancy x value theory of personality and change. In Feather, N. T. (Ed.), Expectations and actions (pp. 97124). Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates.Google Scholar
Renninger, A. (1992). Individual interest and development: Implications for theory and practice. In Renninger, A., Hidi, S. and Krapp, A. (Eds.), The role of interest in learning and development (pp. 361395). Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Renninger, K. A., Hidi, S. and Krapp, A. (1992). The role of interest in learning and development. Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Richards, J. B., Litman, J. A. and Roberts, D. H. (2013). ‘Performance characteristics of measurement instruments of epistemic curiosity in third-year medical students.’ Medical Science Educator 23: 355363.Google Scholar
Richards, P. S. and Bergin, A. E. (1997). A spiritual strategy for counseling and psychotherapy. Washington, DC, APA Press.Google Scholar
Riesman, D. (1950). The lonely crowd: A study of the changing American character. New Haven, CT, Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Roberts, B. W. and Robins, R. W. (2000). ‘Broad dispositions, broad aspirations: The intersection of the Big Five dimensions and major life goals.’ Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26: 12841296.Google Scholar
Roberts, R. C. and Wood, W. J. (2006). Intellectual virtues: An essay in regulative epistemology. Oxford and New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rodin, J., Rennert, K. and Solomon, S. K. (1980). Intrinsic motivation for control: Fact or fiction. In Baum, A. and Singer, J. E. (Eds.), Advances in environmental psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 131148). Hillsdale, NJ: Taylor and Francis.Google Scholar
Rogers, C. (1963). The actualizing tendency in relation to ‘motives’ and to consciousness. In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 11, pp. 124). Lincoln, University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Rogers, C. R. (1961). On becoming a person: A therapist’s view of psychotherapy. Boston, Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Rolls, E. T. (1999). The brain and emotion. Oxford and New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rolls, E. T. (2005). Emotion explained. Oxford and New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rolls, E. T. (2008). Memory, attention, and decision-making: a unifying computational neuroscience approach. Oxford, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rosen, B. C. (1959). ‘Race, ethnicity and the achievement syndrome.’ American Sociological Review 24: 4760.Google Scholar
Rosen, B. C. and D’Andrade, R. G. (1959). ‘The psychosocial origins of achievement motivation.’ Sociometry 22(3): 185218.Google Scholar
Rosenthal, D. (1990). A theory of consciousness: ZIF Report 40/1990. Bielefeld, Zentrumflir Interdisziplinaire Forschung (Reprinted in Block, N., O. Flanagan and G. Guzeldere (1997). The Nature of Consciousness: Philosophical Debates).Google Scholar
Rosenthal, D. (1993). Thinking as one thinks. In Davies, M. and Humphrey, G. W. (Eds.), Consciousness (pp. 197223). Oxford, Blackwell.Google Scholar
Rosenthal, D. (1997). A theory of consciousness. In Block, N., Flanagan, O. and Guzeldere, G. (Eds.), The nature of consciousness: Philosophical debates (pp. 729853). Cambridge MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Rothbaum, F., Weisz, J. R. and Snyder, S. S. (1982). ‘Changing the world and changing the self: A two process model perceived control.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 42: 537.Google Scholar
Rothblatt, S. (1989). The idea of a university and its antithesis. Conversazione. Bundoora, La Trobe University.Google Scholar
Rothblatt, S. (1997). The modern university and its discontents: The fate of Newman’s legacies in Britain and America. Cambridge and New York, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rotter, J. B. (1954). Social learning and clinical psychology. New York, Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Rotter, J. B. (1966). ‘Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement.’ Psychological Monographs 80: 809.Google Scholar
Royce, J. R. and Powell, A. (1983). Theory of personality and individual differences: Factors, systems, processes. Englewood Cliffs, NJ, Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (1994). ‘Is there universal recognition of emotion from facial expressions? A review of cross-cultural studies.’ Psychological Bulletin 115: 102141.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (2003). ‘Core affect and the psychological construction of emotions.’ Psychological Review 110: 145172.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (2009). ‘Emotion, core affect, and psychological construction.’ Cognition and Emotion 23: 12591283.Google Scholar
