Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
  • Cited by 10
Publisher:
Cambridge University Press
Online publication date:
November 2016
Print publication year:
2016
Online ISBN:
9781316665619

Book description

Every person who encounters or learns another language is faced with the challenge of processing many new words in a short period of time. What are the conditions under which a learner can acquire those words successfully and process them to long-term memory? How do language and memory faculties interact? What role do the senses play in this process? Are factors such as age or individual backgrounds of learners to be considered? Taking the reader on a fascinating journey through the brain to demonstrate how language is processed, Ulf Schütze explains the dynamic environment involved in recording and producing words. The book also discusses how to use information technology, such as Apps, to make the learning of words entertaining and efficient.

Refine List

Actions for selected content:

Select all | Deselect all
  • View selected items
  • Export citations
  • Download PDF (zip)
  • Save to Kindle
  • Save to Dropbox
  • Save to Google Drive

Save Search

You can save your searches here and later view and run them again in "My saved searches".

Please provide a title, maximum of 40 characters.
×

Contents

Bibliography

Abrahams, J. P., and Camp, C. J. (1993). Maintenance and generalization of object naming training in anomia associated with degenerative dementia. Clinical Gerontologist, 12(3), 5772.
Aitchison, J. (1992). Slip of the Tongue. In T. McArthur (ed.), The Oxford Companion to the English Language (p. 114). Oxford University Press.
Aitchison, J. (2003). Words in the Mind: An Introduction to the Mental Lexicon. Oxford: Blackwell.
Aleman, A. (2013). Wenn das Gehirn älter wird. [When the Brain Ages.] Munich: Beck.
Alloway, T. P. (2006). How does working memory work in the classroom? Educational Research and Reviews, 1(4), 134–39.
Allport, A., Styles, E. A., and Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting attentional set: exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In Umilta, C. and Moscovitch, M. (eds.), Attention and Performance, vol. 15, Conscious and Nonconscious Information Processing (pp. 421–62). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Altarriba, J., Bauer, L., and Benvenuto, C. (1999). Concreteness, context-availability, and imageability ratings and word associations for abstract, concrete, and emotion words. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 31, 578602.
Altarriba, J., and Isurin, L. (eds.) (2013). Memory, Language and Bilingualism. Cambridge University Press.
Amaral, D., Prince, J., Pitkanen, A., and Carmichael, S. (1992). Anatomical organization of the primate amygdaloid complex. In Aggleton, J. P. (ed.), The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Dysfunction (pp. 166). New York: Wiley.
Anderson, M. C., Bjork, R. A., and Bjork, E. L. (1994). Remembering can cause forgetting: retrieval dynamics in long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 20, 1063–87.
Archibald, J. (1998). Second Language Phonology. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Archibald, L. M. D., and Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Short-term and working memory in specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 41(6), 675–93.
Archibald, L. M. D., and Gathercole, S. E. (2007). Nonword repetition and serial recall: equivalent measures of verbal short-term memory? Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 587606.
Awh, E., Jonides, J., Smith, E. E., Schumacher, E. H., Koeppe, R. A., and Katz, S. (1996). Dissociation of storage and rehearsal in verbal working memory: evidence from positron emission tomography. Psychological Science, 7, 2531.
Baddeley, A. D. (1966a). Short-term memory for word sequences as a function of acoustic, semantic and formal similarity. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 362–65.
Baddeley, A. D. (1966b). The influence of acoustic and semantic similarity on long-term memory for word sequences. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 302–09.
Baddeley, A. D. (1966c). The capacity of generating information by randomization. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 119–29.
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working Memory. Oxford University Press.
Baddeley, A. D. (1999). Essentials of Human Memory. Hove: Psychology Press.
Baddeley, A. D. (2007). Working Memory, Thought, and Action. Oxford University Press.
Baddeley, A. D., Eysenck, M. W., and Anderson, M. C. (2009). Memory. Oxford: Psychology Press.
Baddeley, A. D., Gathercole, S. E., and Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as language learning device. Psychological Review, 105, 158–73.
