Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-qlrfm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-13T09:29:06.193Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 December 2010

Neil Levy
Affiliation:
University of Melbourne
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Neuroethics
Challenges for the 21st Century
, pp. 317 - 336
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, F. and Aizawa, K. (2001) The bounds of cognition. Philosophical Psychology 14: 43–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adams, F. and Aizawa, K. (forthcoming) Clark missed the mark: Andy Clark on intrinsic content and extended cognition. Analysis.
Aglioti, S., DeSouza, J. E. X. and Goodale, M. A. (1995) Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand. Current Biology, 5, 679–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ainslie, G. (2000) A research-based theory of addictive motivation. Law and Philosophy 19: 77–115.Google Scholar
Ainslie, G. (2001) Breakdown of Will. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alexander, M. P., Stuss, D. T. and Benson, D. F. (1979) Capgras' syndrome: a reduplicative phenomenon. Neurology 29: 334–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alloy, L. B. and Abramson, L. Y. (1979) The judgment of contingency in depressed and nondepressed students: sadder but wiser?Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 441–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alloy, L. B. (1995) Depressive realism: sadder but wiser?The Harvard Mental Health Letter, 11, 4–5.Google Scholar
Anderson, S. W., Damasio, H., Tranel, D. and Damasio, A. R. (2000) Long-term sequelae of prefrontal cortex damage acquired in early childhood. Developmental Neuropsychology, 18, 281–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Angell, M. (2004) The Truth About the Drug Companies: How They Deceive Us and What to Do About It. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
Anton, G. (1899) Über die Selbstwahrnehmung der Herderkrankungen durch den Kranken bei Rindenblindheit und Rindentaubheit. Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkranleheiten, 32: 86–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Archibald, S. J., Mateer, C. A., Kerns, K. A. (2001) Utilization behavior: clinical manifestations and neurological mechanisms. Neuropsychology Review 11: 117–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Axelrod, R. (1984) The Evolution of Cooperation. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Baard, E. (2003) The guilt-free soldier. Village Voice, January 22–8.
Baars, B. J. (1988) A Cognitive Theory of Consciousness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Baars, B. J. (1997) In the theatre of consciousness: global workspace theory, a rigorous scientific theory of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies 4: 292–309.Google Scholar
Bargh, J. A. and Chartrand, T. L. (1999) The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist, 54: 462–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnes, A. (1997) Seeing Through Self-Deception. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Batson, C. D., Engel, C. L. and Fridell, S. R. (1999) Value judgments: testing the somatic-marker hypothesis using false physiological feedback. Personality and social psychology bulletin 25: 1021–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M. and Tice, D. M. (1998) Ego-depletion: is the active self a limited resource?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74: 1252–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumeister, R. F. (2002) Ego depletion and self-control failure: an energy model of the self's executive function. Self and Identity 1: 129–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bavidge, M., and A. J. Cole. (1995) Is psychopathy a moral concept? In Almond, B., ed., Introducing Applied Ethics. Oxford: Blackwell, 185–196.Google Scholar
Bayne, T. (2006) Phenomenology and the feeling of doing: Wegner on the conscious will. In Pockett, S., Banks, W. P. and Gallagher, S., eds., Does Consciousness Cause behavior? An Investigation of the Nature of Volition. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 169–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bayne, T. and Levy, N. (2005) Amputees by choice: body integrity identity disorder and the ethics of amputation. Journal of Applied Philosophy 22: 75–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bayne, T. and Pacherie, E. (2005) In defence of the doxastic conception of delusion. Mind and Language 20: 163–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bealer, G. (1998) Intuition and the autonomy of philosophy. In DePaul, M. R. and Ramsey, W., eds., Rethinking Intuition: The Psychology of Intuition and its Role in Philosophical Inquiry. Lanham, Md.: Rowman & Littlefield Publishers, 201–39.Google Scholar
Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R. and Damasio, H. (2003) Role of the amygdala in decision-making. Annals of the New York Academy of Science 985: 356–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D. and Damasio, A. R. (1997) Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science 275: 1293–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D. and Damasio, A. (2005) The Iowa Gambling Task and the somatic marker hypothesis: some questions and answers. Trends in Cognitive Science 9: 159–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berti, A., Làdavas, E. and Della Corte, M. (1996) Anosognosia for hemiplegia, neglect dyslexia, and drawing neglect: clinical findings and theoretical considerations. Journal of the International Neurological Society 2: 426–40.Google ScholarPubMed
Bishop, M. A. and Trout, J. D. (2005) Epistemology and the Psychology of Human Judgment. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bisiach, E. and Geminiani, G. (1991) Anosognosia related to hemiplegia and hemianopia. In Prigatano, G. P. and Schacter, D. L., eds., Awareness of Deficit After Brain Injury: Clinical and Theoretical Issues. New York: Oxford University Press, 17–39.Google Scholar
Bisiach, E., Vallar, G., Perani, D., Papagno, C. and Berti, A. (1986) Unawareness of disease following lesions of the right hemisphere: anosognosia for hemiplegia and anosognosia for hemianopia. Neuropsychologia, 24: 471–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R. (1995) A cognitive development approach to morality: investigating the psychopath. Cognition 57: 1–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R. (1997) Moral reasoning and the child with psychopathic tendencies. Personality and Individual Differences 26: 731–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blair, J.Mitchell, D. and Blair, K. (2005) The Psychopath: Emotion and the Brain. Malden, Mass.: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Block, N. (1997) On a confusion about a function of consciousness. In Block, N., Flanagan, O. and Güzeldere, G., eds., The Nature of Consicousness: Philosophical Debates. Cambridge, Mass: The MIT Press, 375–415.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. (2004) Descartes' Baby. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Blumenthal, J. A.et al. (1999) Effects of exercise training on older patients with major depression. Archives of Internal Medicine 159: 2349–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bourdieu, P. (1986) Forms of capital. In Richardson, J. G., ed., Handbook of Theory and Research for the Sociology of Education. New York: Greenwood Press, 241–58.Google Scholar
Bourtchouladze, R. (2002) Memories Are Made of This: The Biological Building Blocks of Memory. Frome: Weidenfeld & Nicolson.Google Scholar
Brasil-Neto, J. P., Pascual-Leone, A., Valls-Sole, J., Cohen, L. G. and Hallett, M. (1992) Focal transcranial magnetic stimulation and response bias in a forced-choice task. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 55: 964–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breen, N., Caine, D., Coltheart, M., Hendy, J. and Roberts, C. (2000). Delusional misidentification. Mind and Language 15: 74–110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broad, C. D. (1952) Ethics and the History of Philosophy. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Broughton, R., Billings, R., Cartwright, R.et al. (1994) Homicidal somnambulism: a case report. Sleep 17: 253–64.Google ScholarPubMed
Buchanan, A., Brock, D. W., Daniels, N. and Wikler, D. (2000) From Chance to Choice: Genetics and Justice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butler, J. (1970) Butler's Fifteen Sermons Preached at the Rolls Chapel. London: SPCK.Google Scholar
Cabeza, R.Rao, S. M., Wagner, A. D., Mayer, A. R. and Schacter, D. L. (2001) Can medial temporal lobe regions distinguish true from false? An event-related functional MRI study of veridical and illusory recognition memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 98: 4805–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cahill, L., Prins, B., Weber, M. and McGaugh, J. L. (1994) Beta-adrenergic activation and memory for emotional events. Nature 371: 702–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Callaway, E., Halliday, R., Perez-Stable, E. J., Coates, T. J. and Hauck, W. W. (1991) Propranolol and response bias: an extension of findings reported by Corwinet al. Biological Psychiatry 30: 739–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Canli, T. (2006) When genes and brains unite: ethical implications of genomic neuroimaging. In Illes, J., ed., Neuroethics: Defining the Issues in Theory, Practice, and Policy. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 169–83.Google Scholar
Canli, T., Zhao, Z., Desmond, J. E., Kang, E., Gross, J. and Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2001) An fMRI study of personality influences on brain reactivity to emotional stimuli. Behavioral Neuroscience 115: 33–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Canli, T., Sivers, H., Gotlib, I. H. and Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2002) Amygdala activation to happy faces as a function of extraversion. Science 296: 2191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cappa, S., Sterzi, R., Vallar, G. and Bisiach, E. (1987) Remission of hemineglect and anosognosia during vestibular stimulation. Neuropsychologia 25: 775–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carruthers, P. (2005) Why the question of animal consciousness might not matter very much. Philosophical Psychology 18: 83–102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caspi, A.McClay, J., Moffitt, T. E.et al. (2002) Evidence that the cycle of violence in maltreated children depends on genotype. Science 297: 851–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charland, L. C. (2002) Cynthia's dilemma: consenting to heroin prescriptionAmerican Journal of Bioethics 2: 37–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Churchland, P. S. (2003) Self-representation in nervous systems. Annals of the New York Academy of Science 1001: 31–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, A. (1997) Being There: Putting Brain, Body, and World Together Again. Cambridge, Mass.: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2002) Minds, brains and tools. In H. Clapin, ed., Philosophy of Mental Representation. Oxford: Clarendon Press, 66–90.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2003) Natural-Born Cyborgs: Minds, Technologies, and the Future of Human Intelligence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2005) Intrinsic content, active memory and the extended mind. Analysis 65: 1–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, A. (2006) Memento's revenge: The extended mind, extended. In Menary, R., ed., The Extended Mind. Aldershot: Ashgate.Google Scholar
Clark, A. and Chalmers, D. (1998) The extended mind. Analysis 58: 7–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, T. W. (1999) Fear of mechanism: a compatibilist critique of “the volitional brain”. Journal of Consciousness Studies 6: 279–93.Google Scholar
Coady, C. A. J. (1992) Testimony: A Philosophical Study. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cocchini, G., Beschin, N. and Della Sala, S. (2002) Chronic anosognosia: a case report and theoretical account. Neuropsychologia 40: 2030–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, A. and Leckman, J. F. (1992) Sensory phenomena associated with Gilles de la Tourette's syndrome. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 53: 319–23.Google ScholarPubMed
Cohen, D. J. and Leckman, J. F. (1999) Introduction: the self under síege. In Leckman, J. F. and Cohen, D. J., eds., Tourette's Syndrome – Tics, Obsessions, Compulsions. New York, John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 1–19.Google Scholar
Corwin, J., Peselow, E., Feenan, K, Rotrosen, J. and Fieve, R. (1990) Disorders of decision in affective disease: An effect of β-adrenergic dysfunction?Biological Psychiatry 27: 813–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cummins, R. (1998) Reflections on reflective equilibrium. In M. R. DePaul and W. Ramsey, Rethinking Intuition. Leuham Md.: Rowman & Littlefield Publishers, 113–27.Google Scholar
Currie, G. (2000) Imagination, delusion and hallucinations. Mind and Language, 15, 168–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cushman, F. A., Young, L. and Hauser, M. D. (2006) The role of conscious reasoning and intuition in moral judgments: testing three principles of harm. Psychological Science 17: 1082–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Damasio, A. (1994) Descartes' Error: Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain. London: Picador.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (1996) The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society Lond B 351: 1413–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Damasio, A. R. (1999) The Feeling of What Happens. New York: Harcourt Brace.Google Scholar
Damasio, H., Grabowski, T., Frank, R., Galaburda, A. M. and Damasio, A. R. (1994) The return of Phineas Gage: clues about the brain from the skull of a famous patient. Science 264: 1102–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daniels, N. (1985) Just Health Care. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daniels, N. (2003) Reflective equilibrium. In E. N. Zalta ed., The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/sum2003/entries/reflective-equilibrium/.
