Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-75dct Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-08T04:05:43.653Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Bibliography

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 April 2022

Norbert Sachser
Affiliation:
University of Münster
Get access
Type
Chapter
Information
Much Like Us
What Science Reveals about the Thoughts, Feelings, and Behaviour of Animals
, pp. 148 - 154
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Primary Sources

Darwin, C., The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. (Reprint) The University of Chicago Press, Chicago, London, 1965 (Original: 1872).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darwin, C., Die Entstehung der Arten. Neudruck Reclam, Stuttgart, 1981 (Original 1859).Google Scholar
Franck, D., Eine Wissenschaft im Aufbruch. Chronik der Ethologischen Gesellschaft 1949–2000. Verlag Niel & More, Hamburg, 2008.Google Scholar
Frisch, K. v., Tanzsprache und Orientierung der Bienen. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg, 1965.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Immelmann, K., Wörterbuch der Verhaltensforschung. Verlag Paul Parey, Berlin, Hamburg, 1982.Google Scholar
Kaiser, Friedrich der Zweite, Über die Kunst, mit Vögeln zu jagen. Insel-Verlag,Frankfurt, 1965.Google Scholar
Lorenz, K., Der Kumpan in der Umwelt des Vogels. Journal für Ornithologie 83: 137213 and 289413, 1935.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lorenz, K., Vergleichende Bewegungsstudien an Anatiden. Journal für Ornithologie 89: Ergänzungsband, 194293, 1941.Google Scholar
Naguib, M., Methoden der Verhaltensbiologie. Springer-Verlag, Berlin, Heidelberg, 2006.Google Scholar
Pfungst, O., Der kluge Hans (Nachdruck der Originalausgabe von 1907). Fachbuchhandlung für Psychologie, Frankfurt, 1983.Google Scholar
Tinbergen, N., The Study of Instinct. Oxford University Press, London, 1951.Google Scholar
Tinbergen, N., On the Aims and Methods of Ethology. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 20: 410433, 1963.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zippelius, H. M., Die vermessene Theorie. Friedr. Vieweg & Sohn Verlagsgesellschaft, Braunschweig, Wiesbaden, 1992.Google Scholar

Secondary Sources

Bradley, A. J., McDonald, I. R., Lee, A. K., Stress and Mortality in a Small Marsupial (Antechinus stuartii, Macleay). General and Comparative Endocrinology 40: 188200, 1980.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cannon, W. B., Bodily Changes in Pain, Hunger, Fear and Rage. Branford, Boston, 1929.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christian, J. J., Phenomena Associated with Population Density. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 47: 428449, 1961.Google Scholar
Gesquiere, L. R., Learn, N. H., Simao, , M. C. M., et al., Life at the Top: Rank and Stress in Wild Male Baboons. Science 333: 357360, 2011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hennessy, M. B., Kaiser, S., Sachser, N., Social Buffering of the Stress Response: Diversity, Mechanisms, and Functions. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 30: 470482, 2009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henry, J. P., Stephens, P. M., Stress, Health, and the Social Environment. A Sociobiologic Approach to Medicine. Springer, New York, 1977.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaplan, J. R., Manuck, S. B., Clarkson, T. B., Lusso, F. M., Taub, D. M., Social Status, Environment, and Atherosclerosis in Cynomolgus Monkeys. Arteriosclerosis 2: 359368, 1982.Google Scholar
Koolhaas, J. M., Korte, J. M., de Boer, S. F., et al., Coping Styles in Animals: Current Status in Behaviour and Stress-Physiology. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 23: 925935, 1999.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McEwen, B. S., Wingfield, J. C., The Concept of Allostasis in Biology and Biomedicine. Hormones and Behavior 43: 215, 2003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sachser, N., Dürschlag, M., Hirzel, D., Social Relationships and the Management of Stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology 23: 891904, 1998.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sachser, N., Kaiser, S., Meerschweinchen als Sozialstrategen. Spektrum der Wissenschaft January 2010: 56–63, 2010.Google Scholar
Selye, H., Stress. Acta, Montreal, 1950.Google ScholarPubMed
Von Holst, D., The Concept of Stress and Its Relevance for Animal Behaviour. Advances in the Study of Behavior 17: 1131, 1998.Google Scholar