Baddeley, A. D., and Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In Bower, G. H. (ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory (vol. 8, pp. 4789). New York: Academic Press.
Baddeley, A. D., Papagno, C., and Vallar, G. (1988). When long-term learning depends on short-term storage. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 586–95.
Baddeley, A. D., Thomson, N., and Buchanan, M. (1975). Word length and the structure of short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 14, 575–89.
Balota, D., Duchek, J., and Logan, J. (2007). Is expanded retrieval practice a superior form of spaced retrieval? A critical review of the extant literature. In Nairne, J. (ed.), The Foundations of Remembering: Essays in Honor of Henry L. Roediger (vol. 3, pp. 83105). New York: Psychology Press.
Balota, D. A., Duchek, J. M., Sergent-Marshall, S. D., and Roediger, H. L. (2006). Does expanded retrieval produce benefits over equal-interval spacing? Explorations of spacing effects inh aging and early stage Alzheimer’s disease. Psychology and Aging, 21(1), 1931.
Barcroft, J. (2004). Effects of sentence writing in second language lexical acquisition. Second Language Research, 20(4), 303–34.
Barcroft, J. (2015). Can retrieval opportunities increase vocabulary learning during reading? Foreign Language Annals, 48(2), 236–49.
Barnes, J. M., and Underwood, B. (1959). “Fate” of first-list associations in transfer theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 97105.
Basso, A. H., Spinnler, G., Vallar, G., and Zanobio, E. (1982). Left hemisphere damage and selective impairment of auditory verbal short-term memory: a case study. Neuropsychologica, 20, 263–74.
Bauer, P. J. (2004). Getting explicit memory off the ground: steps toward construction of a neuro-developmental account of changes in the first two years of life. Developmental Review, 24, 347–73.
Bloom, I., and La Heij, W. (2003). Semantic facilitation and semantic interference in word translation: implications for models of lexical access in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 468–88.
Bishop, D., Adams, C., and Norbury, C. (2004). Using nonword repetition to distinguish genetic and environmental influences on early literacy development: a study of 6-year-old twins. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 129B, 9496.
Buchsbaum, B. R., and D’Esposito, M. (2008). The search for the phonological store: from loop to convolution. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(5), 762–78.
Burgess, N., and Hitch, G. J. (1999). Memory for serial order: a network model of the phonological loop and its timing. Psychological Review, 106, 551–81.
Carpenter, S. K., and DeLosh, E. L. (2005). Application of the testing and spacing effects to name learning. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19, 619–36.
Cenoz, J., Hufeisen, B., and Jessner, U. (eds.) (2003). The Multilingual Lexicon. Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Chen, L., Shu, H., Liu, Y., Zhao, J., and Li, P. (2007). ERP signatures of subject-verb agreement in L2 learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10(2, 161–74.
Cheung, H. (1996). Non-word span as a unique predictor of second language vocabulary learning. Developmental Psychology, 32(5), 867–73.
Clark, E. V. (1973). What’s in a word? On the child’s acquisition of semantics in his first language. In Moore, T. E. (ed.), Cognitive Development and the Acquisition of Language (pp. 65110). New York: Academic Press.
Cohen, J. D., Perlstein, W. M., Braver, T. S., Nystrom, L. E., Noll, D. C., Jonides, J., and Smith, E. E. (1997). Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task. Nature, 386, 604–07.
Cook, V., and Singleton, D. (2014). Key Topics in Second Language Acquisition. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Cowan, N. (1992). Verbal memory span and the timing of spoken recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 668–84.
Cowan, N. (1993). Activation, attention, and short-term memory. Memory & Cognition, 21(2), 162–67.
Cowan, N. (1999). An embedded-processes model of working memory. In Miyaki, A. and Shah, P. (eds.), Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control (pp. 62101). Cambridge University Press.
Craik, F., and Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: a framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 11, 671–84.
Cull, W. (2000). Untangling the benefits of multiple study opportunities and repeated testing for cued recall. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14(3), 215–35.
Davidson, M., Amso, D., Aderson, L. C., and Diamond, A. (2006). Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia, 44(11), 2037–78.
de Bot, K. (1992). A bilingual production model: Levelt’s “speaking” model adapted. Applied Linguistics, 13, 124.