Darnton, R. (1985) The Great Cat Massacre: And Other Episodes in French Cultural History. New York: Vintage Books.Google Scholar
Dasgupta, N. (2004) Implicit ingroup favoritism, outgroup favoritism, and their behavioral manifestations. Social Justice Research 17: 143–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, D. (1986) Deception and division. In Elster, J., ed., The Multiple Self. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 79–92.Google Scholar
Davies, M., Aimola Davies, A. and Coltheart, M. (2005) Anosognosia and the two-factor theory of delusions. Mind and Language 20: 209–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1983) The Extended Phenotype: The Long Reach of the Gene. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (2006) Let's all stop beating Basil's car. The Edge. <http://www.edge.org/q2006/q06_9.html#dawkins>
DeGrazia, D. (1996) Taking Animals Seriously: Mental Life and Moral Status. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeGrazia, D. (2000) Prozac, enhancement, and self-creation. Hastings Center Report 30: 34–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeGrazia, D. (2005) Human Identity and Bioethics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, D. and Naccache, L. (2001) Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition 79: 1–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennett, D. (1978) Brain writing and mind reading. In Brainstorms: Philosophical Essays on Mind and Psychology. London: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. (1984) Elbow Room: The Varieties of Free Will Worth Wanting. Cambridge, Mass.: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. (1990) The myth of original intentionality. In Said, K. A. Mohyeldin, Newton-Smith, W. H., Viale, R. and Wilkes, K. V., eds., Modeling the Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 43–62.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. (1991) Consciousness Explained. London: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. (1996) Kinds of Minds. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. (2001) Are we explaining consciousness yet?Cognition 79: 221–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennett, D. (2003) Freedom Evolves. London: Allen Lane.Google Scholar
DePaul, M. (1998) Why bother with reflective equilibrium? In DePaul, and Ramsey, , eds., Rethinking Intuition. Lanham, Md.: Rowman & Littlefield Publishers, 293–309.Google Scholar
DePaulo, B. M. (1994) Spotting lies: can humans learn to do better?Current Directions in Psychological Science 3: 83–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waal, F. (1996) Good Natured: The Origins of Right and Wrong in Humans and Other Animals. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Dickerson, F. B. (2000) Cognitive Behavioural Psychotherapy for Schizophrenia: A Review of Recent Empirical Studies. Schizophrenia Research 43: 71–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dijksterhuis, A., Bos, M. W., Nordgren, L. F. and Baaren, R. B. (2006) On making the right choice: the deliberation-without-attention effect. Science 311: 1005–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dienstbier, R. A. (1978) Attribution, socialization, and moral decision making. In Harvey, J. H., Ickes, W. J. and Kidd, R. F., eds., New Directions in Attribution Research, Vol. 2. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 181–206.Google Scholar
Dienstbier, R. A. and Munster, P. O. (1971) Cheating as a function of the labelling of natural arousal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 17: 208–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donald, M. (1991) Origins of the Modern Mind. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Doricchi, F. and Galati, G. (2000) Implicit semantic evaluation of object symmetry and contralesional visual denial in a case of left unilateral neglect with damage of the dorsal paraventricular white matter. Cortex 36: 337–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Double, R. (2004) How to accept Wegner's illusion of conscious will and still defend moral responsibility. Behavior and Philosophy 32: 479–91.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. (1996) Grooming, Gossip, and the Evolution of Language. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Edwards, D. H. and Kravitz, E. A. (1997) Serotonin, social status and aggression. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 7: 811–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P. (2003) Darwin, deception, and facial expression. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1000: 205–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P., O'Sullivan, M. and Frank, M. G. (1999) A few can catch a liar. Psychological Science 10: 263–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekstrom, L. W. (1999) Free Will: A Philosophical Study. Boulder, Co.: Westview.Google Scholar
Elliott, C. (1998) The tyranny of happiness: ethics and cosmetic psychopharmacology. In Parens, E., ed., Enhancing Human Traits: Ethical and Social Implications. Washington, D. C.: Georgetown University Press, 177–188.Google Scholar
Elliott, C. (2000) A new way to be mad, The Atlantic Monthly 286, No. 6, December.Google Scholar
Elliott, C. (2002) Who holds the leash?American Journal of Bioethics 2: 48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, H. D. and Young, A. W. (1990) Accounting for delusional misidentifications. British Journal of Psychiatry 157, 239–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, H. D., Young, A. W., Quayle, A. H. and Pauw, K. W. (1997) Reduced autonomic responses to faces in Capgras delusion. Proceedings of the Royal Society: Biological Sciences, B264, 1085–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elster, J. (1999) Strong Feelings: Emotion, Addiction and Human Behavior. Cambridge, Mass.: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Elster, J. (2000) Ulysses Unbound. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engel, A. K., Fries, P., König, P.Brecht, M. and Singer, W. (1999) Temporal binding, binocular rivalry, and consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition 8: 128–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Estlinger, P. J., Warner, G. C., Grattan, L. M. and Easton, J. D. (1991) Frontal lobe utilization behavior associated with paramedian thalamic infarction. Neurology, 41: 450–2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fahle, M. (2003) Failures of visual analysis: scotoma, agnosia, and neglect. In Fahle, M. and Greenlee, M., eds., The Neuropsychology of Vision. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 179–258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fairclough, S. H. and Houston, K. (2004) A metabolic measure of mental effort. Biological Psychology 66, 177–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farah, M. J. (2002) Emerging ethical issues in neuroscience. Nature Neuroscience 5: 1123–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farah, M. J. (2005) Neuroethics: the practical and the philosophical. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9, 34–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farah, M. J. and Wolpe, P. R. (2004) New neuroscience technologies and their ethical implications. Hastings Center Report 34: 35–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farah, M. J., Noble, K. G. and Hurt, H. (2006) Poverty, privilege, and brain development: empirical findings and ethical implications. In Illes, J., ed., Neuroethics: Defining the Issues in Theory, Practice, and Policy. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 277–87.Google Scholar
Farwell, L. A. and Smith, S. S. (2001) Using brain MERMER testing to detect concealed knowledge despite efforts to conceal. Journal of Forensic Sciences 46 (1): 1–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fingarette, H. (1988) Heavy Drinking: The Myth of Alcoholism as a Disease. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
First, M. B. (2005) Desire for amputation of a limb: paraphilia, psychosis, or a new type of identity disorder. Psychological Medicine 35: 919–928.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Firth, R. (1952) Ethical absolutism and the ideal observer theory. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 12: 317–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flanagan, O. (1996a) Neuroscience, agency, and the meaning of life. In Flanagan, O., ed., Self-Expressions: Mind, Morals, and the Meaning of Life. New York: Oxford University Press, 53–64.Google Scholar
Flanagan, O. (1996b) I remember you. In Self-Expressions: Mind, Morals, and the Meaning of Life. New York: Oxford University Press, 88–98.Google Scholar
Flynn, J. R. (1994) IQ gains over time. In Sternberg, R. J., ed., Encyclopedia of Human Intelligence. New York: Macmillan, 617–23.Google Scholar
Foddy, B. and Savaluscu, J. (2006) Addiction and autonomy: can addicted people consent to the prescription of their drug?Bioethics 20: 1–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foot, P. (1978) The problem of abortion and the doctrine of the double effect. In Virtues and Vices. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Frank, R. H. (1988) Passions Within Reason: The Strategic Role of the Emotions. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Freedman, C. (1998) Aspirin for the mind? Some ethical worries about psychopharmacology. In Parens, E., ed., Enhancing Human Traits: Ethical and Social Implications. Washington, D. C.: Georgetown University Press, 135–50.Google Scholar
Gailliot, M. T., Baumeister, R. F., DeWall, C. N., Maner, J. K. and Plant, E. A. (2007) Self-control relies on glucose as a limited energy source: willpower is more than a metaphor. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 92: 325–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garland, B. (2004) Neuroscience and the Law. New York: Dana Press.Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S. (1985) The Social Brain. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S. (1992) Nature's Mind. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S. (2005) The Ethical Brain. New York: Dana Press.Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. (1991) How mental systems believe. American Psychologist 46: 107–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, D. (1993) The assent of man: mental representation and the control of belief. In Wegner, D. M. and Pennebaker, J., eds., The Handbook of Mental Control. New York: Prentice-Hall, 57–87.Google Scholar
Glannon, W. (2006a) Psychopharmacology and memory. Journal of Medical Ethics 32: 74–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glannon, W. (2006b) Bioethics on the Brain. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glover, J. (1999) Humanity: A Moral History of the Twentieth Century. London: Pimlico.Google Scholar
Goldman, A. and Pust, P. (1998) Philosophical theory and intuitional evidence. In M. R. DePaul and W. Ramsey, Rethinking Intuition. Lenham, Md.: Rowmam and Littlefield Publishers, 179–97.Google Scholar
Goodale, M. A. and Milner, A. D. (2004) Sight Unseen: An Exploration of Conscious and Unconscious Vision. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. (1988) The streak of streaks. New York Review of Books 35: 8–12.Google Scholar
Greely, H. T. (2004) Prediction, litigation, privacy, and property: some possible legal and social implications of advances in neuroscience. In Garland, B., ed., Neuroscience and the Law: Brain, Mind, and the Scales of Justice. New York: Dana Press, 114–56.Google Scholar
Greene, J.Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M. and Cohen, J. D. (2001) An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science 293: 2105–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greene, J. (2003) From neural “is” to moral “ought”: what are the moral implications of neuroscientific moral psychology?Nature Reviews Neuroscience 4: 847–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greene, J. (2005) Cognitive neuroscience and the structure of the moral mind. In Carruthers, P., Laurence, S. and Stich, S., eds., The Innate Mind: Structure and Content. New York: Oxford University Press, 338–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greene. J. (forthcoming) The secret joke of Kant's soul. In Sinnott-Armstrong, ed., Moral Psychology.
Greene, J. and Haidt, J. (2004) How (and where) does moral judgment work?Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6: 517–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haggard, P. and Libet, B. (2001) Conscious intention and brain activity. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8: 47–63.Google Scholar
Haidt, J. (2001) The emotional dog and its rational tail: a social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review 108: 814–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haidt, J. (2003) The emotional dog does learn new tricks: a reply to Pizarro and Bloom. Psychological Review 110: 197–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haidt, J., Koller, S. H. and Dias, M. G. (1993) Affect, culture, and morality, or is it wrong to eat your dog?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 65: 613–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haji, I. (2002) Deontic Morality and Control. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamilton, A. (forthcoming) Against the belief model of delusion. In Chung, M., Fulford, K. W. M. and Graham, G., eds., The Philosophical Understanding of Schizophrenia. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Haugeland, J. (1998) Mind embodied and embedded. In Having Thought. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 207–37.
Hauser, M. D. (2006) The liver and moral organ. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 1: 214–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hauser, M. D., Cushman, F. A., Young, L., Kang-Xing Jin, R. and Mikhail, J. (2007). A dissociation between moral judgments and justifications. Mind and Language 22: 1–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haynes, J.-D., Sakai, K., Rees, G., Gilbert, S., Frith, C and Passnigham, R. E. (2007) Reading hidden intentions in the human brain. Current Biology 17: 323–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hazlett-Stevens, H. and Craske, M. G. (2004) Brief cognitive-behavioral therapy: definition and scientific foundations. In Bond, F. and Dryden, W., eds., Handbook Of Brief Cognitive Behaviour Therapy. Chichester, UK: JohnWiley and Sons, Ltd, 1–20.Google Scholar
Heilman, K. M., Barrett, A. M. and Adair, J. C. (1998) Possible mechanisms of anosognosia: a defect in self-awareness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B 353: 1903–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirstein, W. (2000) Self-deception and confabulation. Philosophy of Science 67: S418–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirstein, W. (2005) Brain Fiction: Self-Deception and Confabulation. Cambridge, Mass.: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Holton, R. (2004) Rational Resolve, Philosophical Review, 113: 507–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horowitz, T. (1998) Philosophical intuitions and psychological theory. In DePaul, M. and Ramsey, W., eds., Rethinking Intuition. Lanham, Md.: Rowmam & Littlefield Publishers, 143–60.Google Scholar
Hunt, E. (1995) The role of intelligence in modern society. American Scientist 83: 356–68.Google Scholar
Illes, J. (2003) Neuroethics in a new era of neuroimaging. American Journal of Neuroradiology 24: 1739–1741.Google Scholar
Illes, J., Racine, E. and Kirschen, M. P. (2006) A picture is worth 1000 words, but which 1000? In Illes, Judy, ed., Neuroethics: Defining the Issues in Theory, Practice, and Policy. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 149–68.Google Scholar
Jack, A. I., and Shallice, T, (2001) Introspective physicalism as an approach to the science of consciousness. Cognition 79: 161–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, W. (1890) Principles of Psychology. New York: Henry Holt and Company.Google Scholar
Jehkonen, M., Ahonen, J.-P., Dastidar, P., Koivisto, A.-M., Laippala, P. and Vilkki, J. (2000) Unawareness of deficits after right hemisphere stroke: double dissociation of anosognosias. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica 102: 378–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, K. (1999) Second-hand moral knowledge. The Journal of Philosophy 96: 55–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, K. (2004) Emotional rationality as practical rationality. In Calhoun, C., ed., Setting the Moral Compass: Essays by Women Philosophers. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 333–52.Google Scholar
Joyce, R. (2001) The Myth of Morality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joyce, R. (2006) The Evolution of Morality. Cambridge, Mass: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Juengst, E. T. (1998) What does enhancement mean? In Parens, E., ed., Enhancing Human Traits: Ethical and Social Implications. Washington, D. C.: Georgetown University Press, 29–47.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., Slovic, P. and Tversky, A. (1982) Judgement Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kamitani, Y. and Tong, F. (2005) Decoding the visual and subjective contents of the human brain. Nature Neuroscience 8: 679–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kane, R. (1996) The Significance of Free Will. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kane, R. (2005) Remarks on the psychology of free will. Presented at the 31st Annual Meeting of The Society Of Philosophy And Psychology, Wake Forest University, Winston-Salem, North Carolina, June 9–12, 2005. Available online: <http://gfp.typepad.com/online_papers/2005/07/robert_kane.html>
Kaplan, J. M. (2000) The Limits and Lies of Human Genetic Research: Dangers for Social Policy. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Keitner, G. I. and Cardemil, E. V. (2004) Psychotherapy for chronic depressive disorders. In Alpert, J. E. and Fava, M., eds., Handbook of Chronic Depression: Diagnosis and Therapeutic Management. New York: Marcel Dekker, Inc: 159–81.Google Scholar
Kessler, R. C., Sonnega, A., Bromet, E., Hughes, M. and Nelson, C. B. (1996) Posttraumatic stress disorder in the national comorbidity survey. Archives of General Psychiatry, 52, 1048–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Khantzian, E. J. (1997) The self-medication hypothesis of substance use disorders: a reconsideration and recent applications. Harvard Review of Psychiatry 4: 231–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, R. A., Leckman, J. F., Scahill, L. and Cohen, D. J. (1999) Obsessive-compulsive disorder, anxiety, and depression. In Leckman, J. F. and Cohen, D. J., eds., Tourette's Syndrome – Tics, Obsessions, Compulsions. New York, John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 43–61.Google Scholar
Knutson, B.et al. (1998) Selective alteration of personality and social behavior by serotonergic intervention. American Journal of Psychiatry 155: 373–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Korsgaard, C. (1986) The Sources of Normativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kramer, P. D. (1993) Listening to Prozac. London: Fourth Estate.Google Scholar
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M. and Cuthbert, B. N. (1990) Emotion, attention, and the startle reflex. Psychological Review 97: 377–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langleben, D. D., Schroeder, L., Maldjian, J. A., Gur, R. C., McDonald, S., Ragland, J. D., O'Brien, C. P. and Childress, A. R. (2002) Brain activity during simulated deception: an event-related functional magnetic resonance study. Neuroimage 15: 727–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leckman, J. F., King, R. A. and Cohen, D. J. (1999) Tics and tic disorders. In Leckman, J. F. and Cohen, D. J., eds., Tourette's Syndrome – Tics, Obsessions, Compulsions. New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc., 23–41.Google Scholar
Ledoux, J. (2003) The self: clues from the brain. Annals of the New York Academy of Science 1001: 295–304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leshner, A. (1999) Science-based views of drug addiction and its treatment. Journal of the American Medical Association 282: 314–16.Google ScholarPubMed
Levine, D. N., Calvanio, R. and Rinn, W. E. (1991) The pathogenesis of anosognosia for hemiplegia. Neurology 41: 1770–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, N. (2002a) Self-ownership: defending Marx against Cohen. Social Theory and Practice 28: 77–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, N. (2002b) Moral Relativism: An Introduction. Oxford: Oneworld.Google Scholar
Levy, N. (2004) What Makes Us Moral?Oxford: Oneworld.Google Scholar
Levy, N. (forthcoming) Restrictivism is a covert compatibilism. In Trahahis, N. ed. Essays on Free Will and Moral Responsibility. Cambridge: Cambridge Scholars Press.