Ryan, R. M. (1993). Agency and organization: Intrinsic motivation, autonomy and the self in psychological development. In Jacobs, J. (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation: Developmental perspectives on motivation (Vol. 40, pp. 156). Lincoln, University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Ryan, R. M. (1995). ‘Psychological needs and the facilitation of integrative processes.’ Journal of Personality 63: 397427.Google Scholar
Ryan, R. M. and Deci, E. L. (2000a). When rewards compete with nature: The undermining of intrinsic motivation and self-regulation. In Sansone, C. and Harackiewicz, J. M. (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation (pp. 1354). San Diego, Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ryan, R. M. and Deci, E. L. (2000b). ‘Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations: Classic definitions and new directions.’ Contemporary Educational Psychology 25: 5467.Google Scholar
Ryan, R. M. and Deci, E. L. (2000c). ‘Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being.’ American Psychologist 55(1): 6878.Google Scholar
Ryan, R. M. and Deci, E. L. (2017). Self-determination theory: basic psychological needs, in motivation, development, and wellness. New York and London, The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Ryan, R. M., Kuhl, J. and Deci, E. L. (1997). ‘Nature and autonomy: Organizational view of social and neurological aspects of self-regulation in behavior and development.’ Development and Psychopathology 9: 701728.Google Scholar
Ryan, R. M., Sheldon, K. M., Kasser, T. and Deci, E. L. (1996). All goals are not created equal: An organismic perspective on the nature of goals and their regulations. In Bargh, J. A. and Gollwitzer, P. M. (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 726). New York, Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Ryle, G. (1949). The concept of mind. London, Hutchinson’s University Library.Google Scholar
Sampson, R. E. (1989a). ‘The challenge of social change for psychology: Globalization and psychology’s theory of the person.’ American Psychologist 44: 914921.Google Scholar
Sampson, R. E. (1989b). The deconstruction of the self. In Shotter, J. and Gergen, K. J. (Eds.), Texts of identity (pp. 119). London, Sage.Google Scholar
Sansone, C. and Harackiewicz, J. M. (1996). ‘I don’t feel like it’: The function of interest in self-regulation. In Martin, L. L. and Tesser, A. E. (Eds.), Striving and feeling: Interactions among goals, affects, and self regulation (pp. 203228). Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sansone, C. and Harackiewicz, J. M. (1998). ‘“Reality” is complicated.’ American Psychologist 53: 673674.Google Scholar
Sansone, C. and Harackiewicz, J. M. (Ed.) (2000a). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: The search for optimal motivation and performance. San Diego, CA and London, Academic Press.Google Scholar
Sansone, C. and Harackiewicz, J. M. (2000b). Looking beyond rewards: The problem and promise of intrinsic rewards. In Sansone, C. and Harackiewicz, J. M. (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: The search for optimal motivation and performance. San Diego, CA and London, Academic Press.Google Scholar
Sansone, C. and Harackiewicz, J. M. (2000c). Controversies and new directions – Is it déjà vu all over again! In Sansone, C. and Harackiewicz, J. M. (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: The search for optimal motivation and performance (pp. 443453). San Diego, CA and London, Academic Press.Google Scholar
Sansone, C. and Morgan, C. (1992). ‘Intrinsic motivation and education: Competence in context.’ Motivation and Emotion 16: 249270.Google Scholar
Sartre, J.-P. and Mairet, P. (1948). Existentialism and humanism. London, Methuen.Google Scholar
Saver, J. L. and Damasio, A. R. (1991). ‘Preserved access and processing of social knowledge in a patient with acquired sociopathy due to ventromedial frontal damage.’ Neuropsychologia 29: 12411249.Google Scholar
Scarry, E. (1994). Resisting representation. New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Scarry, E. (1999). On beauty and being just. Princeton, NJ, Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Schank, R. C. (1990). Tell me a story: A new look at real and artificial memory. New York, Scribner.Google Scholar