de Bot, K. (2004). The multilingual lexicon: modelling selection and control. International Journal of Multilingualism, 1, 1732.
de Bot, K., Lowie, W., and Verspoor, M. (2007). A Dynamic Systems Theory approach to second language acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 10(1), 721.
de Bot, K., and Schreuder, R. (1993). Word production and the bilingual lexicon. In Schreuder, R and Weltens, B. (eds.), The Bilingual Lexicon (pp. 191214). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Deci, E. L., and Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination in Human Behavior. New York: Plenum.
Deci, E. L., and Ryan, R. M. (eds.) (2002). Handbook of Self-Determination Research. Rochester: University of Rochester Press.
De Groot, A. M. B. (1992). Bilingual Lexical Representation: A Closer Look at Conceptual Representations. In Frost, R. and Katz, L. (eds.), Orthography, Phonology, Morphology, and Meaning (pp. 389412). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.
De Groot, A. M. B. (2006). Effects of stimulus characteristics and background music on foreign language vocabulary learning and forgetting. Language Learning, 56(3), 463506.
DeLange, R. J., Kemp, R. G., Riley, W. D., Cooper, R. A., and Krebs, E. (1968). Activation of skeletal muscle phosphorylase kinase by adenosine triphosphate and adenosine 3’, 5’-monophosphate. Journal of Biological Chemistry, 243(9), 2200–08.
Dell, G. S. (1988). The retrieval of phonological forms in production: tests of predictions from a connectionist model. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 124–42.
Dell, G. S., and Reich, P. (1981). Stages in sentence production: an analysis of speech error data. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 20(6), 611–29.
Diamond, A. (2002). Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young: adulthood cognitive functions, anatomy, and biochemistry. In Stuss, D. T. and Knight, R. T. (eds.), Principles of Frontal Lobe Function (pp. 466503). New York: Oxford University Press.
Diamond, A., and Taylor, C. (1996). Development of an aspect of executive control: development of the abilities to remember what I said and to “Do as I say, not as I do.” Developmental Psychobiology, 29(4), 315–34.
Dijkstra, T., and Van Heuven, W. (1998). The BIA model and bilingual word recognition. In Grainger, J. and Jacobs, A. M. (eds.), Localist Connectionist Approaches to Human Cognition (pp. 189225). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Dijkstra, T., and Van Heuven, W. (2002). The architecture of the bilingual word recognition system: from identification to decision. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 5, 191–98.
Doughty, C. J. (2003). Instructed SLA: constraints, compensation, and enhancement. In Doughty, C. J. and Long, M. L. (eds.). The Handbook of Second Language Acquisition (pp. 256310). Oxford: Blackwell.
Draaisma, D. (2004). Why Life Speeds Up as You Get Older. Cambridge University Press.
Ebbinghaus, H. (1913). Grundzüge der Psychologie. [Fundamentals of Psychology.] Leipzig: Viet.
Ellis, N. (1996). Phonological memory, chunking, and points of order. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18, 91126.
Fell, J., Klaver, P., Lehnertz, K., Grunwald, T., Schaller, C., Elger, C., and Fernández, G., (2001). Human memory formation is accompanied by rhinal-hippocampal coupling and decoupling. Nature Neuroscience, 4(12), 1259–64.
French, L., and O’Brien, I. (2008). Phonological memory and children’s second language grammar learning. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29(3), 463–87.
Fromkin, V. (1973). Slips of the tongue. In Fromkin, V. (ed.), Speech Errors as Linguistic Evidence. The Hague: Mouton.
Furnham, A., and Bradley, A. (1997). Music while you work: the differential distraction of background music on the cognitive test performance of introverts and extraverts. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 11, 445–55.
Garagnani, M., Wennekers, T., and Pulvermüller, F. (2006). A neuronal model of the language cortex. Neurocomputing, 70, 1914–19.
Gathercole, S. E., and Alloway, T. P. (2008). Working Memory and Learning: A Practical Guide for Teachers. London: Sage Publications.
Gathercole, S. E., and Baddeley, A. D. (1989). Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: a longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 200–13.
Gathercole, S. E., and Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language-disordered children: is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336–60.
Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Ambridge, B., and Wearing, H. (2004). The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Psychology, 40, 177–90.
Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Hall, M., and Peaker, S. (2001). Dissociable lexical and phonological influences on serial recognition and serial recall. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 130.
