Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-gq7q9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-19T10:24:10.365Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

4 - Coding Third-Order Image Structure

from Part One - Biological Networks

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 May 2010

Roland Baddeley
Affiliation:
University of Oxford
Peter Hancock
Affiliation:
University of Stirling
Peter Földiák
Affiliation:
University of St Andrews, Scotland
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Natural images have been shown to demonstrate enormous structural redundancy; this rinding has motivated the incorporation of statistical image models into computational theories of visual processing, producing a variety of candidate encoding strategies (Field, 1987; Sirovich and Kirby, 1987; Baddeley and Hancock, 1991). Many of these strategies effectively filter out predictable correlational structure so as to reduce directly or indirectly the dimensionality of the visual input. One advantage of such strategies is that if the image data can be encoded into a representation whose axes lie closer to the “natural” axes of the visual input, thresholding might produce a “sparse-distributed” representation, i.e. one which would show only sparse neural activity in response to an expected stimulus. Perhaps the best-documented support for such a strategy has come from work by D. J. Field (1987), who investigated the global 2-D amplitude spectra (averaged across all orientations) of an ensemble of natural images; he found that the amplitude falls off typically as the inverse of radial spatial frequency f, that is, the corresponding power spectra Ŝ(f) fall off as f-2. If visual signals with these properties were to be encoded by a bank of spatial-frequency-selective mechanisms or “channels” whose spatial-frequency bandwidths are constant in octaves, the outputs of each channel (Field, 1987) should exhibit similar energies (and therefore similar r.m.s. contrasts, since the channel outputs are assumed to have zero mean). The advantage of this so-called “scale invar-iance” is that by thresholding these channel outputs, a visual system can easily discount the “expected” struture of natural scenes.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2000

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×