Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-84b7d79bbc-7nlkj Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-29T04:30:54.490Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

38 - Culture

from Part VIII - Behavioral diversity

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 September 2015

Michio Nakamura
Affiliation:
Kyoto University, Japan
Kazuhiko Hosaka
Affiliation:
Kamakura Women’s University, Japan
Noriko Itoh
Affiliation:
Kyoto University, Japan
Koichiro Zamma
Affiliation:
Great Ape Research Institute
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Mahale Chimpanzees
50 Years of Research
, pp. 521 - 532
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2015

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Boesch, C. (1995). Innovation in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). International Journal of Primatology, 16, 116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonnie, K. E. and de Waal, F. B. M. (2006). Affiliation promotes the transmission of a social custom: handclasp grooming among captive chimpanzees. Primates, 47, 2734.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corp, N., Hayaki, H., Matsusaka, T., et al. (2009). Prevalence of muzzle-rubbing and hand-rubbing behavior in wild chimpanzees in Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Primates, 50, 184–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fragaszy, D. (2003). Making space for traditions. Evolutionary Anthropology, 12, 6170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hosaka, K. (1995). [Water play.] Monkey, 39(2), 20–1. In Japanese.Google Scholar
Huffman, M. A. and Kalunde, M. S. (1993). Tool-assisted predation on a squirrel by a female chimpanzee in the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. Primates, 34, 93–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imanishi, K. (1952). [Evolution of humanity.] In [Man], ed. Imanishi, K.. Tokyo: Mainichi-shinbun-sha, pp. 3694. In Japanese.Google Scholar
Ingold, T. (1996). Situating action VI: a comment on the distinction between the material and the social. Ecological Psychology, 8, 183–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ingold, T. (1998). Comments. Current Anthropology, 39, 606–7.Google Scholar
Itani, J. and Nishimura, A. (1973). The study of infrahuman culture in Japan. In Precultural Primate Behavior, ed. Menzel, E. W.. Basel: Karger, pp. 2650.Google Scholar
Itoh, N. (2013). [Dual-rings of mode of coexistence and act choices: Chimpanzee group and emergence of institution.] In [Practices, Conventions and Institutions: the Evolution of Human Sociality], ed. Kawai, K.. Kyoto: Kyoto University Press, pp. 143–66. In Japanese.Google Scholar
Itoh, N. and Nishida, T. (2007). Chimpanzee grouping patterns and food availability in Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Primates, 48, 8796.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krützen, M., van Schaik, C., and Whiten, A. (2007). The animal cultures debate: response to Laland and Janik. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 22, 6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laland, K. N. and Janik, V. M. (2006). The animal cultures debate. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 21, 542–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laland, K. N. and Janik, V. M. (2007). Response to Krützen et al.: further problems with the “method of exclusion”. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 22, 7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lévi-Strauss, C. (1967). Les Structures Élémentaires de la Parenté. The Hague: Mouton & Co and Maison des Sciences de l’Homme.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marchant, L. F. and McGrew, W. C. (1999). Innovative behavior at Mahale: new data on nasal probe and nipple press. Pan Africa News, 6, 1618.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsusaka, T. and Kutsukake, N. (2002). Use of leaf-sponge and leaf-spoon by juvenile chimpanzees at Mahale. Pan Africa News, 9, 69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsusaka, T., Nishie, H., Shimada, M., et al. (2006). Tool-use for drinking water by immature chimpanzees of Mahale: prevalence of an unessential behavior. Primates, 47, 113–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGrew, W. C. (1992). Chimpanzee Material Culture: Implications for Human Evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrew, W. C. (2004). The Cultured Chimpanzee: Reflections on Cultural Primatology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrew, W. C. and Tutin, C. E. G. (1978). Evidence for a social custom in wild chimpanzees? Man, 13, 234–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrew, W. C., Marchant, L. F., Scott, S. E., and Tutin, C. E. G. (2001). Intergroup differences in a social custom of wild chimpanzees: The grooming hand-clasp of the Mahale Mountains. Current Anthropology, 42, 148–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, M. (2002). Grooming-hand-clasp in Mahale M group chimpanzees: implication for culture in social behaviors. In Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos, ed. Boesch, C., Hohmann, G., and Marchant, L. F.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 7183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, M. (2003). [Questions about chimpanzee culture studies.] Ecosophia, 12, 5561. In Japanese.Google Scholar
Nakamura, M. (2010a). Ubiquity of culture and possible social inheritance of sociality among wild chimpanzees. In The Mind of the Chimpanzee, ed. Lonsdorf, E. V., Ross, S. R., and Matsuzawa, T.. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press, pp. 156–67.Google Scholar
Nakamura, M. (2010b). Poke-type social scratching persists at Mahale. Pan Africa News, 17, 1517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, M. and Itoh, N. (2008). Hunting with tools by Mahale chimpanzees. Pan Africa News, 15, 36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, M. and Nishida, T. (2006). Subtle behavioral variation in wild chimpanzees, with special reference to Imanishi’s concept of kaluchua. Primates, 47, 3542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, M. and Uehara, S. (2004). Proximate factors of different types of grooming hand-clasp in Mahale chimpanzees: implications for chimpanzee social custom. Current Anthropology, 45, 108–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, M., McGrew, W. C., Marchant, L. F., and Nishida, T. (2000). Social scratch: another custom in wild chimpanzees? Primates, 41, 5971.