Schank, R. C. and Abelson, R. P. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals, and understanding: An inquiry into human knowledge structures. Hillsdale, NJ and New York, L. Erlbaum Associates; distributed by the Halsted Press Division of John Wiley and Sons.Google Scholar
Schank, R. C. and Schank, R. C. (1999). Dynamic memory revisited. Cambridge and New York, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Schiefele, U., Krapp, A. and Winteler, A. (1992). Interest as a predictor of academic achievement: A meta-analysis of research. In Renninger, K. A., Hidi, S. and Krapp, A. (Eds.), The role of interest in learning and development (pp. 188212). Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Schrödinger, E. (1944). What is life? Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Schrödinger, E. (2000). What is life? With mind and matter and autobiographical sketches. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Schwarz, N. (1990). Feelings as information: Informational and motivational functions of affective states. In Sorrentino, R. M. and Higgins, E. T. (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior. (Vol. 2, pp. 527561). New York, Guilford.Google Scholar
Scott, S. and Kelly, G. (1762/1995). A description of Millenium Hall. Peterborough, ON, Broadview Press.Google Scholar
Searle, J. (1992). The rediscovery of the mind. Cambridge, MA and London, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Searle, J. (2010). Consciousness and the problem of free will. In Baumeister, R. F., Mele, A. R. and Vohs, K. D. (Eds.), Free will and consciousness: How might they work? (pp. 121134). New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1983). Intentionality: An essay in the philosophy of mind. Cambridge and New York, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sears, R. R. (1974). Discussion. In Weiner, B. (Ed.), Cognitive views of human motivation (pp. 8690). New York, Academic Press.Google Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness: On depression, development, and death. San Francisco and New York, W. H. Freeman (trade distributor, Scribner).Google Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P. (1998). Learned optimism. New York, Pocket Books (Simon and Schuster).Google Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P. (2002). Authentic happiness: Using the new positive psychology to realize your potential for lasting fulfillment. New York, Free Press.Google Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P. (2006). Learned optimism: How to change your mind and your life. New York, Vintage Books.Google Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P. (2011). Flourish: A visionary new understanding of happiness and well-being. New York, Free Press.Google Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P. and Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). ‘Positive psychology: An introduction.’ American Psychologist 55 (1), 514.Google Scholar
Shallice, T. (1988). From neuropsychology to mental structure. Cambridge and New York, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Shallice, T. and Burgess, P. (1998). The domain of supervisory processes and the temporal organisation of behaviour. In Roberts, A. C., Robbins, T. W. and Weiskrantz, L. (Eds.), The prefrontal cortex: Executive and cognitive functions. Royal Society (Great Britain). Discussion Meeting (pp. 2235). New York and Oxford, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sheldon, K. M. and Elliot, A. J. (1999). ‘Goal striving, need-satisfaction, and longitudinal well-being: The self-concordance model.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 76: 482497.Google Scholar
Sheldon, K. M., Kashdan, T. B. and Steger, M. F., Eds. (2011). Designing positive psychology. Oxford and New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sheldon, K. M., Ryan, R. M., Deci, E. L. and Kasser, T. (2004). ‘The independent effects of goal contents and motives on well-being: It’s both what you pursue and why you pursue it.’ Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 30: 475486.Google Scholar
Silberman, I. (2005). ‘Religion as a meaning system: Implications for the new millennium.’ Journal of Social Issues 61(4): 641663.Google Scholar
Simon, D., Snow, C. J. and Read, S. J. (2004). ‘The redux of cognitive consistency theories: Evidence judgments by constraint satisfaction.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 86(6): 814837.Google Scholar
Simon, H. A. (1967). ‘Motivational and emotional controls of cognition.’ Psychological Review 74: 2939.Google Scholar
Sirigu, A., Daprati, E., Ciancia, S., Giraux, P., Nighoghossian, N., Posada, A. and Haggard, P. (2003). ‘Altered awareness of voluntary action after damage to the parietal cortex.’ Nature Neuroscience 7: 8084.Google Scholar