Young, C., Majolo, B., Heistermann, M., Schülke, O., Ostner, J., Responses to Social and Environmental Stress Are Attenuated by Strong Male Bonds in Wild Macaques. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 111: 1819518200, 2014.Google Scholar
Broom, D. M., Johnson, K. G., Stress and Animal Welfare. London, 1993.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clubb, R., Mason, G., Animal Welfare: Captivity Effects on Wide-Ranging Carnivores. Nature 425: 473474, 2003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Current Biology, Biology of Fun, 25th Anniversary Special Issue, 1, R1R30, 2015.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawkins, M. S., From an Animal’s Point of View: Motivation, Fitness, and Animal Welfare. Behavioural and Brain Sciences 13: 19 and 5461, 1990.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, C. R., Prouvost, C., Jealousy in Dogs. PLoS One 9: 7, e94597, 2014.Google Scholar
Kaiser, S., Classen, D., Sachser, N., Auswirkungen unterschiedlicher struktureller Anreicherungen auf das Spontanverhalten weiblicher Labormäuse (Stamm NMRI). In Aktuelle Arbeiten zur artgemäßen Tierhaltung 1998. KTBL-Schrift 382: 5662, 1998.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J., Beyond a Joke: From Animal Laughter to Human Joy? Science 308: 6263, 2005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paul, E. S., Harding, E. J., Mendl, M., Measuring Emotional Processes in Animals: The Utility of a Cognitive Approach. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 29: 469491, 2005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richter, S. H., Sachser, N., Kaiser, S., Tiere und Emotionen. In Handbuch Tierethik, Ach, J. S., Borchers, D. (Eds.), J. B. Metzler, Stuttgart, 2018.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sachser, N., Sozialphysiologische Untersuchungen an Hausmeerschweinchen. Gruppenstrukturen, soziale Situation und Endokrinium, Wohlergehen. Verlag Paul Parey, Berlin and Hamburg, 1994.Google Scholar
Sachser, N., Was bringen Präferenztests? In Aktuelle Arbeiten zur artgemäßen Tierhaltung 1997 KTBL Schrift 380, Darmstadt, 920, 1998.Google Scholar
Sachser, N., What Is Important to Achieve Good Welfare in Animals? In Coping with Challenge: Welfare in Animals Including Humans, Broom, D. M. (Ed.), Dahlem Workshop Report 87, Dahlem University Press, Berlin, 3148, 2001.Google Scholar
Sachser, N., Neugier, Spiel und Lernen: Verhaltensbiologische Anmerkungen zur Kindheit. Zeitschrift für Pädagogik, 475486, 2004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sachser, N., Richter, S. H., Kaiser, S., Artgerecht – tiergerecht: eine biologische Perspektive. In Handbuch Tierethik, Ach, J. S., Borchers, D. (Eds.), J. B. Metzler, Stuttgart, 2018.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, C., Sachser, N., Auswirkungen unterschiedlicher Futterverteilungen auf Verhalten und Speichel-Streßhormonkonzentrationen von Breitmaulnashörnern im Allwetterzoo Münster. In Aktuelle Arbeiten zur artgemäßen Tierhaltung 1996. KTBL-Schrift 376: 188198, 1997.Google Scholar
Ambrée, O., Leimer, U., Herring, A., et al., Reduction of Amyloid Angiopathy and Aβ Plaque Burden after Enriched Housing of TgCRND8 Mice. The American Journal of Pathology 169: 544552, 2006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belsky, J., Jonassaint, C., Pluess, M., et al., Vulnerability genes or plasticity genes? Molecular Psychiatry 14: 746754, 2009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brunner, H. G., Nelen, M., Breakefield, X. O., Ropers, H. H., van Ost, B. A., Abnormal Behavior Associated with a Point Mutation in the Structural Gene for Monoamine Oxidase A. Science 262: 578580, 1993.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cases, O., Seif, I., Grimsby, J., et al., Aggressive Behaviour and Altered Amounts of Brain Serotonin and Norepinephrine in Mice Lacking MAOA. Science 268: 17631766, 1995.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., Sugden, K., Moffitt, T. E., et al., Influence of Life Stress on Depression: Moderation by a Polymorphism in the 5-HTT Gene. Science 301: 386389, 2003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, R. M., Zubek, J. P., Effects of Enriched and Restricted Early Environments on the Learning Ability of Bright and Dull Rats. Canadian Journal of Psychology 12: 159164, 1958.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dias, B. G., Ressler, K. J., Parental Olfactory Experience Influences Behaviour and Neural Structure in Subsequent Generations. Nature Neuroscience 17: 8996, 2014.Google Scholar
