Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-jwnkl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-10T13:27:24.514Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

1 - Developmental dynamics: the new view from the life sciences

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

Robert Lickliter
Affiliation:
Professor of Psychology Florida International University
Alan Fogel
Affiliation:
University of Utah
Barbara J. King
Affiliation:
College of William and Mary, Virginia
Stuart G. Shanker
Affiliation:
York University, Toronto
Get access

Summary

James is an eleven-year-old boy who is tall for his age, has blond hair and blue eyes, loves to play baseball, and is the best right-handed pitcher on his little league team. James is easy going, popular among his classmates at school, and excels in math and science classes. What is the source of such traits as athletic ability, temperament, and intelligence? Why are some children outgoing and socially skilled, while others appear introverted and avoid unfamiliar social situations? Why do some children find puzzles of logic interesting and challenging, while others don’t seem interested or willing to apply themselves to such mental tasks?

In the first half of the twentieth century, many biologists and psychologists thought that major aspects of behavioral development progressed in an orderly and preordained sequence under the direct control of genes. From this view, genes were seen to guide the nervous system to mature in a predetermined fashion, giving rise to so-called “innate” or “instinctive” behavior. Likewise, human characteristics like temperament, intelligence, or athletic ability were thought to be genetically based and to be relatively unaffected by experience or environment. Thanks in large part to more than half a century of comparative and developmental research, most biologists and psychologists now appreciate that behavior does not simply unfold from some predetermined genetic blueprint or template. Assumptions of genetically determined “innate” or “hard-wired” behavior have gradually given way to the realization that genes cannot, in and of themselves, produce behavioral or psychological traits or characteristics.

Type
Chapter
Information
Human Development in the Twenty-First Century
Visionary Ideas from Systems Scientists
, pp. 11 - 17
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Coen, E. (1999). The art of genes: how organisms make themselves. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gilbert, S. F. (2000). Developmental biology (6th edn.). Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.Google Scholar
Gottlieb, G., D. Wahlsten, and R. Lickliter (2006). The significance of biology for human development: a developmental psychobiological systems view. In Damon, W. (series ed.) and Lerner, R. (vol. ed.), Handbook of child psychology, vol. 1: theoretical models of human development (6th edn., pp. 210–257). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Johnston, T. D., and Edwards, L. (2002). Genes, interactions, and the development of behavior. Psychological Review, 109, 26–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, E. F. (2000). The century of the gene. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lewontin, R. C. (2000). The triple helix: genes, organism, and environment. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Michel, G., and Moore, C. (1995). Developmental psychobiology: an integrative science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Moore, D. S. (2001). The dependent gene: the fallacy of nature vs. nurture. New York: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Robert, J. S. (2004). Embryology, epigenesis, and evolution: taking development seriously. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×