Skår, J. (2003). ‘Self-organization as an actual theme.’ Philosophical Transactions: Mathematical, Physical and Engineering Sciences: No. 1807, Self-Organization: The Quest for the Origin and Evolution of Structure 361(1807): 10491056.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York, Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Smith, B. (1988). Gestalt theory: An essay in philosophy. In Smith, B. and Ehrenfels, C. (Eds.), Foundations of Gestalt theory (pp. 11–81). München, Philosophia Verlag.Google Scholar
Smith, B. and Ehrenfels, C. (1988). Foundations of Gestalt theory. München, Philosophia Verlag.Google Scholar
Smith, C. P., Ed. (1992). Motivation and personality: Handbook of thematic content analysis. Cambridge and New York, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sokolov, E. N. (2002). The orienting response in information processing. Mahwah, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Sokolov, E. N. and Vinogradova, O. S. (1975). Neuronal mechanisms of the orienting reflex. Hillsdale, NJ and New York, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; distributed by the Halsted Press Division of Wiley.Google Scholar
Sommerhoff, G. (2002). Understanding consciousness: Its function and brain processes. London, Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Soon, C. S., Brass, M., Heinze, H.-J. and Haynes, J.-D. (2008). ‘Unconscious determinants of free decisions in the human brain.’ Nature: Neuroscience 11: 543545.Google Scholar
Sorrentino, R. M., Roney, C. J. R. and Hanna, S. E. (1992). Uncertainty orientation. In Smith, C. P. (Ed.), Motivation and personality: Handbook of thematic content analysis (pp. 419427). Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Spence, K. W. (1956). Behavior theory and conditioning. New Haven, CT, Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Sperry, R., Miner, N. and Myers, R. (1955). ‘Visual pattern perception following subpail slicing and tantalum wire implantations in the visual cortex.’ Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 48: 5058.Google Scholar
Spielberger, C. D. and Starr, L. M. (1994). Curiosity and exploratory behavior. In O’Neil, H. F. J. and Drillings, M. (Eds.), Motivation: Theory and research. (pp. 221243). Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Starr, L. M. (1992). Assessment of curiosity as a multifaceted construct, Tampa, University Press of South Florida.Google Scholar
Steele, C. M. (1988). The psychology of self-affirmation: Sustaining the integrity of the self. In Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 21, pp. 261302). San Diego, CA, Academic Press.Google Scholar
Steele, C. M. and Liu, T. J. (1983). ‘Dissonance processes as self-affirmation.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 45(1): 519.Google Scholar
Steele, C. M., Spencer, S. J. and Lynch, M. (1993). ‘Self-image resilience and dissonance: The role of affirmational resources.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 64: 885896.Google Scholar
Stone, J., Aronson, E., Crain, A. L., Winslow, M. P. and Fried, C. B. (1994). ‘Inducing hypocrisy as a means of encouraging young adults to use condoms.’ Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 1: 116128.Google Scholar
Stringer, C. and McKie, R. (1996). African exodus: The origins of modern humanity. London, Jonathon Cape.Google Scholar
Surian, L., Caldi, S. and Sperber, D. (2007). ‘Attribution of beliefs by 13-month-old infants.’ Psychological Science 18: 580586.Google Scholar
Swartz, H. and Clore, G. L. (1983). ‘Mood, missattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 45: 513523.Google Scholar
Tawney, R. H. (1938). Religion and the rise of capitalism: A historical study. Harmondsworth, Penguin.Google Scholar
Taylor, S. E. (1989). Positive illusions: Creative self-deception and the healthy mind. New York, Basic Books.Google Scholar
Thagard, P. N. (2002). ‘Emotional gestalts: Appraisal, change, and the dynamics of affect.’ Personality and Social Psychology Review 6(4): 274282.Google Scholar
Thibodeau, R. and Aronson, E. (1992). ‘Taking a closer look: Reasserting the role of self-concept in dissonance theory.’ Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 18: 591602.Google Scholar
Thomas, E. J. and Zander, A. (1959). ‘The relationship of goal structure to motivation under extreme conditions.’ Journal of Individual Psychology 15: 121127.Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1911). Animal intelligence: Experimental studies. New York, The Macmillan Company.Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1913). The psychology of learning. New York, Teachers College, Columbia University.Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1935). The psychology of wants, interests and attitudes. New York, D. Appleton-Century.Google Scholar
