Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-84b7d79bbc-fnpn6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-29T08:25:00.873Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 17 - The relationship between general language competence and second language reading proficiency: implications for teaching

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 October 2012

Joanne Devine
Affiliation:
Skidmore College, Saratoga Springs, New York
Get access

Summary

Rosenblatt (1978), Widdowson (1979), and others have argued persuasively that successful reading is an act of creation: the reader creates meaning through the interaction with a text. In this view, the meaning of a text does not reside in a fixed, static form frozen within the words on the page. Rather, it emerges anew in each encounter of a reader with a text. A text, then, does not contain meaning as such but, as Widdowson suggests, potential for meaning, which readers, both native and nonnative, will realize in varying degrees. This ability to create meaning (what is usually referred to as comprehension) depends critically on, and in fact may be said to presuppose, another kind of interaction – that of various types of information the reader brings to the reading task and information available in the text itself. (See Grabe, Chapter 4 of this volume, for a full treatment of the various senses of the term interactive.) Eskey (1986) identifies two categories of knowledge that must interact if there is to be “full, or at least native-like, comprehension of written texts” (p. 17): knowledge of form and knowledge of substance. Formal knowledge includes recognition of graphophonic, lexical, syntactic/ semantic, and rhetorical patterns of a language; knowledge of substance encompasses cultural, pragmatic and subject-specific information.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 1988

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×