Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-84b7d79bbc-c654p Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-29T17:19:03.387Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

10 - A display with two depth layers: attentional segregation and declutter

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 February 2011

Claudia Roda
Affiliation:
The American University of Paris, France
Get access

Summary

Analogous to the introduction of colour displays, 3D displays hold the potential to expand the information that can be displayed without increasing clutter. This is in addition to the application of 3D technology to showing volumetric data. Going beyond colour, separation by depth has in the past been shown to enable very fast (‘parallel’) visual search (Nakayama and Silverman 1986), something that separation by colour alone does not do. The ability to focus attention exclusively on a depth plane provides a potentially powerful (and relatively practical) extension to command-and-control displays. Just one extra depth layer can declutter the display. For this reason we have developed a ‘Dual-layer’ display with two physically separated layers. As expected, conjunction search times become parallel when information is split into two depth layers but only when the stimuli are simple and non-overlapping; complex and overlapping imagery in the rear layer still interferes with visual search in the front layer. With the Dual-layer cockpit display, it is possible to increase information content significantly without substantially affecting ease-of-search. We show experimentally that the secondary depth cues (accommodation and parallax) boost this advantage.

We expect the primary ‘declutter’ market to lie in applications that do not tolerate the overlooking of crucial information, in environments that are space limited, and in mobile displays. Note that the use of 3D to declutter fundamentally differs from the use of 3D to show volumetric spatial relationships.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baudisch, P. 2008. Designing for small screens, Tutorial held at MobileHCI 2008, Amsterdam, 2–5 SeptemberGoogle Scholar
Ellis, S. R., and McGreevy, M. W. 1987. Perspective traffic display format and airline pilot traffic avoidance, Human Factors 29: 371–82CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuchs, G., and Schumann, H. 2004. Visualizing abstract data on maps, in Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Information Visualisation IV, London: 139–45
He, Z. J., and Nakayama, K. 1992. Surfaces vs. features in visual search, Nature 359: 231–3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
He, Z. J., and Nakayama, K. 1995. Visual attention to surfaces in three-dimensional space, in Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 92: 11155–9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kooi, F. L. 2003. Multi-plane display for displaying overlapping images. EUR patent application 03078288.2
Kooi, F. L., and Toet, A. 2004. Visual comfort of binocular and 3D displays, Displays 25: 99–108CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kooi, F. L., Toet, A., Tripathy, S. P., and Levi, D. M. 1994. The effect of similarity and duration on spatial interaction in peripheral vision, Spatial Vision 8: 255–79CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marr, D. 1982. Vision: A Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information. New York: FreemanGoogle Scholar
Nakayama, K., and Silverman, G. H. 1986. Serial and parallel processing of visual feature conjunctions, Nature 320: 264–5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olmos, O., Wickens, C. D., and Chudy, A. 2000. Tactical displays for combat awareness: An examination of dimensionality and frame of reference concepts and the application of cognitive engineering, International Journal of Aviation Psychology 10(3): 247–71CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pastoor, S. 1993. Human factors of 3D displays, Displays 14: 150–7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenholtz, R., Li, Y., and Nakano, L. 2007. Measuring visual clutter, Journal of Vision 7(2): 1–22CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Streefkerk, J. W., Esch-Bussemakers, M. P., and Neerincx, M. A. 2006. Designing personal attentive user interfaces in the mobile public safety domain, Computers in Human Behavior 22: 749–70CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toet, A. 2006. Gaze directed displays as an enabling technology for attention aware systems, Computers in Human Behavior 22: 615–47CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Treisman, A. M., and Gelade, G. 1980. A feature integration theory of attention, Cognitive Psychology 12: 97–136CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verstynen, H. A. 1980. Potential roles for the cockpit traffic display in the evolving ATC system, Report No. SAE-800736. Warrendale, PA: Society of Automotive EngineersGoogle Scholar
Wagner, M., and Hochstein, S. 2000. Visual-search and attention shifts between layers in the 3D environment defined by linear perspective, Perception 29 ECVP Abstracts: 14
Wheatley, C., Cook, M. L., and Vidyasagar, T. R. 2004. Surface segregation influences pre-attentive search in depth, NeuroReport 15: 303–5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zon, R., and Roerdink, M. 2007. HCI testing in flight simulator: Set up and crew briefing procedures. Design and test cycles for the future, in D. Harris, (ed.), Engineering Psychology and Cognitive Ergonomics, Berlin: Springer-Verlag: 867–76CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×