Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-swr86 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-22T04:58:27.721Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

9 - Biological Intelligence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Yale University
Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Elena L. Grigorenko
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Get access

Summary

Over the course of the years, there have been many diverse definitions of intelligence. A common feature of these definitions, however, has been intelligence as the ability to adapt to the environment (see, for example, “Intelligence and its Measurement,” 1921; Sternberg and Detterman, 1986). There is perhaps no other feature of definitions of intelligence that has this level of universality.

What has been left unclear in these definitions and in the theories based on them is exactly what adaptation to the environment means. I argue in this essay that there are two distinct senses of adaptation to the environment, and that their meanings are quite different. Both constitute kinds of intelligence, one biological, the other, cultural. The focus of this article is on the biological.

Before discussing the proposed view of biological intelligence, I briefly review some previous approaches on which any new view must build, or at least, with which any new view must come to terms.

BIOLOGICAL APPROACHES TO INTELLIGENCE

Biological approaches typically seek to understand intelligence by directly studying the brain and its functioning rather than by studying primarily products or processes of behavior (Jerison, 2000; Vernon, Wickett, Bazana, and Stelmack, 2000). Early studies, like those by Karl Lashley (1950) and others seeking to localize biological bases of intelligence and other aspects of mental processes, were a resounding failure, despite great efforts. But Lashley had only relatively primitive tools for studying the brain.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Barrett, P. T., Daum, I., & Eysenck, H. J. (1990). Sensory nerve conduction and intelligence: A methodological study. Journal of Psychophysiology, 4(1), 1–13Google Scholar
Barrett, P. T., & Eysenck, H. J. (1992). Brain evoked potentials and intelligence: The Hendrickson Paradigm. Intelligence, 16, 361–381CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, P. T., & Eysenck, H. J. (1994). The relationship between evoked potential component amplitude, latency, contour length, zero-crossings, and psychometric intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 16, 3–32CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barry, W., & Ertl, J. (1966). Brain waves and human intelligence. In F. B. Davis (Ed.), Modern educational developments: Another look (pp. 191–197). New York: Educational Records Bureau
Bennett, E. L., Diamond, M. C., Krech, D., & Rosenzweig, M. R. (1964). Chemical and anatomical plasticity of the brain. Science, 146, 610–619CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F., & Kipp, K. (2001). Social cognition, inhibition, and theory of mind: The evolution of human intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg & J. C. Kaufman (Eds.), The evolution of intelligence. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Bogen, J. E. (1975). Some educational aspects of hemispheric specialization. UCLA Educator, 17, 24–32Google Scholar
Boring, E. G. (1923, June 6). Intelligence as the tests test it. New Republic, 35–37
Bouchard, T. J., Jr., & McGue, M. (1981). Familial studies of intelligence: A review. Science, 212, 1055–1059CrossRef
Bradshaw, J. L. (2001). The evolution of intellect: Cognitive, neurological and primatological aspects and hominid culture. In R. J. Sternberg & J. C. Kaufman (Eds.), The evolution of intelligence. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Broca, P. P. (1861). Nouvelle observation d'aphemie produite par une lésion de la moitié postérieure des deuxième et troisième circonvolutions frontales gauches [New observations/recordings of aphemie produced by an injury of the rear half of the second and third frontal left convolutions]. Bulletins de la Societé Anotomique de Paris, 36, 398–407Google Scholar
Brody, N. (2000). History of theories and measurements of intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence (pp. 16–33). New York: Cambridge University PressCrossRef
