Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-v9fdk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-16T01:37:57.043Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

1 - Strong Inference in Psychophysiological Science

from Foundations

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 January 2017

John T. Cacioppo
Affiliation:
University of Chicago
Louis G. Tassinary
Affiliation:
Texas A & M University
Gary G. Berntson
Affiliation:
Ohio State University
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ax, A. F. (1964). Goals and methods of psychophysiology. Psychophysiology, 1: 825.Google Scholar
Berntson, G. G., Sarter, M., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2003). Ascending visceral regulation of cortical affective information processing. European Journal of Neuroscience, 18: 21032109.Google Scholar
Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Lorig, T. S., Norris, C. J., Rickett, E., & Nusbaum, H. (2003). Just because you’re imaging the brain doesn’t mean you can stop using your head: a primer and set of first principles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85: 650661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cacioppo, J. T. & Tassinary, L. G. (1990). Inferring psychological significance from physiological signals. American Psychologist, 45: 1628.Google Scholar
Cacioppo, J. T., Tassinary, L. G., & Berntson, G. G. (eds.) (2007). Handbook of Psychophysiology, 3rd edn. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cinzia, D. D. & Vittorio, G. (2009). Neuroaesthetics: a review. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 19: 682687.Google Scholar
Coles, M. G. H., Donchin, E., & Porges, S. W. (1986). Psychophysiology: Systems, Processes, and Applications. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Craig, A. D. (2002). How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3: 655666.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craig, A. D. (2009). How do you feel – now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10: 5970.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Critchley, H. D. & Harrison, N. A. (2013). Visceral influences on brain and behavior. Neuron, 77: 624638.Google Scholar
Donchin, E. (1982). The relevance of dissociations and the irrelevance of dissociationism: a reply to Schwartz and Pritchard. Psychophysiology, 19: 457463.Google Scholar
Drake, S. (1967). Galileo Galilei. In Edwards, P. (ed.), The Encyclopedia of Philosophy. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Field, D. T. & Inman, L. A. (2014). Weighing brain activity with the balance: a contemporary replication of Angelo Mosso’s historical experiment. Brain, 137: 634639.Google Scholar
Garbarini, F. & Adenzato, M. (2004). At the root of embodied cognition: cognitive science meets neurophysiology. Brain and Cognition, 56: 100106.Google Scholar
Gardiner, H. M., Metcalf, R. C., & Beebe-Center, J. G. (1937). Feeling and Emotion: A History of Theories. New York: American Book Company.Google Scholar
Greenfield, N. S. & Sternbach, R. A. (1972). Handbook of Psychophysiology. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.Google Scholar
Harrington, A. (1987). Medicine, Mind, and the Double Brain: Study in Nineteenth-Century Thought. Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrison, N. A., Gray, M. A., Gianaros, P. J., & Critchley, H. D. (2010). The embodiment of emotional feelings in the brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 30: 1287812884.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landis, C. (1930). Psychology and the psychogalvanic reflex. Psychological Review, 37: 381398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meaney, M. J., Bhatnagar, S., Larocque, S., McCormick, C. M., Shanks, N., Sharma, S., … & Plotsky, P. M. (1996). Early environment and the development of individual differences in the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal stress response. In Pfeffer, C. R. (ed.), Severe Stress and Mental Disturbance in Children (pp. 85127). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.Google Scholar
Metalis, S. A. & Hess, E. H. (1982). Pupillary response/semantic differential scale relationships. Journal of Research in Personality, 16: 201216.Google Scholar
Molenberghs, P., Cunnington, R., & Mattingley, J. B. (2012). Brain regions with mirror properties: a meta-analysis of 125 human fMRI studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36: 341349.Google Scholar
Oosterhof, N. N., Tipper, S. P., & Downing, P. (2013). Crossmodal and action-specific: neuroimaging the human mirror neuron system. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17: 311318.Google Scholar
Partala, T. & Surakka, V. (2003). Pupil size variation as an indication of affective processing. International Journal of Human–Computer Studies, 59: 185198.Google Scholar
Platt, J. R. (1964). Strong inference. Science, 146: 347353.Google Scholar
Sandrone, S., Bacigaluppi, M., Galloni, M. R., Cappa, S. F., Moro, A., Catani, M., … & Martino, G. (2014). Weighing brain activity with the balance: Angelo Mosso’s original manuscripts come to light. Brain, 137: 621633.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sarter, M., Berntson, G. G., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1996). Brain imaging and cognitive neuroscience: towards strong inference in attributing function to structure. American Psychologist, 51: 1321.Google Scholar
Shadish, W., Cook, T., & Campbell, D. (2002). Experimental and Quasi-Experimental Designs for Generalized Causal Inference. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Stern, J. A. (1964). Toward a definition of psychophysiology. Psychophysiology, 1: 9091.Google Scholar
Stevens, S. S. (1951). Handbook of Experimental Psychology. New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Townsend, J. T. & Ashby, F. G. (1983). Stochastic Modeling of Elementary Psychological Processes. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tranel, D. & Damasio, A. R. (1985). Knowledge without awareness: an autonomic index of facial recognition by prosopagnosics. Science, 228: 14531454.Google Scholar
Vanderhasselt, M., Remue, J., Kei Ng, K., & De Raedt, R. (2014). The interplay between the anticipation and subsequent online processing of emotional stimuli as measured by pupillary dilatation: the role of cognitive appraisal. Frontiers in Psychology, 5: 207.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×