Gergely, Z. (2007). Selection of grammatical morphemes in early bilingual development. In Lengyel, Z. and Navracsics, J. (eds.), Second Language Lexical Process (pp. 133–45). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Gleason, J. B. (ed.) (2001). The Development of Language. Boston: Pearson, Allyn and Bacon.
Gluck, M., and Myers, C. (2001). Gateway to Memory: An Introduction to Neural Network Modeling of the Hippocampus in Learning and Memory. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Green, D. W. (1986). Control, activation, and resource. Brain and Language, 27, 210–23.
Grosjean, F., and Li, P. (2013). The Psycholinguistics of Bilingualism. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.
Hasselhorn, M. (2005). Lernpotential Arbeitsgedächtnis. Wie verändert es sich him Verlauf der menschlichen Lebensphase? [Learner potential working memory. How does it change during the course of human life spans?] In Göttingen, Akademie der Wissenchaften zu (ed.), Jahrbuch der Akademie der Wissenchaften zu Göttingen (pp. 302–07). Göttingen: Vandenhoeck and Ruprecht.
Hebb, D. O. (1949). The Organization of Behavior. New York: Wiley.
Hebb, D. O. (1961). Distinctive features of learning in the higher animal. In Delafresnaye, J. F. (ed.), Brain Mechanisms and Learning (pp. 3746). Oxford: Blackwell.
Herculano-Houzel, S. (2009). The human brain in numbers: a linearly scaled-up primate brain. Frontiers Human Neuroscience, 3, 31.
Hulstijn, J. H. (2001). Intentional and incidental second-language vocabulary learning: a reappraisal of elaboration, rehearsal and automaticity. In Robinson, P. (ed.), Cognition and Second Language Instruction (pp. 258–86). Cambridge University Press.
Hulstijn, J. H., and Laufer, B. (2001). Some empirical evidence for the involvement load hypothesis in vocabulary acquisition. Language Learning, 51(3), 539–58.
Indefrey, P., and Levelt, W. J. M. (2004). The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components. Cognition, 92, 101–44.
Ittner, L. M., Ke, Y., Delerue, F., Bi, M., Gladbach, A., van Eersel, J. Wölfing, H., Chieng, B., Christie, M., Napier, I., Eckert, A., Staufenbiel, M., Hardeman, E., and Götz, J. (2010). Dendritic function of tau mediates amyloid-beta toxicity in Alzheimer’s disease mouse models. Cell, 142(3), 387–97.
Jacobs, B. (1988). Neurobiological differentiation of primary and secondary language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 10, 303–37.
Jiang, N. (2000). Lexical representation and development in a second language. Applied Linguistics, 21(1), 4777.
Jones, L. C., and Plass, J. L. (2002). Supporting listening comprehension and vocabulary acquisition in French with multimedia annotations. Modern Language Journal, 86(4), 546–61.
Jones, R. L., and Tschirner, E. (2006). A Frequency Dictionary of German. London: Routledge.
Kandel, E. R. (2006). In Search of Memory. The Emergence of a New Science of Mind. New York: W. W. Norton & Co.
Kim, D., and Gilman, D. (2008). Effects of text, audio, and graphic aids in multimedia instruction for vocabulary learning. Educational Technology and Society, 11(3), 114–26.
Kim, K. H., Relkin, N. R., Lee, K.-M., and Hirsch, J. (1997). Distinct cortical areas associated with native and second languages. Nature, 388, 171–74.
Koenigs, M., Acheson, D., Barbey, A., Solomon, J., Postle, B., and Grafman, J. (2011). Areas of left perisylvian cortex mediate auditory-verbal short-term memory. Neuropsychologia, 49, 3612–19.
Kornell, N., and Bjork, R. A. (2010). Optimising self-regulated study: the benefits – and costs – of dropping flashcards. Memory, 16(2), 125–36.
Korte, M. (2009). Wie Kinder heute lernen. [How Children Learn Today.] Munich: Deutsche Verlags-Anstalt.
Krebs, E. G., DeLange, R. J., Kemp, R. G., and Riley, W. D. (1966). Activation of skeletal muscle phosphorylase. Pharmacology Review, 18, 163–71.
Kroll, J. F., and Dijkstra, T. (2010). The bilingual lexicon. In Kaplan, R. B. (ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Applied Linguistics (pp. 349–72). Oxford University Press.
Kroll, J., Michael, E., Tokowicz, N., and Dufour, R. (2002). The development of lexical fluency in a second language. Second Language Research, 18, 137–71.
Kroll, J., and Stewart, E. (1994). Category interference in translation and picture naming: evidence for asymmetric connections between bilingual memory representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 149–74.
Kupfermann, I. and Kandel, E. R. (1969) Neuronal controls of behavioral response mediated by the abdominal ganglion of Aplysia. Science, 164, 847–50.
Landauer, T., and Bjork, R. (1978). Optimal rehearsal patterns and name learning. In Grüneberg, M., Morris, P., and Sykes, R. (eds.), Practical Aspects of Memory (pp. 625–32). London: Academic Press.