Levy, N. and Bayne, T. (2004) Doing without deliberation: automatism, automaticity, and moral accountability. International Review of Psychiatry 16: 209–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, D. (1989) Dispositional theories of values. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, Supplementary Volume 63: 113–37.Google Scholar
Lhermitte, F. (1983) Utilization behavior and its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes. Brain, 106: 237–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lhermitte, F., Pillon, B. and Serdaru, M. (1986) Human autonomy and the frontal lobes: part I. Imitation and utilization behavior: a neuropsychological study of 75 patients. Annals of Neurology, 19: 326–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Libet, B., Gleason, C., Wright, E. and Pearl, D. (1983) Time of unconscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential). Brain 106: 623–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Libet, B. (1999) Do we have free will?Journal of Consciousness Studies 6: 47–57.Google Scholar
Loewenstein, G. (2000) Willpower: a decision theorist's perspective. Law and Philosophy 2000: 51–76.Google Scholar
Loftus, E. F. (1993) The reality of repressed memories. American Psychologist 48: 518–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loftus, E. F. (2003) Make-believe memories. American Psychologist 58: 867–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loftus, E. F. and Pickrell, J. E. (1995) The formation of false memories. Psychiatric Annals 25: 720–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mackenzie, C. and Stoljar, N. eds. (2000) Relational Autonomy: Feminist Perspectives on Autonomy, Agency, and the Social Self. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Maguire, E. A., Gadian, D. G., Johnsrude, I. S.et al. (2000) Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA 97 (8): 4398–403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maia, T. V. and McClelland, J. L. (2004) A re-examination of the evidence for the somatic marker hypothesis. What participants know in the Iowa Gambling Tasle. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 101: 16075–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manninen, B. A. (2006) Medicating the mind: a Kantian analysis of overprescribing psychoactive drugs. Journal of Medical Ethics 32: 100–105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcel, A. J., Tegnér, R. and Nimmo-Smith, I. (2004) Anosognosia for plegia: specificity, extension, partiality and disunity of bodily awareness. Cortex 40: 19–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marshall, J. C. and Halligan, P. W. (1988) Blindsight and insight in visuo-spatial neglect. Nature 336: 766–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masserman, J. H., Wechkin, S. and Terris, W. (1964) “Altruistic” behavior in rhesus monkeys. American Journal of Psychiatry 121: 584–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matte, T. D., Bresnahan, M., Begg, M. D. and Susser, E. (2001) Influence of variation in birth weight within normal range and within sibships on IQ at age 7 years: cohort study. British Medical Journal 323: 310–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCall, S. and Lowe, E. J. (2005) Indeterminist free will. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 70: 681–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGaugh, J. L. (2000) Memory – a century of consolidation. Science 5451, 248–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mele, A. R. (1997) Real self-deception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences20: 91–102. In Campbell, J., O'Rourke, M. and Shier, D., eds., Explanation and Causation: Topics in Contemporary Philosophy. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mele, A. R. (1999) Ultimate responsibility and dumb luck. Social Philosophy and Policy 16: 274–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mele, A. R. (2001) Self-Deception Unmasked. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Mele, A. R. (2007) Decisions, intentions, urges, and free will: why Libet has not shown what he says he has. In Campbell, J., O'Rourke, M. and Shier, D., eds., Expanation and Causation: Topics in Contemporary Philosophy. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Menary, R. (2006) Attacking the bounds of cognition. Philosophical Psychology 19: 329–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Metzinger, T. (2004) Inferences are just folk psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27: 670.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mill, J. S. 1985 [1859] On Liberty. London: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Miller, R. B. (2000) Without intuitions. Metaphilosophy 31: 231–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milner, A. D. and Goodale, M. A. (1995) The Visual Brain in Action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Milner, A. D. and Goodale, M. A. (1998) The visual brain in action. PSYCHE, 4 (12) (http://psyche.cs.monash.edu.au/v4/psyche-4–12-milner.html)
Mischel, W. (1981) Metacognition and the rules of delay. In Flavell, J. H. and Ross, L., eds., Social Cognitive DevelopmentCambridge: Cambridge University Press, 240–71.Google Scholar
Mischel, W., Shoda, Y. and Rodriguez, (1989) Delay of gratification in children. Science 244: 933–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miyake, Y., Mizutani, M. and Yamamura, T. (1993) Event-related potentials as an indicator of detecting information in field polygraph examinations. Polygraph 22: 131–49.Google Scholar
Money, J., Jobaris, R. and Furth, G. (1977) Apotemnophilia: two cases of self-demand amputation as paraphilia. The Journal of Sex Research, 13: 115–125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morse, S. J. (2004) New neuroscience, old problems. In Garland, B., ed., Neuroscience and the Law. New York: Dana Press. 157–98.Google Scholar
Morse, S. J. (2006) Moral and legal responsibility and the new neuroscience. In Illes, J., ed., Neuroethics: Defining the Issues in Theory, Practice, and Policy. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 33–50.Google Scholar
Mottram, D. R., ed., (2003) Drugs in Sport. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Nahmias, E. (2002) When consciousness matters: a critical review of Daniel Wegner's The Illusion of conscious Will. Philosophical Psychology 15: 527–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
National Research Council (2003) The Polygraph and Lie Detection. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.Google Scholar
Neale, J. (2002) Drug Users in Society. New York: Palgrave.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neisser, U. (1997) Rising scores on intelligence tests. American Scientist 85: 440–7.Google Scholar
Neu, J. (2000) A Tear is an Intellectual Thing: the Meanings of Emotion. Oxford, New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nichols, S., Stich, S. and Weinberg, J. M. (2003) Meta-skepticism: meditations in ethno-epistemology. In Luper, S., ed., The Skeptics. Aldershot: Ashgate Publishing, 227–47.Google Scholar
Noakes, T. D., St Clair Gibson, A. and Lamber, E. V. (2004) From catastrophe to complexity: a novel model of integrative central neural regulation of effort and fatigue during exercise in humans. British Journal of Sports Medicine 38: 511–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nozick, R. (1974) Anarchy, State, and Utopia. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Nozick, R. (1981) Philosophical Explanations. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nucci, L. P. (1989) Challenging conventional wisdom about morality: the domain approach to values education. In Nucci, L. P., ed., Moral Development and Character Education: A Dialogue. Berkeley: McCutcheon, 183–203.Google Scholar
Nussbaum, M. C. (2000) Women and Human Development: The Capabilities Approach. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nussbaum, M. C. (2001) Upheavals of Thought: The Intelligence of Emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Connor, T. (2000) Persons and Causes: The Metaphysics of Free Will. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Olson, E. T. (1999) There is no problem of the self. In Gallagher, S. and Shear, J., eds., Models of the Self. Exeter: Imprint Academic, 49–61.Google Scholar
Osborne, L. (2003) Savant for a day. New York Times. June 22.Google Scholar
Parens, E. (1998) Is better always good? The enhancement project. In Parens, E., ed., Enhancing Human Traits: Ethical and Social Implications. Washington, D.C.: Georgetown University Press, 1–28.Google Scholar
Parens, E. (2005) Authenticity and ambivalence: toward understanding the enhancement debate. Hastings Center Report 35 (3): 34–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pereboom, D. (2001) Living Without Free Will. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phelps, E. A., O'Connor, K. J., Cunningham, , et al. (2000) Performance on indirect measures of race evaluation predicts amygdala activity, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12: 1–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phillips, K. (1996) The Broken Mirror: Understanding and Treating Body Dysmorphic Disorder. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1994) The Language Instinct: How the Mind Creates Language. New York: HarperCollins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pitman, R. K. and Delahanty, D. L. (2005) Conceptually driven pharmacologic approaches to acute trauma. CNS Spectrums 10: 99–106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pitman, R. K., Sanders, K. M., Zusman, R. M., et al. (2002) Pilot study of secondary prevention of posttraumatic stress disorder with Propanolol. Biological Psychiatry 51: 189–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pockett, S. (2004) Does consciousness cause behaviour?Journal of Consciousness Studies 11: 23–40.Google Scholar
Pogge, T. (2002) World Poverty and Human Rights. Cambridge: CPolity Press.Google Scholar
Pollack, W. (1998) Real Boys: Rescuing Our Sons from the Myths of Boyhood. New York: Henry Holt.Google Scholar
Prinz, J. (2005) Imitation and moral development. In Hurley, S. and Chater, N., eds. Perspectives on Imitation: From Cognitive Neuroscience to Social Science, vol. 2. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 267–82.Google Scholar
Pust, J. (2000) Intuitions as Evidence. New York: Garland Publishing.Google Scholar
Quian Quiroga, R., Reddy, L.Kreiman, G., Koch, C. and Fried, I. (2005) Invariant visual representation by single neurons in the human brain. Nature 435: 1102–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S. (1996) The evolutionary biology of self-deception, laughter, dreaming and depression. Medical Hypotheses 47: 347–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramachandran, V. S., Altschuler, E. L. and Hillyer, S. (1997) Mirror agnosia. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 264: 645–647.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S. and Blakeslee, S. (1998) Phantoms in the Brain. London: Fourth Estate.Google Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S. and Hirstein, W. (1998) The perception of phantom limbs. Brain 121: 1603–1630.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rawls, J. (1971) A Theory of Justice. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Rawls, J. (1993) Political Liberalism. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Rayner, K. (1998) Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 372–422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reznek, L. (1997) Evil or Ill? Justifying the Insanity Defence. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Rice, G. E. and Gainer, P. (1962) “Altruism” in the Albino Rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 55: 123–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, R. D., Lancaster, M., Wakeley, J. and Bhagwagar, Z. (2004) Effects of beta-adrenoceptor blockade on components of human decision-making. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 172: 157–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rose, S. (2005) The Future of the Brain: The Promise and Perils of Tomorrow's Neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rosenfeld, J. P.Soskins, M., Bosh, G. and Ryan, A. (2004) Simple effective countermeasures to P300-based tests of detection of concealed information. Psychophysiology 4: 205–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenthal, D. M. (2002) The timing of conscious states. Consciousness and Cognition 11: 215–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roskies, A. (2002) Neuroethics for the new millenium. Neuron 35: 21–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roskies, A. (2003) Are ethical judgments intrinsically motivational? Lessons from “acquired sociopathy”. Philosophical Psychology 16: 51–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roskies, A. (2006) A case study in neuroethics: the nature of moral judgment. In Illes, J., ed., Neuroethics: Defining the Issues in Theory, Practice, and Policy. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 17–32.Google Scholar
Rowlands, M. (1999) The Body in Mind: Understanding Cognitive Processes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rowlands, M. (2003) Externalism: Putting Mind and World Back Together Again. Chesham: Acumen.Google Scholar
Rupert, R. D. (2004) Challenges to the hypothesis of extended cognition. Journal of Philosophy CI: 389–428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sacks, O. (1985) The Man Who Mistook his Wife for a Hat. London: Picador.Google Scholar
Safire, W. (2002) The but-what-if factor. The New York Times, May 16.Google Scholar