Thrash, T. M. and Elliot, A. J. (2001). Delimiting and integrating achievement motive and goal constructs. In Efflides, A., Kuhl, J. and Sorrentino, R. M.(Eds.), Trends and prospects in motivation research (pp. 321). Dordrecht, Boston and London, Kluwer Academic Publishers.Google Scholar
Tice, D. M. (2009). How emotions affect self-regulation. In Forgas, J. P., Baumeister, R. and Tice, D. M. (Eds.), Psychology of self-regulation (pp. 201215). New York and London, Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Tillich, P. (1963). Christianity and the encounter of the world religions. New York, Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Todt, E. and Schreiber, S. (1998). Development of interests. In Hoffmann, L., Krapp, A., Renninger, K. A. and Baunert, J. (Eds.), Interest and learning: Proceedings of the Seeon Conference on Interest and Gender (pp. 2540). Keil, Germany, IPN.Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1925). ‘Purpose and cognition: The determiners of animal learning.’ Psychological Review 32: 285297.Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1926). ‘A behavioristic theory of ideas.’ Psychological Review 33: 352369.Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1927). ‘A behaviorist’s definition of consciousness.’ Psychological Review 34: 433439.Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1932/1949). Purposive behavior in animals and men. Berkeley, University of California Press.Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1948). ‘Cognitive maps in rats and men.’ Psychological Review 55: 189208.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. and Call, J. (1997). Primate cognition. New York and Oxford, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T. and Moll, H. (2005). ‘Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition.’ Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28: 675691.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. and Ratner, H. (1993). ‘Cultural learning.’ Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16: 495552.Google Scholar
Tomkins, S. S. (1962–1963). Affect, imagery, consciousness (Vols. 1 and 2). New York, Springer.Google Scholar
Tononi, G. (2004). ‘An information integration theory of consciousness.’ BMC Neuroscience 5(42)22 pages.Google Scholar
Tononi, G. (2005). ‘Consciousness, information integration, and the brain.’ Progress in Brain Research 150 (The Boundaries of Consciousness: Neurobiology and Neuropathology): 109126.Google Scholar
Tononi, G. and Edelman, G. M. (1998). ‘Consciousness and complexity.’ Science 282(5395): 18461851.Google Scholar
Tononi, G. and Koch, C. (2008). ‘The neural correlates of consciousness: An update.’ Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1124: 239261.Google Scholar
Treisman, A. and Gelade, G. (1980). ‘A feature-integration theory of attention.’ Cognitive Psychology 12: 97136.Google Scholar
Treisman, A. M. (1960). ‘Contextual cues in selective listening.’ Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 12: 242248.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of episodic memory. New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Turing, A. (1952). ‘The chemical basis for morphogenesis.’ Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society 237: 3772.Google Scholar
Turner, H. (1998). The Roots of Science: An Investigative Journey through the World’s Religions. Auckland, The Deepsight Trust.Google Scholar
Tweney, R. D. (1997). ‘Jonathan Edwards and determinism.’ Journal for the History of the Behavioral Sciences 33: 365.Google Scholar
Tweney, R. D. and Wachholtz, A. B. (2004). ‘Wegner’s “illusion” anticipated: Jonathan Edwards on the will.’ Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27(5): 676.Google Scholar
Updike, J. (1989). Self-consciousness: Memoirs. New York, Knopf.Google Scholar
Updike, J. (1998). Bech at Bay. London, Hamish Hamilton.Google Scholar
Van Gulick, R. R. (2007). Who’s in charge here? And who’s doing all the work? In Murphy, N. C. and Stoeger, W. R. (Eds.), Evolution and emergence: Systems, organisms, persons (pp. 7487). Oxford and New York, Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Van Yperen, N. W. (2006). ‘A novel approach to assessing achievement goals in the context of the 2X2 framework: Identifying distinct profiles of individuals with different dominant achievement goals.’ Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 32: 14321445.Google Scholar