Larsen-Freeman, D. (1997). Chaos/complexity science and second language acquisition. Applied Linguistics, 18, 141–65.
Larsen-Freeman, D., and Cameron, L. (2008). Complex Systems and Applied Linguistics. Oxford University Press.
Laufer, B. (1997). What’s in a word that makes it hard or easy: some intralexical factors that affect the learning of words. In Schmitt, N. and McCarthy, M. (eds.), Vocabulary. Description, Acquisition, and Pedagogy (pp. 140–55). Cambridge University Press.
Leitner, S. (1972). So lernt man Lernen: Der Weg zum Erfolg. [Learning to Learn: The Road to Success.] Freiburg: Herder.
Lenneberg, E. (1967). Biological Foundations of Language. New York: Wiley and Sons.
Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From Intention to Articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Levelt, W. J. M. (1992). Accessing words in speech production: stages, processes and representations. Cognition, 42, 122.
Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., and Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 175.
Levitin, D. J. (2008). The World in Six Songs: How the Musical Brain Created Human Nature. Toronto: Penguin Canada.
Logan, J. M., and Balota, D. A. (2008). Expanded vs. equal interval spaced retrieval practice: exploring different schedules of spacing and retention interval in younger and older adults. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 15(3), 257–80.
Logie, R. H., Cocchini, G., Della Sala, S., and Baddeley, A. (2004). Is there a specific capacity for dual-task co-ordination? Evidence from Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology, 18, 504–13.
López-Jiménez, M. D. (2010). The treatment of lexical aspects in commercial textbooks for L2 teaching and learning. In Chacón-Beltrán, R., Abello-Contesse, C., and del Mar Torreblance-López, M. (eds.), Insights into Non-native Vocabulary Teaching and Learning (pp. 156–74). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
Mackey, A., Adams, R., Stafford, C., and Winke, P. (2010). Exploring the relationship between modified output and working memory capacity. Language Learning, 60(3), 501–33.
Meara, P. (1984). The study of lexis in interlanguage. In Daves, A., Criper, C., and Howatt, A. (eds.), Interlanguage (pp. 225–39). Edinburgh University Press.
Meara, P. (2009). Connected Words: Word Associations and Second Language Vocabulary Acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Merriënboer, J. J. G., and Sweller, J. (2005). Cognitive load theory and complex learning: recent developments and future directions. Educational Psychology Review, 17(2), 147–77.
Miller, P. (2002). Theories of Developmental Psychology, 4th ed. New York: Worth Publishers.
Milner, B., Squire, L. R., and Kandel, E. R. (1998). Cognitive neuroscience and the study of memory. Neuron, 20(3), 445–68.
Milton, J. (2009). Measuring Second Language Vocabulary Acquisition. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
Milton, J., Jonsen, S., Hirst, S., and Lindenburn, S. (2012). Foreign language vocabulary development through activities in an online 3D environment. Language Learning Journal, 40(1), 99112.
Molnár, H. (2010). Der Einfluss des Faktors Alter auf die Aussprachekompetenz in der L2: Ergebnisse einer Pilotstudie mit DaZ-Lernern. [The influence of the factor age on the competence of pronounciation in L2: results of a pilot study with DaZ-learners.] Zeitschrift für Interkulturellen Fremdsprachenunterricht, 15(1), 19. Available at http://tujournals.ulb.tu-darmstadt.de/index.php/zif/index
Mondria, J., and Mondria-De Vries, S. (1994). Efficiently memorizing words with the help of word cards and “hand computer”: theory and applications. System, 22(1), 4757.
Mueller, S. T., Seymour, T. L., Kieras, D. E., and Meyer, D. E. (2003). Theoretical implications of articulatory duration, phonological similarity, and phonological complexity in verbal working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(6), 1353–80.
Müller, N., and Knight, R. (2006). The functional neuroanatomy of working memory contributions of human brain lesion studies. Neuroscience, 139, 5158.
Murray, D. J. (1968). Articulation and acoustic confusability in short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 78, 679–84.
Muzur, A., Pace-Schott, E. F., and Hobson, J. A. (2002). The prefrontal cortex in sleep. Trends in Cognitive Science, 6(11), 475–81.
Myers-Scotton, C. (2003). Code-switching: evidence for both flexibility and rigidity in language. In Dewaele, J.-M., Housen, A., and Wei, Li (eds.), Bilingualism: Beyond Basic Principles (pp. 189203). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Myers-Scotton, C., and Jake, J. (2000). Four types of morpheme: evidence from aphasia, code switching, and second language acquisition. Linguistics, 38, 1053–100.