Salmon, P. (2001) Effects of physical exercise on anxiety, depression, and sensitivity to stress: a unifying theory. Clinical Psychology Review 21: 33–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sartre, J. -P. (1956) Being and Nothingness. New York: Washington Square Press.Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, S., Taylor, T. J. and Shanker, S. G. (1999) Apes, Language, and the Human Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Saver, J. L. and Damasio, A. R. (1991) Preserved access and processing of social knowledge in a patient with acquired sociopathy due to ventromedial frontal damage. Neuropsychologia 29: 1241–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savulescu, J., Foddy, B. and Clayton, M. (2004) Why we should allow performance enhancing drugs in sport. British Journal Sports Medicine 38: 666–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scanlon, T. (1998) What We Owe to Each Other. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L. (1996) Searching for Memory: The Brain, the Mind, and the Past. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Schelling, T. C. (1992) Self-command: a new discipline. In Loewenstein, G. and Elster, J., eds., Choice Over Time. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Schechtman, M. (1996) The Constitution of Selves. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Schmeichel, B. J. and Baumeister, R. F. (2004) Self-regulatory strength. In Baumeister, R. F. and Vohs, K. D., eds., Handbook of Self-Regulation. New York: The Guilford Press, 84–98.Google Scholar
Schopp, R. F. (1991) Automatism, insanity, and the psychology of criminal Responsibility: A Philosophical Inquiry. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schultz, R. T., Carter, A. S., Schaill, L. and Leckman, J. F. (1999) Neurophysiological findings. In Leckman, J. F. and Cohen, D. J., eds., Tourette's Syndrome – Tics, Obsessions, Compulsions. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 80–103.Google Scholar
Scott, K. (2000) Voluntary amputee ran disability site. The Guardian, February 7.Google Scholar
Schwitzgebel, E. (2002) A phenomenal, dispositional account of beliefNoûs, 36, 249–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1994) The Rediscovery of the Mind. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Sententia, W. (2004) Neuroethical considerations: cognitive liberty and converging technologies for improving human cognition. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1013: 221–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simons, D. J. and Levin, D. T. (1998) Failure to detect changes to people during a real-world interaction. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 5: 644–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singer, P. (1974) Sidgwick and reflective equilibrium, The Monist 58: 490–517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singer, P. (1990) Animal Liberation (2nd edn.). New York: Avon Books.Google Scholar
Singer, P. (2005) Ethics and intuitions. Journal of Ethics 9: 331–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sinnott-Armstrong, W. (2006) Moral intuitionism meets empirical psychology. In Timmons, M. and Horgan, T., eds., Metaethics after Moore. New York: Oxford University Press, 339–65.Google Scholar
Skodol, A. E. and J. M. Oldham (1996) Phenomenology, differential diagnosis, and comorbidity of the obsessive-compulsive spectrum of disorders. In Oldham, J. M., Hollander, E. and Skodol, A. E., eds., Impulsivity and Compulsivity. Washington, D. C.: American Psychiatric Press, 1–36.Google Scholar
Smetana, J. and Breages, J. (1990) The development of toddlers' moral and conventional judgements. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 36: 329–46.Google Scholar
Smillie, S. (2004) Throw your arms around the world. The Guardian, Dec 2.Google Scholar
Snyder, A. W., Mulcahy, E., Taylor, J. L.Mitchell, D. J., Sachdev, P. and Gandevia, S. C. (2003) Savant-like skills exposed in normal people by suppressing the left fronto-temporal lobe. Journal of Integrative Neuroscience 2: 149–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sobell, L. C., Ellingstad, T. P. and Sobell, M. B. (2000) Natural recovery from alcohol and drug problems: methodological review of the research with suggestions for future directions, Addiction 95: 749–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spence, S. (1996) Free will in the light of neuropsychiatry. Philosophy, Psychiatry and Psychology 3: 75–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sterelny, K. and Griffiths, P. E. (1999) Sex and Death: An Introduction to Philosophy of Biology. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Sterelny, K. (2004) Externalism, epistemic artefacts and the extended mind. In Schantz, R., ed., The Externalist Challenge: New Studies in Cognition and Intentionality. Berlin: de Gruyter, 239–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sterelny, K. (2007) Cognitive Load and Human Decision, or, Three Ways of Rolling the Rock Uphill. In Lawrence, S. and Stich, S., eds., The Innate Mind: Culture and Cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Strawson, G. (2000) The unhelpfulness of indeterminism. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 60: 149–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strawson, P. F. (1962) Freedom and resentment. Proceedings of the British Academy 48: 1–25.Google Scholar
Strayhorn, J. M. (2002) Self-control: theory and research. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 41: 7–16CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tancredi, L. (2004) Neuroscience developments and the law. In Garland, Brent, ed., Neuroscience and the Law. New York: Dana Press. 71–113.Google Scholar
Taylor, C. (1989) Sources of the Self: The Making of the Modern Identity. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Taylor, C. (1991) The Ethics of Authenticity. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Taylor, C. (1995) The politics of recognition. In Philosophical Arguments. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 225–6.Google Scholar
Teasdale, T. W. and Owen, D. R. (2005) A long-term rise and recent decline in intelligence test performance: the Flynn Effect in reverse. Personality and Individual Differences 39: 837–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, R. K., Oden, D. L. and Boysen, S. T. 1997, Language-naive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) judge relations between relations in a conceptual matching-to-sample task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 23: 31–43.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A. (1998) Empathy and its origins in early development. In S. Braten, ed., Intersubjective Communication and Emotion in Early Ontogeny. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 144–57.
Thomson, J. J. (1976) Killing, letting die, and the Trolley Problem. The Monist 59: 204–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomson, J. J. (1986) The trolley problem. In Rights, Restitution, and Risk. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 94–116.Google Scholar
Trivers, R. (1985) Social Evolution. Menlo Park, Calif.: Benjamin/Cummings Publishing.Google Scholar
Tse, W. S. and Bond, A. J. (2002) Serotonergic intervention affects both social dominance and affiliative behaviour. Psychopharmacology, 161, 324–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tse, W. S. and Bond, A. J. (2003) Reboxetine promotes social bonding in healthy volunteers. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 17, 189–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turiel, E. (1977) Distinct conceptual and developmental domains: Social convention and morality. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 25: 77–116.Google ScholarPubMed
Turiel, E. (1983) The development of social knowledge: morality and convention. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Turiel, E., Killen, M. and Helwig, C. C. (1987) Morality: its structure, functions and vagaries. In Kagan, J. and Lamb, S., eds., The Emergence of Morality in Young Children. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 155–244.Google Scholar
Tversky, A. and Kahneman, D. (1983) Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: the conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychology Review 90: 293–315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vaiva, G., Ducrocq, F., Jezequel, K., (2003) Immediate treatment with Propranolol decreases posttraumatic stress disorder two months after trauma. Biological Psychiatry 54: 947–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valmaggia, L. R., Gaag, M., Tarrier, N., Pijnenborg, M. and Slooff, C. J. (2005) Cognitive-behavioural therapy for refractory psychotic symptoms of schizophrenia resistant to atypical antipsychotic medication: randomised controlled trial. British Journal of Psychiatry 186: 324–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Venneri, A. and Shanks, M. F. (2004) Belief and awareness: reflections on a case of persistent anosognosia. Neuropsychologia 42: 230–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vuilleumier, P., Schwartz, S., Husain, M., Clarke, K. and Driver, J. (2001) Implicit processing and learning of visual stimuli in parietal extinction and neglect. Cortex 37: 741–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vohs, K. D. and Heatherton, T. F. (2000) Self-regulatory failure: a resource-depletion approach. Psychological Science 11: 249–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vohs, K. D. and Faber, R. (2003) Self-regulation and impulsive spending patterns. In Keller, P. A. and Rook, D. W., eds., Advances in Consumer Research30 (1): 125–6.Google Scholar
Vohs, K. D., Baumeister, R. F., Twenge, J. M. Tice, D. M. and Crocker J. (unpublished) Self-regulation and choice.
Wansink, B., Kent, R. J. and Hoch, S. J. (1998) An anchoring and adjustment model of purchase quantity decisions. Journal of Marketing Research 35: 71–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, P. J. and Andrews, P. W. (2002) Toward a revised evolutionary adaptationist analysis of depression: the social navigation hypothesis. Journal of Affective Disorders 72: 1–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wegner, D. (2002) The Illusion of Conscious Will. Cambridge, Mass.: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wegner, D. (2004) Précis of The Illusion of Conscious Will. Behavioral and Bain Sciences 27: 649–59.Google ScholarPubMed
Wegner, D. M. (2005) Who is the controller of controlled processes? In Hassin, R., Uleman, J. S. and Bargh, J. A., eds., The New Unconscious. New York: Oxford University Press, 19–36.Google Scholar
Wegner, D. and Wheatley, T. (1999) Apparent mental causation: sources of the experience of will. American Psychologist 54: 480–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiskopf, D. (submitted) Pabrolling the Mind's boundaries.
Weiskrantz, L. (1986) Blindsight. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Weithman, P. (2005) Review of John Christman and Joel Anderson, eds., Autonomy and the Challenges to Liberalism. Notre Dame Philosophical Reviews. 09.12.2005 <http://ndpr.nd.edu/review.cfm?id=3921>
Wheatley, T. and Haidt, J. (2005) Hypnotic disgust makes moral judgments more severe. Psychological Science 16: 780–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whetstone, T. and Cross, M. D. (1998) Control of conscious contents in directed forgetting and thought suppression. PSYCHE 4: 16. http://psyche.cs.monash.edu.au/v4/psyche-4–16-whetstone.htmlGoogle Scholar
Wilkinson, G. S. (1990) Food sharing in vampire bats. Scientific American 262: 64–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, B. (1981) Moral Luck: Philosophical Papers 1973–1980. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 20–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, R. A. (2004) Boundaries of the Mind: The Individual in the Fragile Sciences, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, R. A. (forthcoming) Meaning making and the mind of the externalist. In Menary, R.. The Extended Mind, Aldershot: Ashgate.
Wilson, R. A. and Clark, A. (forthcoming) How to situate cognition: letting nature take its course. In Aydede, M. and Robbins, P., eds., The Cambridge Handbook of Situated Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Wolpe, P. R., Foster, K. R. and Langleben, D. D. (2005) Emerging neurotechnologies for lie-detection: promises and perils. American Journal of Bioethics 5: 39–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolpert, L. (2001) Malignant Sadness: The Anatomy of Depression. London: Faber and Faber.Google Scholar
Wong, D. (1984) Moral Relativity. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Young, L., Cushman, F. A., Adolphs, R., Tranel, D. and Hauser, M. D. (2006) Does emotion mediate the relationship between an action's moral status and its intentional status? Neuropsychological evidence. Journal of Cognition and Culture 6: 265–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zaltman, G. (2003) How Customers Think: Essential Insights in the Mind of the Market. Boston, Mass.: Harvard Business Press.Google Scholar
Zeman, A. (2003) Consciousness: A User's Guide. London: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Zhu, J. (2004) Locating volition. Consciousness and Cognition 13: 302–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ziemann, U., Muellbacher, W., Hallett, M. and Cohen, L. G. (2001) Modulation of practice-dependent plasticity in human motor cortex. Brain 124: 1171–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adams, F. and Aizawa, K. (2001) The bounds of cognition. Philosophical Psychology 14: 43–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adams, F. and Aizawa, K. (forthcoming) Clark missed the mark: Andy Clark on intrinsic content and extended cognition. Analysis.