Epstein, R., Lanza, R. P., Skinner, B. F., Symbolic Communication Between Two Pigeons (Columba livia domestica). Science 207: 543545, 1980.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glocker, M. L., Langleben, D. D., Ruparel, K., et al., Baby Schema in Infant Faces Induces Cuteness Perception and Motivation for Caretaking in Adults. Ethology 115: 257263, 2009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glocker, M. L., Langleben, D. D., Ruparel, K., et al., Baby Schema Modulates the Brain Reward System in Nulliparous Women. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 106: 91159119, 2009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heiming, R. S., Jansen, F., Lewejohann, L., et al., Living in a Dangerous World: The Shaping of Behavioural Profile by Early Environment and 5-HTT Genotype. Frontiers in Behavioural Neuroscience 3: 26, 2009.Google Scholar
Immelmann, K., Pröve, E., Sossinka, R., Einführung in die Verhaltensforschung 4. Aufl., Wien, 1996.Google Scholar
Lewejohann, L., Reefmann, N., Widmann, P., et al., Transgenic Alzheimer Mice in a Semi Naturalistic Environment: More Plaques, Yet Not Compromised in Daily Life. Behavioural Brain Research 201: 99102, 2009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meaney, J. M., Maternal Care, Gene Expression, and the Transmission of Individual Differences in Stress Reactivity Across Generations. Annual Review of Neuroscience 24: 11611192, 2001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sachser, N., Lesch, K. P., Das Zusammenspiel von Genotyp und Umwelt bei der Entwicklung von Furcht und Angst. Neuroforum 3: 104109, 2013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L., Wie Affen sich verstehen. Spektrum der Wissenschaft 2: 8895, 1993.Google Scholar
Weaver, I. C. G., Cervoni, N., Champagne, F. A., et al., Epigenetic Programming by Maternal Behavior. Nature Neuroscience 7: 847854, 2004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brosnan, S. F., de Waal, F. B. M., Monkeys Reject Unequal Pay. Nature 425: 297299, 2003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bugnyar, T., Heinrich, B., Ravens, Corvus corax, Differentiate Between Knowledgeable and Ignorant Competitors. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 272: 16411646, 2005.Google Scholar
Emery, N. J., Clayton, N., The Mentality of Crows: Convergent Evolution of Intelligence in Corvids and Apes. Science 306: 19031907, 2004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallup, G. G., Chimpanzees: Self-Recognition. Science 167: 8687, 1970.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodall, J., Tool-Using and Aimed Throwing in a Community of Free-Living Chimpanzees. Nature 201: 1264–166, 1964.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffin, D. R., Animal Thinking. The Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, 1984.Google Scholar
Güntürkün, O., Bugnyar, T., Cognition Without Cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 20: 291303, 2016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hare, B., Call, J., Tomasello, M., Do Chimpanzees Know What Conspecifics Know? Animal Behaviour 61: 139151, 2001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hunt, G. R., Manufacture and Use of Hook-Tools by New Caledonian Crows. Nature 379: 249251, 1996.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Izawa, K., Die Affenkultur der Rotgesichtsmakaken. In: Grzimek, B. (Ed.), Grzimeks Enzyklopädie Säugetiere. Kindler Verlag, München, 286295, 1988.Google Scholar
Kaminski, J., Call, J., Fischer, J., Word Learning in a Domestic Dog: Evidence for ‘Fast Mapping’. Science 304: 16821683, 2004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Köhler, W., Intelligenzprüfungen an Menschenaffen. Unveränderter Nachdruck der 2. Aufl. von 1921. Springer, Berlin, Göttingen, Heidelberg, 1963.Google Scholar
Krupenye, C., Kano, F., Hirata, S., Call, J., Tomasello, M., Great Apes Anticipate That Other Individuals Will Act According to False Beliefs. Science 354: 110114, 2016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manning, A., Dawkins, M. S., Learning and Memory. In An Introduction to Animal Behaviour. Sixth ed., Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 2012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mendes, N., Hanus, D., Call, J., Raising the Level: Orangutans Use Water as Tool. Biology Letters 3: 453455, 2007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mercader, J., Barton, H., Gillespie, J., et al., 4300-Year-Old Chimpanzee Sites and the Origins of Percussive Stone Technology. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 104: 30433048, 2007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pawlow, I. P., Die bedingten Reflexe. Kindler Verlag, München, 1972.Google Scholar