Nagy, N. (1997). Modeling contact-induced language change. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics, 25(4), 399418.
Nation, I. S. P. (2001). Learning Vocabulary in Another Language. Cambridge University Press.
Nation, I. S. P. (2009). Teaching Vocabulary: Strategies and Techniques. Boston: Heinle.
Neary-Sundquist, C. (2015). Aspects of vocabulary knowledge in German textbooks. Foreign Language Annals, 48(1), 6881.
Neville, H. J., Mills, D. M., and Lawson, D. S. (1992). Fractionating language: different neural subsystems with different sensitive periods. Cerebral Cortex, 2(3), 244–58.
O’Connor, M., Butters, N., and Miliotis, P. (1992). The dissociation of anterograde and retrograde amnesia in a patient with herpes encephalitis. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 14, 159–78.
Oxford, R. L. (1990). Language Learning Strategies: What Every Teacher Should Know. Boston: Heinle.
Oxford, R. L. (2003). Language learning styles and strategies: concepts and relationships. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 41, 271–78.
Page, M. P. A. and Norris, D. (1998). The primacy model: a new model of immediate serial recall. Psychological Review, 105, 761–81.
Papagno, C., Valentine, T., and Baddeley, A. D. (1991). Phonological short-term memory and foreign-language vocabulary learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 331–47.
Papagno, C., and Vallar, G. (1992). Verbal short-term memory and the learning of novel words: the effects of phonological similarity and item length. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44A, 4767.
Paradis, M. (1994). Neurolinguistic aspects of implicit and explicit memory: implications for bilingualism and SLA. In Ellis, N. (ed.), Implicit and Explicit Learning of Languages (pp. 393419). London: Academic Press.
Pavlenko, A. (2009). Conceptual representation in the bilingual lexicon and second language vocabulary learning. In Pavlenko, A. (ed.), The Bilingual Mental Lexicon. Interdisciplinary Approaches (pp. 125–60). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Petersen, S. E., Fox, P. T., Posner, M. I., Mintun, M., and Raichle, M. E. (1989). Positron emission tomographic studies of the processing of single words. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(2), 153–70.
Piaget, J. (1955). The Construction of Reality in the Child. New York: Basic Books.
Piaget, J. (1962). The Language and Thought of the Child. London: Routledge.
Pickering, S. J., and Gathercole, S. E. (2001). The Working Memory Test Battery for Children. Oxford: Psychological Corporation.
Pimsleur, P. (1967). A memory schedule. Modern Language Journal, 51, 7375.
Pollock, E., Chandler, P., and Sweller, J. (2002). Assimilating complex information. Learning and Instruction, 12, 6186.
Port, R. (2007). How are words stored in memory? Beyond phones and phonemes. New Ideas in Psychology, 25, 143–70.
Postle, B. R. (2006). Working memory as an emergent property of the mind and brain. Neuroscience, 139, 2338.
Pulvermüller, F. (1996). Hebb’s concept of cell assemblies and the psychophysiology of word processing. Psychophysiology, 33, 317–33.
Pulvermüller, F. (1999). Words in the brain’s language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 253–79.
Pulvermüller, F. (2000). Cell assemblies, axonal conduction times, and the interpretation of high-frequency dynamics in the EEG and MEG. In Miller, R. (ed.), Time and the Brain (pp. 241–49). Newark, NJ: Harwood Academic.
Pulvermüller, F., Shtyrov, Y. Ilmoniemi, R., and Marslen-Wilson, W. (2006). Tracking speech comprehension in space and time. NeuroImage, 31, 1297–305.
Raichle, M. (2010). Two views of brain function. Trends in Cognitive Science, 14(4), 180–90.
Raichle, M. (2011). The restless brain. Brain Connectivity, 1, 312.
Raichle, M., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A., Powers, W., Gusnard, D., and Shulman, G. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 98(2), 676–82.
Richmond, J. and Nelson, C. A. (2007). Accounting for change in declarative memory: a cognitive neuroscience perspective. Developmental Review, 27, 349–73.
Rimrott, A. (2010). Computer-assisted vocabulary learning: multimedia annotations, word concreteness, and individualized instruction. Ph.D. thesis dissertation, Simon Fraser University. Available at http://summit.sfu.ca/item/11876.
Robinson, P. (2003). Attention and memory during SLA. In Doughty, C. J. and Long, M. H. (eds.), The Handbook of Second Language Acquisition (pp. 631–78). Oxford: Blackwell.
Robinson, B., and Mervis, C. (1998). Disentangling early language development: modeling lexical and grammatical acquisition using and extension of case-study methodology. Developmental Psychology, 34, 363–75.