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Ceci, S. J. (1994). Nature-nurture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: A bioecological model. Psychological Review, 101, 568–586CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, R. L. (2001). The primate origins of human intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg & J. C. Kaufman (Eds.), The evolution of intelligence. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Calvin, W. H. (2001). Pumping up intelligence: Abrupt climate jumps and the evolution of higher intellectual functions during the ice ages. In R. J. Sternberg & J. C. Kaufman (Eds.), The evolution of intelligence. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Caryl, P. G. (1994). Early event-related potentials correlate with inspection time and intelligence. Intelligence, 18(1), 15–46CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Comery, T. A., Shah, R., & Greenough, W. T. (1995). Differential rearing alters spine density on medium-sized spiny neurons in the rat corpus striatum: Evidence for association of morphological plasticity with early response gene expression. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 63, 217–219CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corballis, M. C. (2001). Evolution of the generative mind. In R. J. Sternberg & J. C. Kaufman (Eds.), The evolution of intelligence. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2001). Unraveling the enigma of human intelligence: evolutionary psychology and the multimodular mind. In R. J. Sternberg & J. C. Kaufman (Eds.), The evolution of intelligence. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Darwin, C. (1859). The origin of species. London: Murray
Davis, F. B. (1971). The measurement of mental capability through evoked-potential recordings. Greenwich, CT: Educational Records Bureau
Deary, I. J. (2000). Simple information processing. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence (pp. 267–284). New York: Cambridge University Press
Deary, I. J., & Caryl, P. G. (1994). Intelligence, inspection time, and cognitive strategies. In P. A. Vernon (Ed.), Biological approaches to the study of human intelligence. Norwood, NJ: Ablex
Diamond, M. C., Ingham, C. A., Johnson, R. E., Bennett, E. L., & Rosenzweig, M. R. (1976). Effects of environment on morphology of rat cerebral cortex and hippocampus. Journal of Neurobiology, 7, 75–85CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dustman, R. E., & Beck, E. C. (1972). Relationship of intelligence to visually evoked potentials. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 33, 254Google Scholar
Ertl, J., & Schafer, E. (1969). Brain response correlates of psychometric intelligence. Nature, 233, 421–2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farah, M. J. (1988). The neuropsychology of mental imagery: Converging evidence from brain-damaged and normal subjects. In J. Stiles-Davis, M. Kritchevsky, & U. Bellugi (Eds.), Spatial cognition: Brain bases and development (pp. 33–56). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Farah, M. J. (1994). Beyond “pet” methodologies to converging evidence. Trends in Neurosciences, 17(12), 514–515CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farah, M. J. (1996). Is face recognition “special”? Evidence from neuropsychology. Behavioural Brain Research, 76, 181–189CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flanagan, O., Hardcastle, V., & Nahmias, E. (2001). Is human intelligence an adaptation? Cautionary observations from the philosophy of biology. In R. J. Sternberg & J. C. Kaufman (Eds.), The evolution of intelligence. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Fuchs, J. L., Montemayor, M., & Greenough, W. T. (1990). Effect of environmental complexity on size of the superior colliculus. Behavioral and Neural Biology, 54, 198–203CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galton, F. (1883). Inquiry into human faculty and its development. London: Macmillan
Gazzaniga, M. S. (1985). The social brain: Discovering the networks of the mind. New York: Basic Books
Godfrey-Smith, P. (2001). Environmental complexity and the evolution of cognition. In R. J. Sternberg & J. C. Kaufman (Eds.), The evolution of intelligence. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Gould, S. J. (1996). Full house. New York: Harmony
Haier, R. J. (1990). The end of intelligence research. Intelligence, 14, 371–374CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haier, R. J., Nuechterlein, K. H., Hazlett, E., Wu, J. C., Pack, J., Browning, H. L., & Buchsbaum, M. S. (1988). Cortical glucose metabolic rate correlates of abstract reasoning and attention studied with positron emission tomography. Intelligence, 12, 199–217CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haier, R. J., Siegel, B., Tang, C., Abel, L., & Buchsbaum, M. S. (1992). Intelligence and changes in regional cerebral glucose metabolic rate following learning. Intelligence, 16, 415–426CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological theory. New York: Wiley
Hendrickson, A. E. (1982). The biological basis of intelligence. Part I: Theory. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.), A model for intelligence (pp. 151–196). Berlin: Springer-Verlag
Hendrickson, A. E., & Hendrickson, D. E. (1980). The biological basis for individual differences in intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 1, 3–33CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hendrickson, D. E. (1982). The biological basis of intelligence. Part II: Measurement. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.), A model for intelligence (pp. 197–228). Berlin: Springer-Verlag
Hoadley, M. F. (with Pearson, K.). (1929). On measurement of the internal diameters of the skull in relation: I. To the prediction of its capacity; II. To the “preeminence” of the left hemisphere. Biometrika, 21, 85-123