Rensch, B., Döhl, J., Wahlen zwischen zwei überschaubaren Labyrinthwegen durch einen Schimpansen. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 25: 216231, 1968.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F., The Behavior of Organisms. Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York, 1938.Google Scholar
Van Schaik, C. P., Ancrenaz, M., Borgen, G., et al., Orangutan Cultures and the Evolution of Material Culture. Science 299: 102105, 2003.Google Scholar
Agrawal, A. A., Laforsch, C., Tollrian, R., Transgenerational Induction of Defences in Animals and Plants. Nature 401: 6063, 1999.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bateson, P., Gluckman, P., Hanson, M., The Biology of Developmental Plasticity and the Predictive Adaptive Response Hypothesis. Journal of Physiology 592: 23572368, 2014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dall, S. R. X., Houston, A. I., McNamara, J. M., The Behavioural Ecology of Personality: Consistent Individual Differences from an Adaptive Perspective. Ecology Letters 7: 734739, 2004.Google Scholar
Dingemanse, N. J., Bouwman, K. M., van de Pol, M., et al., Variation in Personality and Behavioural Plasticity Across Four Populations of Great Tit Parus major. Journal of Animal Ecology 81: 116126, 2012.Google Scholar
Freund, J., Brandmaier, A. M., Lewejohann, L., et al., Emergence of Individuality in Genetically Identical Mice. Science 340: 756759, 2013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harlow, H. F., Harlow, M., Social Deprivation in Monkeys. Scientific American 207: 136146, 1962.Google ScholarPubMed
Immelmann, K., Barlow, G., Petrinovitch, L., Main, M., Behavioral Development: the Bielefeld Interdisciplinary Project. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 1981.Google Scholar
Kaiser, S., Sachser, N., The Effects of Prenatal Stress on Behaviour: Mechanisms and Function. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 29: 283294, 2005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mousseau, T. A., Fox, C. W., The Adaptive Significance of Maternal Effects. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 13: 403407, 1998.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Réale, D., Reader, S. M., Sol, D., McDougall, P. T., Dingemanse, N. J., Integrating Animal Temperament Within Ecology and Evolution. Biological Reviews Cambridge Philosophical Society 82: 291318, 2007.Google Scholar
Sachser, N., Hennessy, M. B., Kaiser, S., Adaptive Modulation of Behavioural Profiles by Social Stress During Early Phases of Life and Adolescence. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 35: 15181533, 2011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sachser, N., Kaiser, S., Hennessy, M. B., Behavioural Profiles Are Shaped by Social Experiences: When, How and Why. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 368: DOI 201203344, 2013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sih, A., Bell, A. M., Johnson, J. C., Ziemba, R. E., Behavioral Syndromes: An Integrative Overview. Quarterly Review of Biology 79: 241277, 2004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spear, L. P., The Adolescent Brain and Age-Related Behavioral Manifestations. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 24: 417463, 2000.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trivers, R. L., Parent–Offspring Conflict. American Zoologist 14: 249264, 1974.Google Scholar
Zimmermann, T. D., Kaiser, S., Hennessy, M. B., Sachser, N., Adaptive Shaping of the Behavioural and Neuroendocrine Phenotype During Adolescence. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 2784: DOI 101098/rspb.2016, 2017.Google Scholar
Adrian, O., Brockmann, I., Hohoff, C., Sachser, N., Paternal Behaviour in Wild Guinea Pigs: A Comparative Study in Three Closely Related Species with Different Social and Mating Systems. Journal of Zoology (London) 265: 97105, 2005.Google Scholar
Adrian, O., Sachser, N., Diversity of Social and Mating Systems in Cavies. Journal of Mammalogy 92: 3953, 2011.Google Scholar
Alcock, J., Animal Behavior. Ninth ed., Sinauer, Sunderland, MA, 2009.Google Scholar