Roediger, H. L., and Karpicke, J. D. (2010). Intricacies of spaced retrieval: a resolution. In Benjamin, A. S. (ed.), Successful Remembering and Successful Forgetting: Essays in Honor of Robert A. Bjork (pp. 136). New York: Psychology Press.
Rolke, B., Heil, M., Streb, J., and Henninghausen, E. (2001). Missed prime words within the attentional blink evoke an N400 semantic priming effect. Psychophysiology, 38, 165–74.
Rothbart, M. K., and Posner, M. I. (2005). Genes and experience in the development of executive attention and effortful control. In Jensen, L. A. and Larson, R. W.(eds.), New Horizons in Developmental Theory and Research (pp. 101–08). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Routtenberg, A., and Rekart, J. L. (2005). Post-translational protein modification as the substrate for long-lasting memory. Trends in Neurosciences, 28(1), 1219.
Schacter, D. L., Rich, S. A., and Stampp, M. S. (1985). Remediation of memory disorders: experimental evaluation of the spaced-retrieval technique. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 7(1), 7996.
Scheibel, A. B. (1992). Structural changes in the aging brain. In Birren, J. E., Sloane, R. B., and Cohen, G. (eds.), Handbook of Mental Health and Aging (pp. 147–73). New York: Academic Press.
Schmidt, R. (1995). Consciousness and foreign language learning: a tutorial on the role of attention and awareness in learning. In Schmidt, R. (ed.). Attention and Awareness in Foreign Language Learning (pp. 164). Honolulu: University of Hawai’i Press.
Schmidt, R. (2001). Attention. In Robinson, P. (ed.), Cognition and Second Language Instruction (pp. 332). Cambridge University Press.
Schmitt, N. (2010a). Key issues in teaching and learning vocabulary. In Chacón-Beltrán, R., Abello-Contesse, C., and del Mar Torreblance-López, M. (eds.), Insights into Non-native Vocabulary Teaching and Learning (pp. 2840). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Schmitt, N. (2010b). Researching Vocabulary. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.
Schütze, U. (2015). Spacing techniques in second language vocabulary acquisition: short-term gains vs. long-term memory. Language Teaching Research, 19(1), 2842.
Schütze, U., and Weimer-Stuckmann, G. (2010). Virtual vocabulary: research and learning in lexical processing. CALICO Journal, 27, 517–28.
Schuetze, U., and Weimer-Stuckmann, G. (2011). Retention in SLA processing. CALICO Journal, 28, 460–72.
Service, L. (1992) Phonology, working memory, and foreign-language learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45A, 2150.
Shallice, T. and Warrington, E. K. (1970). Independent functioning of verbal memory stores: a neuropsychological study. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 22, 261–73.
Singer, W. (2006). Vom Gehirn zum Bewußtsein. [From Mind to Consciousness.] Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp.
Singleton, D. (1995). Introduction: a critical look at the Critical Period Hypothesis in second language acquisition research. In Singleton, D. and Lengyel, Z. (eds.), The Age Factor in Second Language Acquisition: A Critical Look at the Critical Period Hypothesis (pp. 129). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Singleton, D. (2007). The Critical Period Hypothesis: some problems. Interlingüística, 17, 4856.
Skehan, P. (1998). A Cognitive Approach to Language Learning. Oxford University Press.
Skehan, P. (2009a). Modeling second language performance: integrating complexity, accuracy, fluency, and lexis. Applied Linguistics 30, 510–32.
Skehan, P. (2009b). Lexical performance by native and non-native speakers on language learning tasks. In Richards, B., Daller, H. M., Malvern, D., Meara, P., Milton, J., and Treffers-Daller, J. (eds), Vocabulary Studies in First and Second Language Acquisition: The Interface between Theory and Application (pp. 107–24). Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.
Skehan, P. (2012). Researching Tasks: Performance, Assessment, Pedagogy. Shanghai: Shanghai Foreign Language Education Press/Amsterdam: De Gruyter.
Skehan, P., and Foster, P. (1997). Task type and processing conditions as influences on foreign language performance. Language Teaching Research, 1(3), 185211.
Skehan, P., and Foster, P. (1999). The influence of task structure and processing conditions on narrative retellings. Language Learning, 49(1), 93120.
Skehan, P., and Foster, P. (2001). Cognition and tasks. In P. Robinson (ed.), Cognition and Second Language Learning (pp. 183205). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Sokoloff, L., Wechsler, R. L., Mangold, R., Balls, K., and Kety, S. S. (1953). Cerebral blood flow and oxygen consumption in hyperthyroidism before and after treatment. Journal of Clinical Investigation, 32, 202–08.
Speciale, G., Ellis, N., and Bywater, T. (2004). Phonological sequence learning and short-term store capacity determine second language vocabulary acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25(2), 293321.