Van Yperen, N. W., Elliot, A. J. and Ansell, F. (2009). ‘The influence of mastery-avoidance goals on performance improvement.’ European Journal of Social Psychology 39: 932943.Google Scholar
Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Lens, W., Soenens, B., Matos, L. and Lacan, M. (2004). ‘Less is sometimes more: Goal content matters.’ Journal of Educational Psychology 96(4): 755764.Google Scholar
Velmans, P. H. (2000). Understanding consciousness. London, Routledge.Google Scholar
Velmans, P. H. (2003). ‘Preconscious free will.’ Journal of Consciousness Studies 10(12): 4261.Google Scholar
Veroff, J. (1969). ‘Social comparison and the development of achievement motivation.’ In C. Smith (Ed.), Achievement related motives in children (pp. 46–100).Google Scholar
Verstegen, I. (2005). Arnheim, Gestalt and art: A psychological theory. Wien and New York, Springer.Google Scholar
Vittersø, J., Søholt, Y., Hetland, A., Thoresen, I. A. and Røysamb, E. (2010). ‘Was Hercules happy? Some answers from a functional model of human well-being.’ Social Indicators Research 95: 118.Google Scholar
Vohs, K. D. and Baumeister, R. F., Eds. (2010). Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (2nd ed). New York, Guilford.Google Scholar
Vohs, K. D., Baumeister, R. F., Schmeichel, B. J., Twenge, J. M., Nelson, N. M. and Tice, D. M. (2008). ‘Making choices impairs subsequent self-control: A limited-resource account of decision making, self-regulation, and active initiative.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 94: 883898.Google Scholar
von Ehrenfels, C. (1890). ‘Über Gestaltqualtitaten.’ Vierteljahrsschriftfur wissenschaftliche (Philosophical Psychology): 249292.Google Scholar
von Ehrenfels, C. (1890/1988). On “gestalt qualities”. In Smith, B. (Eds.), Foundations of Gestalt theory (pp. 82117). Munchen, Wien, Philosopia Verlag.Google Scholar
von Ehrenfels, C. (1916/1988). Gestalt level and gestalt purity. In Smith, B. (Eds.), Foundations of Gestalt theory (pp. 118120). Munchen, Wien, Philosopia Verlag.Google Scholar
von Ehrenfels, C. (1937). ‘On gestalt-qualities.’ Psychological Review 44(6): 521524.Google Scholar
Vytal, K. and Hamann, S. (2010). ‘Neuroimaging support for discrete neural correlates of basic emotions: A Voxel-based meta-analysis.’ Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22(12): 28642885.Google Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1913). ‘Psychology as the behaviorist views it.’ Psychological Review 20: 158177.Google Scholar
Weary, G. and Edwards, J. A. (1994). ‘Individual differences in causal uncertainty.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 67: 308318.Google Scholar
Weary, G. and Edwards, J. A. (1996). Causal uncertainty beliefs and related goal structures. In Sorrentino, R. and Higgins, E. T. (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: The interpersonal context (Vol. 3, pp. 148181). New York, Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Weary, G., Vaughn, L. A., Stewart, B. D. and Edwards, John A. (2006). ‘Adjusting for the correspondence bias: Effects of causal uncertainty, cognitive busyness, and causal strength of situational information.’ Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 42: 8794.Google Scholar
Weber, M., Parsons, T. and Tawney, R. H. (1930). The Protestant ethic and the spirit of capitalism. London, G. Allen & Unwin, Ltd.Google Scholar
Webster, D. M. and Kruglanski, A. W. (1994). ‘Individual differences in need for cognitive closure.’ Journal of Personality & Social Psychology 67: 10491062.Google Scholar
Wegner, D. M. (2002). The illusion of conscious will. Cambridge, MA and London, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wegner, D. M. and commentators (2004). ‘Precis of The illusion of conscious will with open peer commentary.’ Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27: 649694.Google Scholar
Weil, S. (1951/1979). Waiting on God. London, Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Weiner, B. (1972). Theories of motivation: From mechanism to cognition. Chicago, Markham Pub. Co.Google Scholar
Weiner, B., Ed. (1974a). Achievement motivation and attribution theory. Morristown, NJ, General Learning Press.Google Scholar
Weiner, B., Ed. (1974b). Cognitive views of human motivation. New York, Academic Press.Google Scholar
Weiner, B., (1974c). An attributional interpretation of expectacy-value theory. In Weiner, B. (Ed.), Cognitive views of human motivation (pp. 5170). New York, Academic Press.Google Scholar
Weiner, B. (1979). ‘A theory of motivation for some classroom experience.’ Journal of Educational Psychology 71: 325.Google Scholar
Weiner, B. (1985a). ‘An attributional theory of achievement motivation and emotion.’ Psychological Review 92: 548573.Google Scholar