“Intelligence and its measurement”: A symposium (1921). Journal of Educational Psychology, 12, 123–147, 195–216, 271–275CrossRef
Jensen, A. R. (1982). Reaction time and psychometric g. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.), A model for intelligence. Heidelberg, Germany: Springer-VerlagCrossRef
Jensen, A. R. (1997). The puzzle of nongenetic variance. In R. J. Sternberg & E. L. Grigorenko (Eds.), Intelligence, heredity, and environment (pp. 42–88). New York: Cambridge University Press
Jerison, H. J. (1982): The evolution of biological intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of human intelligence. New York: Cambridge University Press (pp.: 723–791)
Jerison, H. J. (2000). The evolution of intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence (pp. 216–244). New York: Cambridge University PressCrossRef
Jerison, H. J. (2001). On theory in comparative psychology. In R. J. Sternberg & J. C. Kaufman (Eds.), The evolution of intelligence. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Jones, T. A., & Greenough, W. T. (1996). Ultrastructural evidence for increased contact between astrocytes and synapses in rats reared in a complex environment. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 65, 48–56CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Juel-Nielsen, N. (1965). Individual and environment; a psychiatric-psychological investigation of monozygotic twins reared apart. New York: Humanities Press
Kutas, M., McCarthy, G., & Donchin, E. (1977). Augmenting mental chronometry: The P300 as a measure of stimulus evaluation time. Science, 197, 792–795CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lashley, K. S. (1950). In search of the engram. Symposia of the Society for Experimental Biology, 4, 454–482Google Scholar
Levy, J. (1974). Cerebral asymmetries as manifested in split-brain man. In M. Kinsbourne & W. L. Smith (Eds.), Hemispheric disconnection and cerebral function. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas
Loeb, V. (2000, December 25). Future threats: A panel of experts outlines global conflicts and economic trends over the next 15 years. The Washington Post Weekly Edition, 16
Loehlin, J. C. (1989). Partitioning environmental and genetic contributions to behavioral development. American Psychologist, 44, 1285–1292CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lohman, D. F. (2000). Complex information processing and intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence (pp. 285–340). New York: Cambridge University PressCrossRef
Luria, A. R. (1966). The human brain and psychological processes. New York: Harper & Row
Luria, A. R. (1973). The working brain. New York: Basic Books
Luria, A. R. (1980). Higher cortical functions in man (2d ed., rev. and expanded). New York: Basic Books
Mackintosh, N. J. (1998). IQ and human intelligence. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press
Magliero, A., Bashore, T. R., Coles, M. G., & Donchin, E. (1984). On the dependence of P300 latency on stimulus evaluation processes. Psychophysiology, 21(2), 171–186CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matarazzo, J. D. (1992). Biological and physiological correlates of intelligence. Intelligence, 16(3,4), 257–258CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCarthy, G., & Donchin, E., (1981). A metric for thought: A comparison of P300 latency and reaction time. Science, 211, 77–9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGarry-Roberts, P. A., Stelmack, R. M., & Campbell, K. B. (1992). Intelligence, reaction time, and event-related potentials. Intelligence, 16(3,4), 289–313CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newman, H. H., Freeman, F. N., & Holzinger, K. J. (1937). Twins: A study of heredity and environment. Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Perry, N. W., McCow, J. G., Cunningham, W. R., Falgout, J. C., & Street, W. J. (1976). Multivariate visual evoked response correlates of intelligence. Psychophysiology, 13, 323–329Google ScholarPubMed
Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. New York: W. W. Norton & Co
Plomin, R. (1997). Identifying genes for cognitive abilities and disabilities. In R. J. Sternberg & E. L. Grigorenko (Eds.), Intelligence, heredity, and environment (pp. 89–104). New York: Cambridge University Press
