Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-qs9v7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-11T18:44:13.637Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

13 - On the Persistence of Private Speech: Empirical and Theoretical Considerations

from Part III - Symbols and Tools throughout the Life Span

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 July 2009

Adam Winsler
Affiliation:
George Mason University, Virginia
Charles Fernyhough
Affiliation:
University of Durham
Ignacio Montero
Affiliation:
Universidad Autónoma de Madrid
Get access

Summary

Young children often talk to themselves out loud. Indeed, there is a substantial literature on children's private speech demonstrating that verbal mediation in the form of overt self-directed speech is an important feature of children's engagement with the world (e.g., see edited volumes by Díaz & Berk, 1992, and Zivin, 1979). Adults, too, talk to themselves out loud – this in spite of the stigma often associated with private speech. In this regard, the American sociologist Irving Goffman (1981) reminds us that “in our society a taboo is placed on self-talk” (p. 81), a taboo whose existence could quite reasonably be expected to inhibit private speech in those (mostly adults) for whom this taboo has some authoritative value. Goffman notes that due to the taboo nature of self-talk, “it is mainly through self-observation and hearsay that one can find out that a considerable amount goes on” (p. 81). A small but growing empirical literature on adolescents' and adults' use of private speech – on its “persistence,” as our title indicates – appears to confirm this suspicion.