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nishida, T. (1968). The social group of wild chimpanzees in the Mahali Mountains. Primates, 9, 167224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. (1973). The ant-gathering behaviour by the use of tools among wild chimpanzees of the Mahali Mountains. Journal of Human Evolution, 2, 357–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. (1980a). Local differences in responses to water among wild chimpanzees. Folia Primatologica, 33, 189209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nishida, T. (1980b). The leaf-clipping display: a newly-discovered expressive gesture in wild chimpanzees. Journal of Human Evolution, 9, 117–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. (1983). Alloparental behavior in wild chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. Folia Primatologica, 41, 133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. (1987). Local traditions and cultural transmission. In Primate Societies, ed. Smuts, B. B., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Wrangham, R. W., and Struhsaker, T. T.. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press, pp. 462–74.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. (1994). Review of recent findings on Mahale chimpanzees: implications and future research directions. In Chimpanzee Cultures, ed. Wrangham, R. W., McGrew, W. C., de Waal, F. B. M., and Heltne, P. G.. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, pp. 373–96.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. (1997). Sexual behavior of adult male chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Primates, 38, 379–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. (2003). Individuality and flexibility of cultural behavior patterns in chimpanzees. In Animal Social Complexity: Intelligence, Culture, and Individualized Societies, ed. de Waal, F. B. M. and Tyack, P.. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, pp. 392413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. (2012). Chimpanzees of the Lakeshore: Natural History and Culture at Mahale. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. and Hiraiwa, M. (1982). Natural history of a tool-using behavior by wild chimpanzees in feeding upon wood-boring ants. Journal of Human Evolution, 11, 7399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. and Uehara, S. (1980). Chimpanzees, tools, and termites: another example from Tanzania. Current Anthropology, 21, 671–2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. and Uehara, S. (1983). Natural diet of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii): long-term record from the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. African Study Monographs, 3, 109–30.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. and Wallauer, W. (2003). Leaf-pile pulling: an unusual play pattern in wild chimpanzees. American Journal of Primatology, 60, 167–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nishida, T., Wrangham, R. W., Goodall, J., and Uehara, S. (1983). Local differences in plant-feeding habits of chimpanzees between the Mahale Mountains and Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Journal of Human Evolution, 12, 467–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T., Kano, T., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., and Nakamura, M. (1999). Ethogram and ethnography of Mahale chimpanzees. Anthropological Science, 107, 141–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T., Mitani, J. C., and Watts, D. P. (2004). Variable grooming behaviors in wild chimpanzees. Folia Primatologica, 75, 31–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nishida, T., Matsusaka, T., and McGrew, W. C. (2009). Emergence, propagation or disappearance of novel behavioral patterns in the habituated chimpanzees of Mahale: A review. Primates, 50, 2336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nishida, T., Zamma, K., Matsusaka, T., Inaba, A., and McGrew, W. C. (2010). Chimpanzee Behavior in the Wild. Tokyo: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishie, H. (2008). [Chimpanzee culture and sociality: reconsidering “the metaphor of transmission of knowledge”.] Primate Research, 24, 7390. In Japanese with English abstract.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishie, H. (2011). Natural history of Camponotus ant fishing by the M group chimpanzees at the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Primates, 52, 329–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishie, H. (2012). [Chimpanzee Culture and Sociality.] Doctoral Dissertation. Kyoto: Kyoto University. In Japanese.Google Scholar
Sakamaki, T. (1998). First record of algae-feeding by a female chimpanzee at Mahale. Pan Africa News, 5, 13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sakamaki, T., Nakamura, M., and Nishida, T. (2007). Evidence of cultural differences in diet between two neighboring unit groups of chimpanzees in Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Pan Africa News, 14, 35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shimada, M. (2002). Social scratch among chimpanzees in Gombe. Pan Africa News, 9, 21–3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sperber, D. (1982). Le Savoir des Anthropologues. Paris: Hermann.Google Scholar
Takahata, Y. (1982). Termite fishing observed in M group chimpanzees. Mahale Mountains Chimpanzee Research Project Ecological Report, 18.Google Scholar
Takahata, Y., Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, M., Takasaki, H., and Nyundo, R. (1986). Newly acquired feeding habits among the chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Human Evolution, 1, 277–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Takasaki, H. (1983). Mahale chimpanzees taste mangoes: toward acquisition of a new food item? Primates, 24, 273–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uehara, S. (1982). Seasonal change in the techniques employed by wild chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania, to feed on termites (Pseudacanthotermes spiniger). Folia Primatologica, 37, 4476.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Waal, F. B. M. (1999). Cultural primatology comes of age. Nature, 399, 635–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Waal, F. B. M. (2001). The Ape and the Sushi Master. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
de Waal, F. B. M. and Seres, M. (1997). Propagation of handclasp grooming among captive chimpanzees. American Journal of Primatology, 43, 339–46.3.0.CO;2-Y>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Washburn, S. and Benedict, B. (1979). Non-human primate culture. Man, 14, 163–4.Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., et al. (1999). Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature, 399, 682–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., et al. (2001). Charting cultural variation in chimpanzees. Behaviour, 138, 1481–516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wrangham, R. W., de Waal, F. B. M., and McGrew, W. C. (1994). The challenge of behavioral diversity. In Chimpanzee Cultures, ed. Wrangham, R. W., McGrew, W. C., de Waal, F. B. M., and Heltne, P. G.. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, pp.118.Google Scholar
Zamma, K. (2002). Leaf-grooming by a wild chimpanzee in Mahale. Primates, 43, 8790.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zamma, K. (2006). A louse egg left on a leaf. Pan Africa News, 13, 810.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×