Weiner, B. (1985b). ‘“Spontaneous” causal search.’ Psychological Bulletin 97(7484).Google Scholar
Weiner, B. (1986). An attributional theory of motivation and emotion. New York, Springer- Verlag.Google Scholar
Weiner, B. (1995). Judgements of responsibility: A foundation for a theory of social conduct. New York, Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Weiner, B. (2005). Motivation from an attribution perspective and the social psychology of perceived competence. In Elliot, A. J. and Dweck, C. S. (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 7384). New York and London, The Guildford Press.Google Scholar
Weiner, B. (2010). ‘The development of an attribution-based theory of motivation: A history of ideas.’ Educational Psychologist 45(1): 2836.Google Scholar
Weiner, B., Frieze, I., Kukla, A., Reed, L., Stanley, R. and Rosenbaum, R. M. (1971). Perceiving the causes of success and failure. In Jones, E. E., Kanouse, D. E., Kelly, H. H. et al. (Eds.), Attribution: Perceiving the causes of behaviour (pp. 95120). Morristown, NJ, General Learning Press.Google Scholar
Weiner, B. and Kukla, A. (1970). ‘An attributional analysis of achievement motivation.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 15: 120.Google Scholar
Weiner, B., Russell, D. and Lerman, D. (1978). Affective consequences of causal ascriptions. In Harvey, J. H., Ickes, W. and Kidd, R. F. (Eds.), New directions in attribution research (Vol. 2, pp. 5990). Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Werner, H. (1948). Comparative psychology of mental development. New York, International Universities Press.Google Scholar
Wertheimer, M. (1912). ‘Experimentelle Studien uber das Sehen von Bewegung.’ Zeitschrift fur Psychologie 61: 161265.Google Scholar
Wertheimer, M. (1922/1938). The general theoretical situation. In Ellis, W. D. (Ed.), A source book of Gestalt psychology (pp. 1216). New York, Humanities Press.Google Scholar
Wertheimer, M. (1923/1938). Laws of organization in perceptual forms. In Ellis, W. D. (Ed.), A source book of Gestalt psychology (pp. 71–88). London, Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Wertheimer, M. (1945). Productive thinking. New York, Harper.Google Scholar
Wesley, J. and Wesley, C. (1747). Hymns and sacred poems. Dublin, Samuel Powell.Google Scholar
West, R., Schwarb, H. and Johnson, B. N. (2010). ‘The influence of age and individual differences in executive function on stimulus processing in the oddball task.’ Cortex: 46(4 Special issue: The Cognitive Neuroscience of Aging): 550563.Google Scholar
White, G., Greenoak, F. and Mabey, R. (1986). The journals of Gilbert White. London, Century.Google Scholar
White, R. W. (1959). ‘Motivation reconsidered: The concept of competence.’ Psychological Review 66: 297333.Google Scholar
White, R. W. (1960). Competence and the psychosexual stages of development. In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (pp. 97140). Lincoln, University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
White, R. W. (1972). The enterprise of living: Growth and organization in personality. New York, Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Wicklund, R. A. and Gollwitzer, P. M. (1982). Symbolic self-completion. Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Wilson, A. N. (2003). Iris Murdoch, as I knew her. London, Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Wilson, T. D., Centerbar, D. B., Kermer, D. A., & Gilbert, D. T. (2005). ‘The pleasures of uncertainty: Prolonging positive moods in ways people do not anticipate.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 88: 521.Google Scholar
Winter, D. G. (1973). The power motive. New York, Free Press.Google Scholar
Winter, D. G. (1992a). Power motivation revisited. In Smith, C. P. E. (Ed.), Motivation and personality: Handbook of thematic content analysis (pp. 301310). Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Winter, D. G. (1992b). Responsibility. In Smith, C. P. (Ed.), Motivation and personality: Handbook of thematic content analysis (pp. 500505). Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Winterbottom, M. R. (1958). The relation of need for achievement to learning experiences in independence and mastery. In Atkinson, J. W. (Ed.), Motives and fantasy, action and society (pp. 453–478). Princeton, NJ, Nan Nostrand.Google Scholar
Wolfe, J. M. (1994b). ‘Guided search II: A revised model of visual search.’ Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 1: 202238.Google Scholar