Aglioti, S., DeSouza, J. E. X. and Goodale, M. A. (1995) Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand. Current Biology, 5, 679–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ainslie, G. (2000) A research-based theory of addictive motivation. Law and Philosophy 19: 77–115.Google Scholar
Ainslie, G. (2001) Breakdown of Will. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alexander, M. P., Stuss, D. T. and Benson, D. F. (1979) Capgras' syndrome: a reduplicative phenomenon. Neurology 29: 334–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alloy, L. B. and Abramson, L. Y. (1979) The judgment of contingency in depressed and nondepressed students: sadder but wiser?Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 441–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alloy, L. B. (1995) Depressive realism: sadder but wiser?The Harvard Mental Health Letter, 11, 4–5.Google Scholar
Anderson, S. W., Damasio, H., Tranel, D. and Damasio, A. R. (2000) Long-term sequelae of prefrontal cortex damage acquired in early childhood. Developmental Neuropsychology, 18, 281–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Angell, M. (2004) The Truth About the Drug Companies: How They Deceive Us and What to Do About It. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
Anton, G. (1899) Über die Selbstwahrnehmung der Herderkrankungen durch den Kranken bei Rindenblindheit und Rindentaubheit. Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkranleheiten, 32: 86–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Archibald, S. J., Mateer, C. A., Kerns, K. A. (2001) Utilization behavior: clinical manifestations and neurological mechanisms. Neuropsychology Review 11: 117–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Axelrod, R. (1984) The Evolution of Cooperation. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Baard, E. (2003) The guilt-free soldier. Village Voice, January 22–8.
Baars, B. J. (1988) A Cognitive Theory of Consciousness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Baars, B. J. (1997) In the theatre of consciousness: global workspace theory, a rigorous scientific theory of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies 4: 292–309.Google Scholar
Bargh, J. A. and Chartrand, T. L. (1999) The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist, 54: 462–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnes, A. (1997) Seeing Through Self-Deception. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Batson, C. D., Engel, C. L. and Fridell, S. R. (1999) Value judgments: testing the somatic-marker hypothesis using false physiological feedback. Personality and social psychology bulletin 25: 1021–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M. and Tice, D. M. (1998) Ego-depletion: is the active self a limited resource?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74: 1252–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumeister, R. F. (2002) Ego depletion and self-control failure: an energy model of the self's executive function. Self and Identity 1: 129–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bavidge, M., and A. J. Cole. (1995) Is psychopathy a moral concept? In Almond, B., ed., Introducing Applied Ethics. Oxford: Blackwell, 185–196.Google Scholar
Bayne, T. (2006) Phenomenology and the feeling of doing: Wegner on the conscious will. In Pockett, S., Banks, W. P. and Gallagher, S., eds., Does Consciousness Cause behavior? An Investigation of the Nature of Volition. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 169–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bayne, T. and Levy, N. (2005) Amputees by choice: body integrity identity disorder and the ethics of amputation. Journal of Applied Philosophy 22: 75–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bayne, T. and Pacherie, E. (2005) In defence of the doxastic conception of delusion. Mind and Language 20: 163–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bealer, G. (1998) Intuition and the autonomy of philosophy. In DePaul, M. R. and Ramsey, W., eds., Rethinking Intuition: The Psychology of Intuition and its Role in Philosophical Inquiry. Lanham, Md.: Rowman & Littlefield Publishers, 201–39.Google Scholar
Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R. and Damasio, H. (2003) Role of the amygdala in decision-making. Annals of the New York Academy of Science 985: 356–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D. and Damasio, A. R. (1997) Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science 275: 1293–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D. and Damasio, A. (2005) The Iowa Gambling Task and the somatic marker hypothesis: some questions and answers. Trends in Cognitive Science 9: 159–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berti, A., Làdavas, E. and Della Corte, M. (1996) Anosognosia for hemiplegia, neglect dyslexia, and drawing neglect: clinical findings and theoretical considerations. Journal of the International Neurological Society 2: 426–40.Google ScholarPubMed
Bishop, M. A. and Trout, J. D. (2005) Epistemology and the Psychology of Human Judgment. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bisiach, E. and Geminiani, G. (1991) Anosognosia related to hemiplegia and hemianopia. In Prigatano, G. P. and Schacter, D. L., eds., Awareness of Deficit After Brain Injury: Clinical and Theoretical Issues. New York: Oxford University Press, 17–39.Google Scholar
Bisiach, E., Vallar, G., Perani, D., Papagno, C. and Berti, A. (1986) Unawareness of disease following lesions of the right hemisphere: anosognosia for hemiplegia and anosognosia for hemianopia. Neuropsychologia, 24: 471–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R. (1995) A cognitive development approach to morality: investigating the psychopath. Cognition 57: 1–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R. (1997) Moral reasoning and the child with psychopathic tendencies. Personality and Individual Differences 26: 731–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blair, J.Mitchell, D. and Blair, K. (2005) The Psychopath: Emotion and the Brain. Malden, Mass.: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Block, N. (1997) On a confusion about a function of consciousness. In Block, N., Flanagan, O. and Güzeldere, G., eds., The Nature of Consicousness: Philosophical Debates. Cambridge, Mass: The MIT Press, 375–415.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. (2004) Descartes' Baby. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Blumenthal, J. A.et al. (1999) Effects of exercise training on older patients with major depression. Archives of Internal Medicine 159: 2349–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bourdieu, P. (1986) Forms of capital. In Richardson, J. G., ed., Handbook of Theory and Research for the Sociology of Education. New York: Greenwood Press, 241–58.Google Scholar
Bourtchouladze, R. (2002) Memories Are Made of This: The Biological Building Blocks of Memory. Frome: Weidenfeld & Nicolson.Google Scholar
Brasil-Neto, J. P., Pascual-Leone, A., Valls-Sole, J., Cohen, L. G. and Hallett, M. (1992) Focal transcranial magnetic stimulation and response bias in a forced-choice task. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 55: 964–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breen, N., Caine, D., Coltheart, M., Hendy, J. and Roberts, C. (2000). Delusional misidentification. Mind and Language 15: 74–110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broad, C. D. (1952) Ethics and the History of Philosophy. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Broughton, R., Billings, R., Cartwright, R.et al. (1994) Homicidal somnambulism: a case report. Sleep 17: 253–64.Google ScholarPubMed
Buchanan, A., Brock, D. W., Daniels, N. and Wikler, D. (2000) From Chance to Choice: Genetics and Justice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butler, J. (1970) Butler's Fifteen Sermons Preached at the Rolls Chapel. London: SPCK.Google Scholar
Cabeza, R.Rao, S. M., Wagner, A. D., Mayer, A. R. and Schacter, D. L. (2001) Can medial temporal lobe regions distinguish true from false? An event-related functional MRI study of veridical and illusory recognition memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 98: 4805–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cahill, L., Prins, B., Weber, M. and McGaugh, J. L. (1994) Beta-adrenergic activation and memory for emotional events. Nature 371: 702–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Callaway, E., Halliday, R., Perez-Stable, E. J., Coates, T. J. and Hauck, W. W. (1991) Propranolol and response bias: an extension of findings reported by Corwinet al. Biological Psychiatry 30: 739–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Canli, T. (2006) When genes and brains unite: ethical implications of genomic neuroimaging. In Illes, J., ed., Neuroethics: Defining the Issues in Theory, Practice, and Policy. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 169–83.Google Scholar
Canli, T., Zhao, Z., Desmond, J. E., Kang, E., Gross, J. and Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2001) An fMRI study of personality influences on brain reactivity to emotional stimuli. Behavioral Neuroscience 115: 33–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Canli, T., Sivers, H., Gotlib, I. H. and Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2002) Amygdala activation to happy faces as a function of extraversion. Science 296: 2191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cappa, S., Sterzi, R., Vallar, G. and Bisiach, E. (1987) Remission of hemineglect and anosognosia during vestibular stimulation. Neuropsychologia 25: 775–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carruthers, P. (2005) Why the question of animal consciousness might not matter very much. Philosophical Psychology 18: 83–102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caspi, A.McClay, J., Moffitt, T. E.et al. (2002) Evidence that the cycle of violence in maltreated children depends on genotype. Science 297: 851–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charland, L. C. (2002) Cynthia's dilemma: consenting to heroin prescriptionAmerican Journal of Bioethics 2: 37–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Churchland, P. S. (2003) Self-representation in nervous systems. Annals of the New York Academy of Science 1001: 31–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, A. (1997) Being There: Putting Brain, Body, and World Together Again. Cambridge, Mass.: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2002) Minds, brains and tools. In H. Clapin, ed., Philosophy of Mental Representation. Oxford: Clarendon Press, 66–90.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2003) Natural-Born Cyborgs: Minds, Technologies, and the Future of Human Intelligence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2005) Intrinsic content, active memory and the extended mind. Analysis 65: 1–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, A. (2006) Memento's revenge: The extended mind, extended. In Menary, R., ed., The Extended Mind. Aldershot: Ashgate.Google Scholar
Clark, A. and Chalmers, D. (1998) The extended mind. Analysis 58: 7–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, T. W. (1999) Fear of mechanism: a compatibilist critique of “the volitional brain”. Journal of Consciousness Studies 6: 279–93.Google Scholar
Coady, C. A. J. (1992) Testimony: A Philosophical Study. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cocchini, G., Beschin, N. and Della Sala, S. (2002) Chronic anosognosia: a case report and theoretical account. Neuropsychologia 40: 2030–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, A. and Leckman, J. F. (1992) Sensory phenomena associated with Gilles de la Tourette's syndrome. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 53: 319–23.Google ScholarPubMed
Cohen, D. J. and Leckman, J. F. (1999) Introduction: the self under síege. In Leckman, J. F. and Cohen, D. J., eds., Tourette's Syndrome – Tics, Obsessions, Compulsions. New York, John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 1–19.Google Scholar
Corwin, J., Peselow, E., Feenan, K, Rotrosen, J. and Fieve, R. (1990) Disorders of decision in affective disease: An effect of β-adrenergic dysfunction?Biological Psychiatry 27: 813–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cummins, R. (1998) Reflections on reflective equilibrium. In M. R. DePaul and W. Ramsey, Rethinking Intuition. Leuham Md.: Rowman & Littlefield Publishers, 113–27.Google Scholar
Currie, G. (2000) Imagination, delusion and hallucinations. Mind and Language, 15, 168–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cushman, F. A., Young, L. and Hauser, M. D. (2006) The role of conscious reasoning and intuition in moral judgments: testing three principles of harm. Psychological Science 17: 1082–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Damasio, A. (1994) Descartes' Error: Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain. London: Picador.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (1996) The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society Lond B 351: 1413–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Damasio, A. R. (1999) The Feeling of What Happens. New York: Harcourt Brace.Google Scholar
Damasio, H., Grabowski, T., Frank, R., Galaburda, A. M. and Damasio, A. R. (1994) The return of Phineas Gage: clues about the brain from the skull of a famous patient. Science 264: 1102–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daniels, N. (1985) Just Health Care. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daniels, N. (2003) Reflective equilibrium. In E. N. Zalta ed., The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/sum2003/entries/reflective-equilibrium/.