Plotkin, H. (2001). Intelligence as predisposed skeptical induction engines. In R. J. Sternberg & J. C. Kaufman (Eds.), The evolution of intelligence. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Polich, J., & Kok, A. (1995). Cognitive and biological determinants of P300: An integrative review. Biological Psychology, 41(2), 103–146CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reed, T. E. (1984). Residual latency (delay at the neuromuscular junction): Normative values and heritability in mice. Behavior Genetics, 14(3), 209–219CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reed, T. E., & Jensen, A. R. (1991). Arm nerve conduction velocity (NCV), brain NCV, reaction time, and intelligence. Intelligence, Vol 15, 33–47CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reed, T. E., & Jensen, A. R. (1992). Conduction velocity in a brain nerve pathway of normal adults correlates with intelligence level. Intelligence, 16, 259–272CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rijsdijk, F. V., & Boomsma, D. I. (1997). Genetic mediation of the correlation between peripheral nerve conduction velocity and IQ. Behavior Genetics, 27(2), 87–98CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rijsdijk, F. V., Boomsma, D. I., & Vernon, P. A. (1995). Genetic analysis of peripheral nerve conduction velocity in twins. Behavior Genetics, 25(1) 341–348CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scarr, S. (1997). Behavior-genetic and socialization theories of intelligence: Truce and reconciliation. In R. J. Sternberg & E. L. Grigorenko (Eds.), Intelligence, heredity and environment (pp. 3–41). New York: Cambridge University Press
Schafer, R. (1982). The relevance of the “here and now” transference interpretation to the reconstruction of early development. International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 63(1) 77–82Google ScholarPubMed
Segalowitz, S. J., Unsal, A., & Dywan, J. (1992). Cleverness and wisdom in 12-year-olds: Electrophysiological evidence for late maturation of the frontal lobe. Developmental Neuropsychology, 8(2–3) 179–298CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shields, J. (1962). Monozygotic twins brought up apart and brought up together. London: Oxford University Press
Shucard, D. W., & Callaway, E. (1973). Relationship between human intelligence and frequency analysis of cortical evoked response. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 36, 147–151CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shucard, D. W., & Horn, J. L. (1972). Evoked cortical potentials and measurement of human abilities. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 78(1) 59–68CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sperry, R. W. (1961). Cerebral organization and behavior. Science, 133, 1749–1757CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stenhouse, D. (1973). The evolution of intelligence: A general theory and some of its implications. New York: Harper & Row
Sternberg, R. J. (1995). Expertise in complex problem solving: A comparison of alternative conceptions. In P. A. Frensch & J. Funke (Eds.), Complex problem solving: European perspectives (pp. 295–321). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Sternberg, R. J., & Detterman, D. K. (1986). What is intelligence? Norwood, N.J.: Ablex Publishing Corporation
Sternberg, R. J., & Grigorenko, E. L. (Eds.). (1997). Intelligence, heredity, and environment. New York: Cambridge University Press
Sternberg, R. J., & Horvath, J. A. (1998). Cognitive conceptions of expertise nad their relations to giftedness. In R. Friedman & K. Rogers (Eds.), Talent in context (pp. 177–191). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association
Sternberg, R. J., & Kaufman, J. C. (Eds.). (2001). The evolution of intelligence. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Tan, Ü. (1996). Correlations between nonverbal intelligence and peripheral nerve conduction velocity in right-handed subjects: Sex-related differences. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 22, 123–128CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vernon, P. A., & Mori, M. (1989). Intelligence, reaction times, and nerve conduction velocity. Behavior Genetics (abstracts), 19, 779
Vernon, P. A., & Mori, M. (1992). Intelligence, reaction times, and peripheral nerve conduction velocity. Intelligence, 16(3–4), 273–288CrossRef
Vernon, P. A., Wickett, J. C., Bazana, P. G., & Stelmack, R. M. (2000). The neuropsychology and psycholophysiology of human intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence (pp. 245–264). New York: Cambridge University Press
Wahlsten, D., & Gottlieb, G. (1997). The invalid separation of effects of nature and nurture: Lessons from animal experimentation. In R. J. Sternberg & E. L. Grigorenko (Eds.), Intelligence, heredity, and environment (pp. 163–192). New York: Cambridge University Press
Wickett, J. C., & Vernon, P. A. (1994). Peripheral nerve conduction velocity, reaction time, and intelligence: An attempt to replicate Vernon and Mori. Intelligence, 18, 127–132CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wickett, J. C., Vernon, P. A., & Lee, D. H. (2000). The relationships between factors of intelligence and brain volume. Personality and Individual Differences, 29(6) 1095–1122CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Widaman, K. F., Carlson, J. S., Saetermoe, C. L., & Galbraith, G. C. (1993). The relationship of auditory evoked potentials to fluid and crystallized intelligence. Personality & Individual Differences, 15(2) 205–217CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zentall, T. R. (2000). Animal intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence (pp. 197–215). New York: Cambridge University PressCrossRef

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×