Wolfe, J. M. (1998). Visual search. In Pashler, H. (Ed.), Attention (pp. 13–73). Hove, East Sussex, Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Wolfe, J. M., Cave, K. R. and Franzel, S. L. (1989). ‘Guided search: An alternative to the feature integration model for visual search.’ Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 15: 419433.Google Scholar
Wolford, G., Miller, M. B. and Gazzaniga, M. S. (2004). Split decisions. In Gazzaniga, M. S. (Ed.), The cogntive neurosciences III (pp. 11891200). Cambridge, MA, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wolpert, D. and Miall, R. (1996). ‘Forward models for physiological motor control.’ Neural Networks 9: 12651279.Google Scholar
Wong, P. T. P. (1998c). Spirituality, meaning, and successful aging. In Wong, P. T. P. and Fry, P. S. (Eds.), The human quest for meaning: A handbook of psychological research and clinical applications (pp. 359394). Mahwah, NJ and London, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Wong, P. T. P. (2011). ‘Positive psychology 2.0: Towards a balanced interactive model of the good life.’ Canadian Psychology 52(2): 6981Google Scholar
Wong, P. T. P. and Fry, P. S., Eds. (1998). The human quest for meaning: A handbook of psychological research and clinical applications. Mahwah, NJ and London, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Wood, N. and Cowan, N. (1995). ‘The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: How frequent are attention shifts to one’s name in an irrelevant auditory channel?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 21(1): 255260.Google Scholar
Woodworth, R. S. (1918). Dynamic psychology. New York, Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Woodworth, R. S. (1921). Psychology: A study of mental life. New York, Henry Holt.Google Scholar
Woodworth, R. S. (1932). Psychology: A study of mental life. London, Methuen.Google Scholar
Woodworth, R. S. (1958). Dynamics of behavior. New York, Holt.Google Scholar
Wordsworth, W. (1977). The Poems. Harmondsworth, Penguin Classics.Google Scholar
Young, P. T. (1949). ‘Food-seeking drive, affective process, and learning.’ Psychological Review 56: 98121.Google Scholar
Zachar, P. and Ellis, R., Eds. (2012). Emotional theories of Jaak Panksepp and Jim Russell. Amsterdam, John Benjamin.Google Scholar
Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Cognitive theories in social psychology. In Lindzey, G. and Aronson, E. (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (2nd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 320411). Reading, MA, Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Zajonc, R. B. (1980). ‘Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences.’ American Psychologist 35: 151175.Google Scholar
Zajonc, R. B. (1984). ‘On the primacy of affect.’ American Psychologist 39: 117123.Google Scholar
Zajonc, R. B. (1990). ‘Leon Festinger (1919–1989).’ American Psychologist 45: 661662.Google Scholar
Zajonc, R. B. (1998). Emotions. In Gilbert, D., Fiske, S. T. and Lindzey, G. (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (pp. 591632). New York, McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Zanna, M. P. and Cooper, J. (1974). ‘Dissonance and the pill: An attributional approach to studying the arousal properties of dissonance.’ Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 29: 703709.Google Scholar
Zanna, M. P. and Cooper, J. (1976). Dissonance and the attribution process. In Harvey, J. H., Ickes, W. J. and Kidd, R. F. (Eds.), New directions in attribution research (Vol. 1, pp. 199217). Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Zanna, M. P., Higgins, E. T. and Taves, P. A. (1975). Dissonance and the phenomenology of arousal: A test of the aversive nature of dissonance arousal. Unpublished manuscript, Princeton University.Google Scholar
Zeigarnik, B. (1927). ‘Das Behalten erledigten und unerledigten Handlungen.’ Psychologische Forschung 9: 185.Google Scholar
Zuckerman, M. (1994). Behavioral expressions and biosocial bases of sensation seeking. Cambridge and New York, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • David Beswick, University of Melbourne
  • Book: Cognitive Motivation
  • Online publication: 30 August 2017
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316822920.015
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • David Beswick, University of Melbourne
  • Book: Cognitive Motivation
  • Online publication: 30 August 2017
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316822920.015
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • David Beswick, University of Melbourne
  • Book: Cognitive Motivation
  • Online publication: 30 August 2017
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316822920.015
Available formats
×