Darnton, R. (1985) The Great Cat Massacre: And Other Episodes in French Cultural History. New York: Vintage Books.Google Scholar
Dasgupta, N. (2004) Implicit ingroup favoritism, outgroup favoritism, and their behavioral manifestations. Social Justice Research 17: 143–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, D. (1986) Deception and division. In Elster, J., ed., The Multiple Self. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 79–92.Google Scholar
Davies, M., Aimola Davies, A. and Coltheart, M. (2005) Anosognosia and the two-factor theory of delusions. Mind and Language 20: 209–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1983) The Extended Phenotype: The Long Reach of the Gene. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (2006) Let's all stop beating Basil's car. The Edge. <http://www.edge.org/q2006/q06_9.html#dawkins>
DeGrazia, D. (1996) Taking Animals Seriously: Mental Life and Moral Status. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeGrazia, D. (2000) Prozac, enhancement, and self-creation. Hastings Center Report 30: 34–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeGrazia, D. (2005) Human Identity and Bioethics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, D. and Naccache, L. (2001) Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition 79: 1–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennett, D. (1978) Brain writing and mind reading. In Brainstorms: Philosophical Essays on Mind and Psychology. London: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. (1984) Elbow Room: The Varieties of Free Will Worth Wanting. Cambridge, Mass.: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. (1990) The myth of original intentionality. In Said, K. A. Mohyeldin, Newton-Smith, W. H., Viale, R. and Wilkes, K. V., eds., Modeling the Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 43–62.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. (1991) Consciousness Explained. London: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. (1996) Kinds of Minds. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. (2001) Are we explaining consciousness yet?Cognition 79: 221–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennett, D. (2003) Freedom Evolves. London: Allen Lane.Google Scholar
DePaul, M. (1998) Why bother with reflective equilibrium? In DePaul, and Ramsey, , eds., Rethinking Intuition. Lanham, Md.: Rowman & Littlefield Publishers, 293–309.Google Scholar
DePaulo, B. M. (1994) Spotting lies: can humans learn to do better?Current Directions in Psychological Science 3: 83–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waal, F. (1996) Good Natured: The Origins of Right and Wrong in Humans and Other Animals. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Dickerson, F. B. (2000) Cognitive Behavioural Psychotherapy for Schizophrenia: A Review of Recent Empirical Studies. Schizophrenia Research 43: 71–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dijksterhuis, A., Bos, M. W., Nordgren, L. F. and Baaren, R. B. (2006) On making the right choice: the deliberation-without-attention effect. Science 311: 1005–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dienstbier, R. A. (1978) Attribution, socialization, and moral decision making. In Harvey, J. H., Ickes, W. J. and Kidd, R. F., eds., New Directions in Attribution Research, Vol. 2. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 181–206.Google Scholar
Dienstbier, R. A. and Munster, P. O. (1971) Cheating as a function of the labelling of natural arousal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 17: 208–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donald, M. (1991) Origins of the Modern Mind. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Doricchi, F. and Galati, G. (2000) Implicit semantic evaluation of object symmetry and contralesional visual denial in a case of left unilateral neglect with damage of the dorsal paraventricular white matter. Cortex 36: 337–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Double, R. (2004) How to accept Wegner's illusion of conscious will and still defend moral responsibility. Behavior and Philosophy 32: 479–91.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. (1996) Grooming, Gossip, and the Evolution of Language. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Edwards, D. H. and Kravitz, E. A. (1997) Serotonin, social status and aggression. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 7: 811–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P. (2003) Darwin, deception, and facial expression. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1000: 205–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P., O'Sullivan, M. and Frank, M. G. (1999) A few can catch a liar. Psychological Science 10: 263–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekstrom, L. W. (1999) Free Will: A Philosophical Study. Boulder, Co.: Westview.Google Scholar
Elliott, C. (1998) The tyranny of happiness: ethics and cosmetic psychopharmacology. In Parens, E., ed., Enhancing Human Traits: Ethical and Social Implications. Washington, D. C.: Georgetown University Press, 177–188.Google Scholar
Elliott, C. (2000) A new way to be mad, The Atlantic Monthly 286, No. 6, December.Google Scholar
Elliott, C. (2002) Who holds the leash?American Journal of Bioethics 2: 48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, H. D. and Young, A. W. (1990) Accounting for delusional misidentifications. British Journal of Psychiatry 157, 239–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, H. D., Young, A. W., Quayle, A. H. and Pauw, K. W. (1997) Reduced autonomic responses to faces in Capgras delusion. Proceedings of the Royal Society: Biological Sciences, B264, 1085–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elster, J. (1999) Strong Feelings: Emotion, Addiction and Human Behavior. Cambridge, Mass.: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Elster, J. (2000) Ulysses Unbound. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engel, A. K., Fries, P., König, P.Brecht, M. and Singer, W. (1999) Temporal binding, binocular rivalry, and consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition 8: 128–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Estlinger, P. J., Warner, G. C., Grattan, L. M. and Easton, J. D. (1991) Frontal lobe utilization behavior associated with paramedian thalamic infarction. Neurology, 41: 450–2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fahle, M. (2003) Failures of visual analysis: scotoma, agnosia, and neglect. In Fahle, M. and Greenlee, M., eds., The Neuropsychology of Vision. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 179–258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fairclough, S. H. and Houston, K. (2004) A metabolic measure of mental effort. Biological Psychology 66, 177–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farah, M. J. (2002) Emerging ethical issues in neuroscience. Nature Neuroscience 5: 1123–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farah, M. J. (2005) Neuroethics: the practical and the philosophical. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9, 34–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farah, M. J. and Wolpe, P. R. (2004) New neuroscience technologies and their ethical implications. Hastings Center Report 34: 35–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farah, M. J., Noble, K. G. and Hurt, H. (2006) Poverty, privilege, and brain development: empirical findings and ethical implications. In Illes, J., ed., Neuroethics: Defining the Issues in Theory, Practice, and Policy. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 277–87.Google Scholar
Farwell, L. A. and Smith, S. S. (2001) Using brain MERMER testing to detect concealed knowledge despite efforts to conceal. Journal of Forensic Sciences 46 (1): 1–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fingarette, H. (1988) Heavy Drinking: The Myth of Alcoholism as a Disease. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
First, M. B. (2005) Desire for amputation of a limb: paraphilia, psychosis, or a new type of identity disorder. Psychological Medicine 35: 919–928.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Firth, R. (1952) Ethical absolutism and the ideal observer theory. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 12: 317–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flanagan, O. (1996a) Neuroscience, agency, and the meaning of life. In Flanagan, O., ed., Self-Expressions: Mind, Morals, and the Meaning of Life. New York: Oxford University Press, 53–64.Google Scholar
Flanagan, O. (1996b) I remember you. In Self-Expressions: Mind, Morals, and the Meaning of Life. New York: Oxford University Press, 88–98.Google Scholar
Flynn, J. R. (1994) IQ gains over time. In Sternberg, R. J., ed., Encyclopedia of Human Intelligence. New York: Macmillan, 617–23.Google Scholar
Foddy, B. and Savaluscu, J. (2006) Addiction and autonomy: can addicted people consent to the prescription of their drug?Bioethics 20: 1–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foot, P. (1978) The problem of abortion and the doctrine of the double effect. In Virtues and Vices. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Frank, R. H. (1988) Passions Within Reason: The Strategic Role of the Emotions. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Freedman, C. (1998) Aspirin for the mind? Some ethical worries about psychopharmacology. In Parens, E., ed., Enhancing Human Traits: Ethical and Social Implications. Washington, D. C.: Georgetown University Press, 135–50.Google Scholar
Gailliot, M. T., Baumeister, R. F., DeWall, C. N., Maner, J. K. and Plant, E. A. (2007) Self-control relies on glucose as a limited energy source: willpower is more than a metaphor. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 92: 325–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garland, B. (2004) Neuroscience and the Law. New York: Dana Press.Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S. (1985) The Social Brain. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S. (1992) Nature's Mind. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S. (2005) The Ethical Brain. New York: Dana Press.Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. (1991) How mental systems believe. American Psychologist 46: 107–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, D. (1993) The assent of man: mental representation and the control of belief. In Wegner, D. M. and Pennebaker, J., eds., The Handbook of Mental Control. New York: Prentice-Hall, 57–87.Google Scholar
Glannon, W. (2006a) Psychopharmacology and memory. Journal of Medical Ethics 32: 74–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glannon, W. (2006b) Bioethics on the Brain. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glover, J. (1999) Humanity: A Moral History of the Twentieth Century. London: Pimlico.Google Scholar
Goldman, A. and Pust, P. (1998) Philosophical theory and intuitional evidence. In M. R. DePaul and W. Ramsey, Rethinking Intuition. Lenham, Md.: Rowmam and Littlefield Publishers, 179–97.Google Scholar
Goodale, M. A. and Milner, A. D. (2004) Sight Unseen: An Exploration of Conscious and Unconscious Vision. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. (1988) The streak of streaks. New York Review of Books 35: 8–12.Google Scholar
Greely, H. T. (2004) Prediction, litigation, privacy, and property: some possible legal and social implications of advances in neuroscience. In Garland, B., ed., Neuroscience and the Law: Brain, Mind, and the Scales of Justice. New York: Dana Press, 114–56.Google Scholar
Greene, J.Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M. and Cohen, J. D. (2001) An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science 293: 2105–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greene, J. (2003) From neural “is” to moral “ought”: what are the moral implications of neuroscientific moral psychology?Nature Reviews Neuroscience 4: 847–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greene, J. (2005) Cognitive neuroscience and the structure of the moral mind. In Carruthers, P., Laurence, S. and Stich, S., eds., The Innate Mind: Structure and Content. New York: Oxford University Press, 338–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greene. J. (forthcoming) The secret joke of Kant's soul. In Sinnott-Armstrong, ed., Moral Psychology.
Greene, J. and Haidt, J. (2004) How (and where) does moral judgment work?Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6: 517–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haggard, P. and Libet, B. (2001) Conscious intention and brain activity. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8: 47–63.Google Scholar
Haidt, J. (2001) The emotional dog and its rational tail: a social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review 108: 814–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haidt, J. (2003) The emotional dog does learn new tricks: a reply to Pizarro and Bloom. Psychological Review 110: 197–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haidt, J., Koller, S. H. and Dias, M. G. (1993) Affect, culture, and morality, or is it wrong to eat your dog?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 65: 613–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haji, I. (2002) Deontic Morality and Control. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamilton, A. (forthcoming) Against the belief model of delusion. In Chung, M., Fulford, K. W. M. and Graham, G., eds., The Philosophical Understanding of Schizophrenia. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Haugeland, J. (1998) Mind embodied and embedded. In Having Thought. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 207–37.
Hauser, M. D. (2006) The liver and moral organ. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 1: 214–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hauser, M. D., Cushman, F. A., Young, L., Kang-Xing Jin, R. and Mikhail, J. (2007). A dissociation between moral judgments and justifications. Mind and Language 22: 1–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haynes, J.-D., Sakai, K., Rees, G., Gilbert, S., Frith, C and Passnigham, R. E. (2007) Reading hidden intentions in the human brain. Current Biology 17: 323–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hazlett-Stevens, H. and Craske, M. G. (2004) Brief cognitive-behavioral therapy: definition and scientific foundations. In Bond, F. and Dryden, W., eds., Handbook Of Brief Cognitive Behaviour Therapy. Chichester, UK: JohnWiley and Sons, Ltd, 1–20.Google Scholar
Heilman, K. M., Barrett, A. M. and Adair, J. C. (1998) Possible mechanisms of anosognosia: a defect in self-awareness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B 353: 1903–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirstein, W. (2000) Self-deception and confabulation. Philosophy of Science 67: S418–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirstein, W. (2005) Brain Fiction: Self-Deception and Confabulation. Cambridge, Mass.: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Holton, R. (2004) Rational Resolve, Philosophical Review, 113: 507–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horowitz, T. (1998) Philosophical intuitions and psychological theory. In DePaul, M. and Ramsey, W., eds., Rethinking Intuition. Lanham, Md.: Rowmam & Littlefield Publishers, 143–60.Google Scholar
Hunt, E. (1995) The role of intelligence in modern society. American Scientist 83: 356–68.Google Scholar
Illes, J. (2003) Neuroethics in a new era of neuroimaging. American Journal of Neuroradiology 24: 1739–1741.Google Scholar
Illes, J., Racine, E. and Kirschen, M. P. (2006) A picture is worth 1000 words, but which 1000? In Illes, Judy, ed., Neuroethics: Defining the Issues in Theory, Practice, and Policy. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 149–68.Google Scholar
Jack, A. I., and Shallice, T, (2001) Introspective physicalism as an approach to the science of consciousness. Cognition 79: 161–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, W. (1890) Principles of Psychology. New York: Henry Holt and Company.Google Scholar
Jehkonen, M., Ahonen, J.-P., Dastidar, P., Koivisto, A.-M., Laippala, P. and Vilkki, J. (2000) Unawareness of deficits after right hemisphere stroke: double dissociation of anosognosias. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica 102: 378–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, K. (1999) Second-hand moral knowledge. The Journal of Philosophy 96: 55–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, K. (2004) Emotional rationality as practical rationality. In Calhoun, C., ed., Setting the Moral Compass: Essays by Women Philosophers. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 333–52.Google Scholar
Joyce, R. (2001) The Myth of Morality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joyce, R. (2006) The Evolution of Morality. Cambridge, Mass: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Juengst, E. T. (1998) What does enhancement mean? In Parens, E., ed., Enhancing Human Traits: Ethical and Social Implications. Washington, D. C.: Georgetown University Press, 29–47.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., Slovic, P. and Tversky, A. (1982) Judgement Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kamitani, Y. and Tong, F. (2005) Decoding the visual and subjective contents of the human brain. Nature Neuroscience 8: 679–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kane, R. (1996) The Significance of Free Will. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kane, R. (2005) Remarks on the psychology of free will. Presented at the 31st Annual Meeting of The Society Of Philosophy And Psychology, Wake Forest University, Winston-Salem, North Carolina, June 9–12, 2005. Available online: <http://gfp.typepad.com/online_papers/2005/07/robert_kane.html>
Kaplan, J. M. (2000) The Limits and Lies of Human Genetic Research: Dangers for Social Policy. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Keitner, G. I. and Cardemil, E. V. (2004) Psychotherapy for chronic depressive disorders. In Alpert, J. E. and Fava, M., eds., Handbook of Chronic Depression: Diagnosis and Therapeutic Management. New York: Marcel Dekker, Inc: 159–81.Google Scholar
Kessler, R. C., Sonnega, A., Bromet, E., Hughes, M. and Nelson, C. B. (1996) Posttraumatic stress disorder in the national comorbidity survey. Archives of General Psychiatry, 52, 1048–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Khantzian, E. J. (1997) The self-medication hypothesis of substance use disorders: a reconsideration and recent applications. Harvard Review of Psychiatry 4: 231–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, R. A., Leckman, J. F., Scahill, L. and Cohen, D. J. (1999) Obsessive-compulsive disorder, anxiety, and depression. In Leckman, J. F. and Cohen, D. J., eds., Tourette's Syndrome – Tics, Obsessions, Compulsions. New York, John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 43–61.Google Scholar
Knutson, B.et al. (1998) Selective alteration of personality and social behavior by serotonergic intervention. American Journal of Psychiatry 155: 373–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Korsgaard, C. (1986) The Sources of Normativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kramer, P. D. (1993) Listening to Prozac. London: Fourth Estate.Google Scholar
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M. and Cuthbert, B. N. (1990) Emotion, attention, and the startle reflex. Psychological Review 97: 377–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langleben, D. D., Schroeder, L., Maldjian, J. A., Gur, R. C., McDonald, S., Ragland, J. D., O'Brien, C. P. and Childress, A. R. (2002) Brain activity during simulated deception: an event-related functional magnetic resonance study. Neuroimage 15: 727–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leckman, J. F., King, R. A. and Cohen, D. J. (1999) Tics and tic disorders. In Leckman, J. F. and Cohen, D. J., eds., Tourette's Syndrome – Tics, Obsessions, Compulsions. New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc., 23–41.Google Scholar
Ledoux, J. (2003) The self: clues from the brain. Annals of the New York Academy of Science 1001: 295–304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leshner, A. (1999) Science-based views of drug addiction and its treatment. Journal of the American Medical Association 282: 314–16.Google ScholarPubMed
Levine, D. N., Calvanio, R. and Rinn, W. E. (1991) The pathogenesis of anosognosia for hemiplegia. Neurology 41: 1770–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, N. (2002a) Self-ownership: defending Marx against Cohen. Social Theory and Practice 28: 77–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, N. (2002b) Moral Relativism: An Introduction. Oxford: Oneworld.Google Scholar
Levy, N. (2004) What Makes Us Moral?Oxford: Oneworld.Google Scholar
Levy, N. (forthcoming) Restrictivism is a covert compatibilism. In Trahahis, N. ed. Essays on Free Will and Moral Responsibility. Cambridge: Cambridge Scholars Press.