Bradbury, J. W., Andersson, M. B., Sexual Selection: Testing the Alternatives. Wiley, Chichester, 1987.Google Scholar
Carter, G. G., Wilkinson, G. S., Food Sharing in Vampire Bats: Reciprocal Help Predicts Donations More Than Relatedness or Harassment. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 280: DOI 20122573, 2013.Google Scholar
Clutton-Brock, T. H., Cooperation Between Non-Kin in Animal Societies. Nature 462: 5157, 2009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clutton-Brock, T. H., Mammal Societies. John Wiley & Sons, Chichester, 2016.Google Scholar
Clutton-Brock, T. H., Guiness, F. E., Albon, S. D., Red Deer. Behavior and Ecology of Two Sexes. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago, 1982.Google Scholar
Darwin, C., Die Entstehung der Arten. Neudruck Reclam, Stuttgart, 1981 (Original 1859).Google Scholar
Dawkins, R., The Selfish Gene. Oxford University Press, Oxford, 1976.Google Scholar
Gilg, O., Hanski, I., Sittler, B., Cyclic Dynamics in a Simple Vertebrate Predator–Prey Community. Science 203: 866868, 2003.Google Scholar
Hamilton, W. D., The Genetical Theory of Social Behaviour, I, II. Journal of Theoretical Biology 7: 152, 1964.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofer, H., East, M. L., Siblicide in Serengeti Spotted Hyenas: A Long-Term Study of Maternal Input and Cub Survival. Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology 62: 341351, 2008.Google Scholar
Hrdy, S. B., Infanticide Among Animals: Review, Classification, and Examination of the Implications for Reproductive Strategies of Females. Ethology and Sociobiology 1: 1340, 1979.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kappeler, P., Verhaltensbiologie. 3. Auflage, Springer, Heidelberg, 2012.Google Scholar
Keil, A., Sachser, N., Reproductive Benefits from Female Promiscuous Mating in a Small Mammal. Ethology 104: 897903, 1998.Google Scholar
Kempenaers, B., Verheyen, G. R., Vandenbroeck, M., et al., Extra-Pair Paternity Results from Female Preference for High-Quality Males in the Blue Tit. Nature 357: 494496, 1992.Google Scholar
Packer, C., Pusey, A. E., Infanticide in carnivores. In Infanticide: Comparative and Evolutionary Perspectives. Hausfater, G., Hrdy, S. B. (eds.), Aldine, New York, S. 3142, 1984.Google Scholar
Sachser, N., Kaiser, S., Meerschweinchen als Sozialstrategen. Spektrum der Wissenschaft, 56–63, 2010.Google Scholar
Sherman, P. W., Nepotism and the Evolution of Alarm Calls. Science 197: 12461253, 1977.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trivers, R. L., The Evolution of Reciprocal Altruism. The Quarterly Review of Biology 46: 3557, 1971.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trivers, R., Social Evolution. The Benjamin/Cummings Publishing Company, Inc., Menlo Park, CA, 1985.Google Scholar
Wickler, W., Seibt, U., Das Prinzip Eigennutz. Ursachen und Konsequenzen sozialen Verhaltens. Hoffmann und Campe, Hamburg, 1977.Google Scholar
Wilkinson, G. S., Reciprocal Food Sharing in the Vampire Bat. Nature 308: 181184, 1984.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, E. O., Sociobiology. The New Synthesis. The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, 1975.Google Scholar
Wilson, M. L., Boesch, C., Fruth, B., et al., Lethal Aggression in Pan Is Better Explained by Adaptive Strategies Than Human Impacts. Nature 513: 414417, 2014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Waal, F., Are We Smart Enough to Know How Smart Animals Are? W. W. Norton & Company Inc., New York, 2016.Google Scholar
Kershenbaum, A., Bowles, A. E., Freeberg, T. M., et al., Animal Vocal Sequences: Not the Markov Chains We Thought They Were. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 281: DOI 10.1098/rspb.2014.1370, 2017.Google Scholar
Lorenz, K., Das sogenannte Böse. Borotha-Schoeler Verlag, Wien, 1963.Google Scholar
Natterson-Horowitz, B., Bowers, K., Zoobiguity: What Animals Can Teach Us About Health and the Science of Healing. Alfred A. Knopf, New York, 2012.Google Scholar
Raby, C. R., Alexis, D. M., Dickinson, A., Clayton, N. S., Planning for the Future by Western Scrub-Jays. Nature 445: 919921, 2007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thornton, A., McAuliffe, K., Teaching in Wild Meerkats. Science 313: 227229, 2006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Die kulturelle Entwicklung des menschlichen Denkens. Zur Evolution der Kognition. Suhrkamp Verlag, Berlin, 2006.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×