Our emphasis on the persistence of private speech already suggests a direction for our argument: namely, that of expanding the conventional Vygotskian view – perpetuated, implicitly if not explicitly, in almost all the contemporary literature on the subject – that private speech is limited to a particular developmental stage occurring during childhood.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bakhtin, M. (1984). Problems of Dostoevsky's poetics (Emerson, C., Ed. & Trans.), Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. (Original work published 1972)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bakhtin, M. M. (1986). Speech genres and other late essays (Emerson, C. & Holquist, M., Eds., McGee, V. W., Trans.). Austin: University of Texas Press. (Original work published 1979)Google Scholar
Beaudichon, J. (1973). Nature and instrumental function of private speech in problem solving situations. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 19, 117–135.Google Scholar
Behrend, D. A., Rosengren, K. S., & Perlmutter, M. (1989). A new look at children's private speech: The effects of age, task difficulty, and parent presence. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 12, 305–320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berk, L. E. (1986). Relationship of elementary school children's private speech to behavioral accompaniment to task, attention, and task performance. Developmental Psychology, 22, 671–680.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berk, L. E. (1992). Children's private speech: An overview of theory and the status of research. In Díaz, R. M. & Berk, L. E. (Eds.), Private speech: From social interaction to self-regulation (pp. 17–53). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Berk, L. E., & Garvin, R. A. (1984). Development of private speech among low-income Appalachian children. Developmental Psychology, 20, 271–286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bivens, J. A., & Berk, L. E. (1990). A longitudinal study of the development of elementary school children's private speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 36, 443–463.Google Scholar
Conrad, R. (1971). The chronology of the development of covert speech in children. Developmental Psychology, 5, 398–405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daugherty, M. (1993). Creativity and private speech: Developmental trends. Creativity Research Journal, 6, 287–296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daugherty, M., White, C., & Manning, B. (1994). Relationships among private speech and creativity measurements of young children. Gifted Child Quarterly, 38, 21–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deniz, C. B. (2004). Early childhood teachers' beliefs about and self-reported practices toward children's private speech. Dissertation Abstracts International, Section A: Humanities and Social Sciences, 64(9-A), 3185.Google Scholar
Díaz, R., & Berk, L. (Eds.). (1992). Private speech: From social interaction to self-regulation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Dreyfus, H. L. (1991). Being-in-the-world: A commentary on Heidegger's Being and Time. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Duncan, R. M., & Cheyne, J. A. (1999). Incidence and functions of self-reported private speech in young adults: A self-verbalization questionnaire. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 31, 133–136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duncan, R. M., & Cheyne, J. A. (2001). Private speech in young adults: Task difficulty, self-regulation, and psychological predication. Cognitive Development, 16, 889–906.Google Scholar
Duncan, R., Cheyne, J. A., Tarcza, S., & Murray, K. (2005). Young adults' private speech: Effects of task difficulty, task type, and private writing. Unpublished manuscript, St. Francis Xavier University, Antigonish, Nova Scotia, Canada.Google Scholar
Duncan, R. M., & Pratt, M. W. (1997). Microgenetic change in the quantity and quality of preschoolers' private speech. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 20, 367–383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duncan, R., Smith, , McLaren, A., , T., & Scott, , , J. (2001). The effect of awareness of audiovisual recording on private speech: A cross-sectional experimental study. Poster presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Minneapolis, MN.Google Scholar
Fernyhough, C. (2004). Alien voices and inner dialogue: Towards a developmental account of auditory verbal hallucinations. New Ideas in Psychology, 22, 49–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L., Flavell, E. R., & Grossman, J. B. (1997). The development of children's knowledge about inner speech. Child Development, 68, 39–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallimore, R., & Tharp, R. (1990). Teaching mind in society: Teaching, schooling, and literate discourse. In Moll, L. C. (Ed.), Vygotsky and education: Instructional implications and applications of sociohistorical psychology (pp. 175–205). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goffman, I. (1981). Forms of talk. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Hardy, J., Hall, C., & Hardy, L. (2004). A note on athletes' use of self-talk. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 16, 251–257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heidegger, M. (1962). Being and time (J. Macquarrie & E. Robinson, Trans.). New York: Harper & Row. (Original work published 1953)Google Scholar
Highlen, P. S., & Bennett, B. B. (1983). Elite divers and wrestlers: A comparison between open- and closed-skill athletes. Journal of Sport Psychology, 5, 390–409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
John-Steiner, V. (1992). Private speech among adults. In Díaz, R. M. & Berk, L. E. (Eds.), Private speech: From social interaction to self-regulation (pp. 285–296). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kohlberg, L., Yaeger, J., & Hjertholm, E. (1968). Private speech: Four studies and a review of theories. Child Development, 39, 691–736.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kronk, C. M. (1994). Private speech in adolescents. Adolescence, 29, 781–804.Google ScholarPubMed
Manfra, L., & Winsler, A. (2006). Preschool children's awareness of private speech. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 30, 537–549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCafferty, S. G. (1992). The use of private speech by adult second language learners: A cross-cultural study. Modern Language Journal, 76, 179–189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCafferty, S. G. (1994). Adult second language learners' use of private speech: A review of studies. Modern Language Journal, 78, 421–436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murray, J. (1979). Spontaneous private speech and performance on a delayed match-to-sample task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 27, 286–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olszewski, P. (1987). Individual differences in preschool children's production of verbal fantasy play. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 69–86.Google Scholar
Pellegrini, A. D. (1981). The development of preschoolers' private speech. Journal of Pragmatics, 5, 445–458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1959). The language and thought of the child. London: Routledge & Paul. (Original work published 1923)Google Scholar
Powell, T. (2001). Young adults' private speech during intentional and incidental verbal memory task. Unpublished honours thesis, University of Prince Edward Island, Charlottetown, Prince Edward Island, Canada.Google Scholar
Quay, L., & Blaney, R. (1992). Verbal communication, non-verbal communication, and private speech in lower and middle socioeconomic status preschool children. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 153, 129–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, R. (1979). Private speech in academic problem-solving: A naturalistic perspective. In Zivin, G. (Ed.), The development of self-regulation through private speech (pp. 295–323). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Roberts, R., & Tharp, R. (1980). A naturalistic study of school children's private speech in an academic problem-solving task. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 4, 341–352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, J. F. (2002). Relations among self-talk, self-consciousness, and self-knowledge. Psychological Reports, 91, 807–812.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegrist, M. (1995). Inner speech as a cognitive process mediating self-consciousness and inhibiting self-deception. Psychological Reports, 76, 259–265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soskin, W. F., & John, V. (1963). The study of spontaneous talk. In Barker, G. (Ed.), The stream of behavior: Explorations of its structure and content (pp. 228–281). New York: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steeves, L. (2003). Young adults' private speech during repeated trials on the Tower of Hanoi problem. Unpublished honours thesis, St. Francis Xavier University, Antigonish, Nova Scotia, Canada.Google Scholar
Toulmin, S. (1979). The inwardness of mental life. Critical Inquiry, 6, 1–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language (E. Hanfmann & G. Vakar, Eds. & Trans.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Original work published 1934)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes (M. Cole, V. John-Steiner, S. Scribner, & E. Souberman, Eds. & Trans.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (Original work published 1930)Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). Thinking and speech. In Rieber, R. W., Carton, A. S. (Eds.), & N. Minick (Trans.), The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky: Vol. I. Problems of general psychology (pp. 37–285). New York: Plenum Press. (Original work published 1934)Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S., & Luria, A. R. (1974). The function and fate of egocentric speech. In Ninth International Congress of Psychology (pp. 464–465). Nendeln, Germany: Kraus Reprints. (Original work published 1929)Google Scholar
Watkins, M. (1986). Invisible guests: The development of imaginal dialogues. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Wertsch, J. V. (1991). Voices of the mind: A sociocultural approach to mediated action. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Winsler, A., Fernyhough, C., McClaren, E. M., & Way, E. (2005). Private speech coding manual. Unpublished manuscript, George Mason University, Fairfax, VA. Available at: http://classweb.gmu.edu/awinsler/Resources/PsCodingManual.pdf.Google Scholar
Winsler, A., & Naglieri, J. (2003). Overt and covert verbal problem-solving strategies: Developmental trends in use, awareness, and relations with task performance in children aged 5 to 17. Child Development, 74, 659–678.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zivin, G. (Ed.). (1979). The development of self-regulation through private speech. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×