Levy, N. and Bayne, T. (2004) Doing without deliberation: automatism, automaticity, and moral accountability. International Review of Psychiatry 16: 209–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, D. (1989) Dispositional theories of values. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, Supplementary Volume 63: 113–37.Google Scholar
Lhermitte, F. (1983) Utilization behavior and its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes. Brain, 106: 237–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lhermitte, F., Pillon, B. and Serdaru, M. (1986) Human autonomy and the frontal lobes: part I. Imitation and utilization behavior: a neuropsychological study of 75 patients. Annals of Neurology, 19: 326–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Libet, B., Gleason, C., Wright, E. and Pearl, D. (1983) Time of unconscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential). Brain 106: 623–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Libet, B. (1999) Do we have free will?Journal of Consciousness Studies 6: 47–57.Google Scholar
Loewenstein, G. (2000) Willpower: a decision theorist's perspective. Law and Philosophy 2000: 51–76.Google Scholar
Loftus, E. F. (1993) The reality of repressed memories. American Psychologist 48: 518–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loftus, E. F. (2003) Make-believe memories. American Psychologist 58: 867–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loftus, E. F. and Pickrell, J. E. (1995) The formation of false memories. Psychiatric Annals 25: 720–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mackenzie, C. and Stoljar, N. eds. (2000) Relational Autonomy: Feminist Perspectives on Autonomy, Agency, and the Social Self. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Maguire, E. A., Gadian, D. G., Johnsrude, I. S.et al. (2000) Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA 97 (8): 4398–403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maia, T. V. and McClelland, J. L. (2004) A re-examination of the evidence for the somatic marker hypothesis. What participants know in the Iowa Gambling Tasle. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 101: 16075–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manninen, B. A. (2006) Medicating the mind: a Kantian analysis of overprescribing psychoactive drugs. Journal of Medical Ethics 32: 100–105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcel, A. J., Tegnér, R. and Nimmo-Smith, I. (2004) Anosognosia for plegia: specificity, extension, partiality and disunity of bodily awareness. Cortex 40: 19–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marshall, J. C. and Halligan, P. W. (1988) Blindsight and insight in visuo-spatial neglect. Nature 336: 766–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masserman, J. H., Wechkin, S. and Terris, W. (1964) “Altruistic” behavior in rhesus monkeys. American Journal of Psychiatry 121: 584–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matte, T. D., Bresnahan, M., Begg, M. D. and Susser, E. (2001) Influence of variation in birth weight within normal range and within sibships on IQ at age 7 years: cohort study. British Medical Journal 323: 310–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCall, S. and Lowe, E. J. (2005) Indeterminist free will. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 70: 681–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGaugh, J. L. (2000) Memory – a century of consolidation. Science 5451, 248–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mele, A. R. (1997) Real self-deception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences20: 91–102. In Campbell, J., O'Rourke, M. and Shier, D., eds., Explanation and Causation: Topics in Contemporary Philosophy. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mele, A. R. (1999) Ultimate responsibility and dumb luck. Social Philosophy and Policy 16: 274–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mele, A. R. (2001) Self-Deception Unmasked. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Mele, A. R. (2007) Decisions, intentions, urges, and free will: why Libet has not shown what he says he has. In Campbell, J., O'Rourke, M. and Shier, D., eds., Expanation and Causation: Topics in Contemporary Philosophy. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Menary, R. (2006) Attacking the bounds of cognition. Philosophical Psychology 19: 329–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Metzinger, T. (2004) Inferences are just folk psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27: 670.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mill, J. S. 1985 [1859] On Liberty. London: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Miller, R. B. (2000) Without intuitions. Metaphilosophy 31: 231–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milner, A. D. and Goodale, M. A. (1995) The Visual Brain in Action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Milner, A. D. and Goodale, M. A. (1998) The visual brain in action. PSYCHE, 4 (12) (http://psyche.cs.monash.edu.au/v4/psyche-4–12-milner.html)
Mischel, W. (1981) Metacognition and the rules of delay. In Flavell, J. H. and Ross, L., eds., Social Cognitive DevelopmentCambridge: Cambridge University Press, 240–71.Google Scholar
Mischel, W., Shoda, Y. and Rodriguez, (1989) Delay of gratification in children. Science 244: 933–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miyake, Y., Mizutani, M. and Yamamura, T. (1993) Event-related potentials as an indicator of detecting information in field polygraph examinations. Polygraph 22: 131–49.Google Scholar
Money, J., Jobaris, R. and Furth, G. (1977) Apotemnophilia: two cases of self-demand amputation as paraphilia. The Journal of Sex Research, 13: 115–125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morse, S. J. (2004) New neuroscience, old problems. In Garland, B., ed., Neuroscience and the Law. New York: Dana Press. 157–98.Google Scholar
Morse, S. J. (2006) Moral and legal responsibility and the new neuroscience. In Illes, J., ed., Neuroethics: Defining the Issues in Theory, Practice, and Policy. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 33–50.Google Scholar
Mottram, D. R., ed., (2003) Drugs in Sport. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Nahmias, E. (2002) When consciousness matters: a critical review of Daniel Wegner's The Illusion of conscious Will. Philosophical Psychology 15: 527–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
National Research Council (2003) The Polygraph and Lie Detection. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.Google Scholar
Neale, J. (2002) Drug Users in Society. New York: Palgrave.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neisser, U. (1997) Rising scores on intelligence tests. American Scientist 85: 440–7.Google Scholar
Neu, J. (2000) A Tear is an Intellectual Thing: the Meanings of Emotion. Oxford, New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nichols, S., Stich, S. and Weinberg, J. M. (2003) Meta-skepticism: meditations in ethno-epistemology. In Luper, S., ed., The Skeptics. Aldershot: Ashgate Publishing, 227–47.Google Scholar
Noakes, T. D., St Clair Gibson, A. and Lamber, E. V. (2004) From catastrophe to complexity: a novel model of integrative central neural regulation of effort and fatigue during exercise in humans. British Journal of Sports Medicine 38: 511–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nozick, R. (1974) Anarchy, State, and Utopia. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Nozick, R. (1981) Philosophical Explanations. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nucci, L. P. (1989) Challenging conventional wisdom about morality: the domain approach to values education. In Nucci, L. P., ed., Moral Development and Character Education: A Dialogue. Berkeley: McCutcheon, 183–203.Google Scholar
Nussbaum, M. C. (2000) Women and Human Development: The Capabilities Approach. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nussbaum, M. C. (2001) Upheavals of Thought: The Intelligence of Emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Connor, T. (2000) Persons and Causes: The Metaphysics of Free Will. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Olson, E. T. (1999) There is no problem of the self. In Gallagher, S. and Shear, J., eds., Models of the Self. Exeter: Imprint Academic, 49–61.Google Scholar
Osborne, L. (2003) Savant for a day. New York Times. June 22.Google Scholar
Parens, E. (1998) Is better always good? The enhancement project. In Parens, E., ed., Enhancing Human Traits: Ethical and Social Implications. Washington, D.C.: Georgetown University Press, 1–28.Google Scholar
Parens, E. (2005) Authenticity and ambivalence: toward understanding the enhancement debate. Hastings Center Report 35 (3): 34–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pereboom, D. (2001) Living Without Free Will. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phelps, E. A., O'Connor, K. J., Cunningham, , et al. (2000) Performance on indirect measures of race evaluation predicts amygdala activity, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12: 1–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phillips, K. (1996) The Broken Mirror: Understanding and Treating Body Dysmorphic Disorder. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1994) The Language Instinct: How the Mind Creates Language. New York: HarperCollins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pitman, R. K. and Delahanty, D. L. (2005) Conceptually driven pharmacologic approaches to acute trauma. CNS Spectrums 10: 99–106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pitman, R. K., Sanders, K. M., Zusman, R. M., et al. (2002) Pilot study of secondary prevention of posttraumatic stress disorder with Propanolol. Biological Psychiatry 51: 189–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pockett, S. (2004) Does consciousness cause behaviour?Journal of Consciousness Studies 11: 23–40.Google Scholar
Pogge, T. (2002) World Poverty and Human Rights. Cambridge: CPolity Press.Google Scholar
Pollack, W. (1998) Real Boys: Rescuing Our Sons from the Myths of Boyhood. New York: Henry Holt.Google Scholar
Prinz, J. (2005) Imitation and moral development. In Hurley, S. and Chater, N., eds. Perspectives on Imitation: From Cognitive Neuroscience to Social Science, vol. 2. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press, 267–82.Google Scholar
Pust, J. (2000) Intuitions as Evidence. New York: Garland Publishing.Google Scholar
Quian Quiroga, R., Reddy, L.Kreiman, G., Koch, C. and Fried, I. (2005) Invariant visual representation by single neurons in the human brain. Nature 435: 1102–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S. (1996) The evolutionary biology of self-deception, laughter, dreaming and depression. Medical Hypotheses 47: 347–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramachandran, V. S., Altschuler, E. L. and Hillyer, S. (1997) Mirror agnosia. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 264: 645–647.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S. and Blakeslee, S. (1998) Phantoms in the Brain. London: Fourth Estate.Google Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S. and Hirstein, W. (1998) The perception of phantom limbs. Brain 121: 1603–1630.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rawls, J. (1971) A Theory of Justice. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Rawls, J. (1993) Political Liberalism. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Rayner, K. (1998) Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 372–422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reznek, L. (1997) Evil or Ill? Justifying the Insanity Defence. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Rice, G. E. and Gainer, P. (1962) “Altruism” in the Albino Rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 55: 123–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, R. D., Lancaster, M., Wakeley, J. and Bhagwagar, Z. (2004) Effects of beta-adrenoceptor blockade on components of human decision-making. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 172: 157–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rose, S. (2005) The Future of the Brain: The Promise and Perils of Tomorrow's Neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rosenfeld, J. P.Soskins, M., Bosh, G. and Ryan, A. (2004) Simple effective countermeasures to P300-based tests of detection of concealed information. Psychophysiology 4: 205–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenthal, D. M. (2002) The timing of conscious states. Consciousness and Cognition 11: 215–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roskies, A. (2002) Neuroethics for the new millenium. Neuron 35: 21–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roskies, A. (2003) Are ethical judgments intrinsically motivational? Lessons from “acquired sociopathy”. Philosophical Psychology 16: 51–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roskies, A. (2006) A case study in neuroethics: the nature of moral judgment. In Illes, J., ed., Neuroethics: Defining the Issues in Theory, Practice, and Policy. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 17–32.Google Scholar
Rowlands, M. (1999) The Body in Mind: Understanding Cognitive Processes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rowlands, M. (2003) Externalism: Putting Mind and World Back Together Again. Chesham: Acumen.Google Scholar
Rupert, R. D. (2004) Challenges to the hypothesis of extended cognition. Journal of Philosophy CI: 389–428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sacks, O. (1985) The Man Who Mistook his Wife for a Hat. London: Picador.Google Scholar
Safire, W. (2002) The but-what-if factor. The New York Times, May 16.Google Scholar