Spencer, J. P., Clearfield, M., Corbetta, D., Ulrich, B., Buchanan, P., and Schöner, G. (2006). Moving toward a grand theory of development: in memory of Esther Thelen. Child Development, 77, 1521–38.
Spivey, M. (2008). The Continuity of Mind. Oxford University Press.
Stemmer, B. (2010). A cognitive neuroscience perspective on learning and memory in aging. Zeitschrift für Interkulturellen Fremdsprachenunterricht, 15(1), 725. Available at http://zif.spz.tu-darmstadt.de/
Szagun, G. (1996). Sprachentwicklung beim Kind, 6th ed. Weinheim: Beltz.
Tager-Flusberg, H. (2001). Putting words together. In Gleason, J. B. (ed.), The Development of Language (pp. 148–90). Boston: Pearson, Allyn and Bacon.
Thelen, E. (2005). Dynamic systems theory and the complexity of change. Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 15, 255–83.
Thomas, R. M. (2001). Connectionism and dynamic systems. In Recent Theories of Human Development. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Tranel, D., and Damasio, A. R. (2002). Neurobiological foundations of human memory. In Baddeley, A. D., Koppelman, M. D., and Wilson, B. A. (eds.), Handbook of Memory Disorders (2nd ed., pp. 1756). Chichester: Wiley.
Tranel, D., Damasio, H., and Damasio, A. R. (2000). Amnesia caused by herpes simplex encephalitis, infarctions in basal forebrain, and anoxia/ischemia. In Boller, F. and Grafman, J. (eds.), Handbook of Neuropsychology (2nd ed., pp. 85110). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.
Trojano, L, Stanzione, M., and Grossi, D. (1992). Short-term memory and verbal learning with auditory phonological coding defect: a neuropsychological case study. Brain & Cognition, 18, 1233.
Tschichold, C. (2012). French vocabulary in Encore Tricolore: do pupils have a chance? Language Learning Journal, 40(1), 719.
Turvey, M. T., Brick, P., and Osborne, J. (1970). Proactive interference in short-term memory as a function of prior-item retention interval. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 22, 142–47.
Vallar, G., and Baddeley, A. D. (1984). Fractionation of working memory: neuropsychological evidence for a phonological short-term store. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 23, 151–61.
Vallar, G., and Papagno, C. (1993). Preserved vocabulary acquisition in Down’s syndrome: the role of phonological short-term memory. Cortex, 29, 467–83.
Vallar, G., and Papagno, C. (2002). Neuropsychological impairments of verbal short-term memory. In Baddeley, A. D., Kopelman, M. D., and Wilson, B. A. (eds.), Handbook of Memory Disorders (2nd ed., pp. 249–70). Chichester: Wiley.
van Geert, P. (1991). A dynamic systems theory model of cognitive and language growth. Psychological Review, 98, 353.
van Geert, P. (2008). The dynamic systems approach in the study of L1 and L2 acquisition: an introduction. Modern Language Journal, 92(2), 179–99.
Wagner, A., Maril, A., and Schacter, D. (2000). Interactions between forms of memory: when priming hinders new learning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 5260.
Wagner, A., Schacter, D., Rotte, M., Koutstaal, W., Maril, A., Dale, A., Rosen, B., and Buckner, R. (1998). Building memories: remembering and forgetting of verbal experiences as predicted by brain activity. Science, 281, 1188–91.
Wartenburger, I., Heekeren, H. R., Abutalebi, J., Cappa, S. F., Villringer, A., and Perani, D. (2003). Early setting of grammatical processing in the bilingual brain. Neuron, 37, 159–70.
Webb, S. (2005). Receptive and productive vocabulary learning: the effects of reading and writing on word knowledge. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27, 3352.
Wilhelm, I., Diekelmann, S., Molzow, I., Ayoub, A., Mölle, M., and Born, J. (2011). Sleep selectively enhances memory expected to be of future relevance. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(5), 1563–69.
Willms, J., Shapiro, K., Peelen, M., Pajtas, P., Costa, A., Moo, L., and Caramazza, A. (2011). Language-invariant verb processing regions in Spanish-English bilinguals. Neuroimage, 57(1), 251–61.
Wilson, B. A., Baddeley, A., Evans, J., and Shiel, A. (1994). Errorless learning in the rehabilitation of memory impaired people. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 4, 307–26.
Wong, W., and Pyun, D. O. (2012). The effects of sentence writing on second language French and Korean lexical retention. Canadian Modern Language Review, 68(2), 164–89.
Zimmerman, C. B. (2009). Word Knowledge: A Vocabulary Teacher’s Handbook. Oxford University Press.

Metrics

Altmetric attention score

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Book summary page views

Total views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between #date#. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed.