Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-84b7d79bbc-g5fl4 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-25T12:55:27.082Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

8 - The development of auxiliary DO

from PART II - INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES AND AUXILIARY VERB LEARNING IN SEVEN CHILDREN

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 September 2009

Brian J. Richards
Affiliation:
University of Reading
Get access

Summary

Introduction

A dimension on which children might be expected to differ is the extent to which auxiliaries remain context restricted, or are applied to a variety of available contexts fairly quickly. In certain cases context-restricted usage would also indicate unanalysed usage. This is amenable to investigation by examining the set of main verbs with which specified auxiliary forms, or subforms co-occur. The forms chosen must, however, occur with sufficient regularity to allow comparisons both between children, and within children over time. For this reason, Chapters 8 and 9 concentrate on do and can as the forms which occur most frequently.

doas an operator

In standard English, forming the negative and in most cases the interrogative of main verbs requires do-insertion (see Section 1.5). Auxiliary do, therefore, plays a central role in the development of negation and questions. Like other auxiliaries, it is also available for emphasis and ellipsis. It can be supplied as a means of stressing the propositional truth of the predicate of a clause containing no auxiliary (# ‘He went out’ → # ‘He 'did go out’) and functions as an operator (with or without emphasis) where there is ellipsis of the main verb (# ‘He did’).

Unstressed forms

As a consequence of the limitation of auxiliary do to the NICE operations and its lack of independent propositional content, its paradigm is deficient in comparison with that of other auxiliaries; there is no place for do forms which are unstressed, declarative and affirmative (UDAs). Palmer (1965) suggests that such forms occur in ‘code’ (ellipsis) but their existence is difficult to substantiate.

Type
Chapter
Information
Language Development and Individual Differences
A Study of Auxiliary Verb Learning
, pp. 113 - 125
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 1990

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×