Salmon, P. (2001) Effects of physical exercise on anxiety, depression, and sensitivity to stress: a unifying theory. Clinical Psychology Review 21: 33–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sartre, J. -P. (1956) Being and Nothingness. New York: Washington Square Press.Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, S., Taylor, T. J. and Shanker, S. G. (1999) Apes, Language, and the Human Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Saver, J. L. and Damasio, A. R. (1991) Preserved access and processing of social knowledge in a patient with acquired sociopathy due to ventromedial frontal damage. Neuropsychologia 29: 1241–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savulescu, J., Foddy, B. and Clayton, M. (2004) Why we should allow performance enhancing drugs in sport. British Journal Sports Medicine 38: 666–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scanlon, T. (1998) What We Owe to Each Other. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L. (1996) Searching for Memory: The Brain, the Mind, and the Past. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Schelling, T. C. (1992) Self-command: a new discipline. In Loewenstein, G. and Elster, J., eds., Choice Over Time. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Schechtman, M. (1996) The Constitution of Selves. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Schmeichel, B. J. and Baumeister, R. F. (2004) Self-regulatory strength. In Baumeister, R. F. and Vohs, K. D., eds., Handbook of Self-Regulation. New York: The Guilford Press, 84–98.Google Scholar
Schopp, R. F. (1991) Automatism, insanity, and the psychology of criminal Responsibility: A Philosophical Inquiry. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schultz, R. T., Carter, A. S., Schaill, L. and Leckman, J. F. (1999) Neurophysiological findings. In Leckman, J. F. and Cohen, D. J., eds., Tourette's Syndrome – Tics, Obsessions, Compulsions. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 80–103.Google Scholar
Scott, K. (2000) Voluntary amputee ran disability site. The Guardian, February 7.Google Scholar
Schwitzgebel, E. (2002) A phenomenal, dispositional account of beliefNoûs, 36, 249–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1994) The Rediscovery of the Mind. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Sententia, W. (2004) Neuroethical considerations: cognitive liberty and converging technologies for improving human cognition. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1013: 221–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simons, D. J. and Levin, D. T. (1998) Failure to detect changes to people during a real-world interaction. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 5: 644–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singer, P. (1974) Sidgwick and reflective equilibrium, The Monist 58: 490–517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singer, P. (1990) Animal Liberation (2nd edn.). New York: Avon Books.Google Scholar
Singer, P. (2005) Ethics and intuitions. Journal of Ethics 9: 331–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sinnott-Armstrong, W. (2006) Moral intuitionism meets empirical psychology. In Timmons, M. and Horgan, T., eds., Metaethics after Moore. New York: Oxford University Press, 339–65.Google Scholar
Skodol, A. E. and J. M. Oldham (1996) Phenomenology, differential diagnosis, and comorbidity of the obsessive-compulsive spectrum of disorders. In Oldham, J. M., Hollander, E. and Skodol, A. E., eds., Impulsivity and Compulsivity. Washington, D. C.: American Psychiatric Press, 1–36.Google Scholar
Smetana, J. and Breages, J. (1990) The development of toddlers' moral and conventional judgements. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 36: 329–46.Google Scholar
Smillie, S. (2004) Throw your arms around the world. The Guardian, Dec 2.Google Scholar
Snyder, A. W., Mulcahy, E., Taylor, J. L.Mitchell, D. J., Sachdev, P. and Gandevia, S. C. (2003) Savant-like skills exposed in normal people by suppressing the left fronto-temporal lobe. Journal of Integrative Neuroscience 2: 149–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sobell, L. C., Ellingstad, T. P. and Sobell, M. B. (2000) Natural recovery from alcohol and drug problems: methodological review of the research with suggestions for future directions, Addiction 95: 749–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spence, S. (1996) Free will in the light of neuropsychiatry. Philosophy, Psychiatry and Psychology 3: 75–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sterelny, K. and Griffiths, P. E. (1999) Sex and Death: An Introduction to Philosophy of Biology. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Sterelny, K. (2004) Externalism, epistemic artefacts and the extended mind. In Schantz, R., ed., The Externalist Challenge: New Studies in Cognition and Intentionality. Berlin: de Gruyter, 239–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sterelny, K. (2007) Cognitive Load and Human Decision, or, Three Ways of Rolling the Rock Uphill. In Lawrence, S. and Stich, S., eds., The Innate Mind: Culture and Cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Strawson, G. (2000) The unhelpfulness of indeterminism. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 60: 149–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strawson, P. F. (1962) Freedom and resentment. Proceedings of the British Academy 48: 1–25.Google Scholar
Strayhorn, J. M. (2002) Self-control: theory and research. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 41: 7–16CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tancredi, L. (2004) Neuroscience developments and the law. In Garland, Brent, ed., Neuroscience and the Law. New York: Dana Press. 71–113.Google Scholar
Taylor, C. (1989) Sources of the Self: The Making of the Modern Identity. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Taylor, C. (1991) The Ethics of Authenticity. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Taylor, C. (1995) The politics of recognition. In Philosophical Arguments. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 225–6.Google Scholar
Teasdale, T. W. and Owen, D. R. (2005) A long-term rise and recent decline in intelligence test performance: the Flynn Effect in reverse. Personality and Individual Differences 39: 837–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, R. K., Oden, D. L. and Boysen, S. T. 1997, Language-naive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) judge relations between relations in a conceptual matching-to-sample task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 23: 31–43.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A. (1998) Empathy and its origins in early development. In S. Braten, ed., Intersubjective Communication and Emotion in Early Ontogeny. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 144–57.
Thomson, J. J. (1976) Killing, letting die, and the Trolley Problem. The Monist 59: 204–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomson, J. J. (1986) The trolley problem. In Rights, Restitution, and Risk. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press, 94–116.Google Scholar
Trivers, R. (1985) Social Evolution. Menlo Park, Calif.: Benjamin/Cummings Publishing.Google Scholar
Tse, W. S. and Bond, A. J. (2002) Serotonergic intervention affects both social dominance and affiliative behaviour. Psychopharmacology, 161, 324–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tse, W. S. and Bond, A. J. (2003) Reboxetine promotes social bonding in healthy volunteers. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 17, 189–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turiel, E. (1977) Distinct conceptual and developmental domains: Social convention and morality. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 25: 77–116.Google ScholarPubMed
Turiel, E. (1983) The development of social knowledge: morality and convention. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Turiel, E., Killen, M. and Helwig, C. C. (1987) Morality: its structure, functions and vagaries. In Kagan, J. and Lamb, S., eds., The Emergence of Morality in Young Children. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 155–244.Google Scholar
Tversky, A. and Kahneman, D. (1983) Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: the conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychology Review 90: 293–315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vaiva, G., Ducrocq, F., Jezequel, K., (2003) Immediate treatment with Propranolol decreases posttraumatic stress disorder two months after trauma. Biological Psychiatry 54: 947–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valmaggia, L. R., Gaag, M., Tarrier, N., Pijnenborg, M. and Slooff, C. J. (2005) Cognitive-behavioural therapy for refractory psychotic symptoms of schizophrenia resistant to atypical antipsychotic medication: randomised controlled trial. British Journal of Psychiatry 186: 324–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Venneri, A. and Shanks, M. F. (2004) Belief and awareness: reflections on a case of persistent anosognosia. Neuropsychologia 42: 230–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vuilleumier, P., Schwartz, S., Husain, M., Clarke, K. and Driver, J. (2001) Implicit processing and learning of visual stimuli in parietal extinction and neglect. Cortex 37: 741–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vohs, K. D. and Heatherton, T. F. (2000) Self-regulatory failure: a resource-depletion approach. Psychological Science 11: 249–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vohs, K. D. and Faber, R. (2003) Self-regulation and impulsive spending patterns. In Keller, P. A. and Rook, D. W., eds., Advances in Consumer Research30 (1): 125–6.Google Scholar
Vohs, K. D., Baumeister, R. F., Twenge, J. M. Tice, D. M. and Crocker J. (unpublished) Self-regulation and choice.
Wansink, B., Kent, R. J. and Hoch, S. J. (1998) An anchoring and adjustment model of purchase quantity decisions. Journal of Marketing Research 35: 71–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, P. J. and Andrews, P. W. (2002) Toward a revised evolutionary adaptationist analysis of depression: the social navigation hypothesis. Journal of Affective Disorders 72: 1–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wegner, D. (2002) The Illusion of Conscious Will. Cambridge, Mass.: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wegner, D. (2004) Précis of The Illusion of Conscious Will. Behavioral and Bain Sciences 27: 649–59.Google ScholarPubMed
Wegner, D. M. (2005) Who is the controller of controlled processes? In Hassin, R., Uleman, J. S. and Bargh, J. A., eds., The New Unconscious. New York: Oxford University Press, 19–36.Google Scholar
Wegner, D. and Wheatley, T. (1999) Apparent mental causation: sources of the experience of will. American Psychologist 54: 480–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiskopf, D. (submitted) Pabrolling the Mind's boundaries.
Weiskrantz, L. (1986) Blindsight. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Weithman, P. (2005) Review of John Christman and Joel Anderson, eds., Autonomy and the Challenges to Liberalism. Notre Dame Philosophical Reviews. 09.12.2005 <http://ndpr.nd.edu/review.cfm?id=3921>
Wheatley, T. and Haidt, J. (2005) Hypnotic disgust makes moral judgments more severe. Psychological Science 16: 780–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whetstone, T. and Cross, M. D. (1998) Control of conscious contents in directed forgetting and thought suppression. PSYCHE 4: 16. http://psyche.cs.monash.edu.au/v4/psyche-4–16-whetstone.htmlGoogle Scholar
Wilkinson, G. S. (1990) Food sharing in vampire bats. Scientific American 262: 64–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, B. (1981) Moral Luck: Philosophical Papers 1973–1980. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 20–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, R. A. (2004) Boundaries of the Mind: The Individual in the Fragile Sciences, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, R. A. (forthcoming) Meaning making and the mind of the externalist. In Menary, R.. The Extended Mind, Aldershot: Ashgate.
Wilson, R. A. and Clark, A. (forthcoming) How to situate cognition: letting nature take its course. In Aydede, M. and Robbins, P., eds., The Cambridge Handbook of Situated Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Wolpe, P. R., Foster, K. R. and Langleben, D. D. (2005) Emerging neurotechnologies for lie-detection: promises and perils. American Journal of Bioethics 5: 39–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolpert, L. (2001) Malignant Sadness: The Anatomy of Depression. London: Faber and Faber.Google Scholar
Wong, D. (1984) Moral Relativity. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Young, L., Cushman, F. A., Adolphs, R., Tranel, D. and Hauser, M. D. (2006) Does emotion mediate the relationship between an action's moral status and its intentional status? Neuropsychological evidence. Journal of Cognition and Culture 6: 265–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zaltman, G. (2003) How Customers Think: Essential Insights in the Mind of the Market. Boston, Mass.: Harvard Business Press.Google Scholar
Zeman, A. (2003) Consciousness: A User's Guide. London: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Zhu, J. (2004) Locating volition. Consciousness and Cognition 13: 302–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ziemann, U., Muellbacher, W., Hallett, M. and Cohen, L. G. (2001) Modulation of practice-dependent plasticity in human motor cortex. Brain 124: 1171–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • Neil Levy, University of Melbourne
  • Book: Neuroethics
  • Online publication: 16 December 2010
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511811890.011
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • Neil Levy, University of Melbourne
  • Book: Neuroethics
  • Online publication: 16 December 2010
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511811890.011
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • Neil Levy, University of Melbourne
  • Book: Neuroethics
  • Online publication: 16 December 2010
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511811890.011
Available formats
×