Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-m8s7h Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-25T03:07:10.960Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

9 - Language in autism spectrum disorders

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 February 2011

Ilona Roth
Affiliation:
The Open University, Milton Keynes
Payam Rezaie
Affiliation:
The Open University, Milton Keynes
Get access

Summary

Studies of structural language in individuals with autistic spectrum disorder (ASD) are reviewed, and theories of the causes of structural language anomalies and impairments in ASD are presented and discussed. It is concluded that the factors that may contribute to language impairment in individuals with ASD are many and various; that impaired mindreading is always implicated, but that some additional and critical causal factor remains to be conclusively identified.

Introduction

Languages, defined in formal or structural terms, are systems of mainly arbitrary items (e.g. sounds, signs or written letters; morphemes; words) with rules for combining items to convey meaning to others with shared knowledge of the language. Language can be analysed and characterised at the level of grammar (morphology and syntax) and meaning (semantics); and, in the case of spoken language, phonology.

Communication, on the other hand, involves the use of language in social interaction, whether directly in face-to-face talk, or indirectly as in, for example, a recorded phone message or a Last Will and Testament. Thus, language is a means, or method, of communicating. Non-linguistic, or non-verbal, signals including facial expression, gesture, and body language also provide means, or methods, of communicating. Prosody, which involves the use of vocal tone, pitch, rhythm and inflexion during speech also comes under the heading of non-verbal communication. Factors influencing the communicative use of language and non-verbal communication signals in specific instances can be analysed and characterised in the study of pragmatics.

Type
Chapter
Information
Researching the Autism Spectrum
Contemporary Perspectives
, pp. 284 - 315
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

,American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual: 4th Edn, Text Revised (DSM IV-TR). New York: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Allen, D. (1989). Developmental language disorders in preschool children: Clinical subtypes and syndromes. School Psychology Review, 18: 442–451.Google Scholar
Asperger, H. (1944). Die ‘autistichen psychopathen’ im kindersalter. Archive für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankenheiten, 117: 76–136. (Reprinted in Autism and Asperger Syndrome, by U. Frith, editor, 1991, Cambridge UK: Cambridge University Press).Google Scholar
Baltaxe, C. (1977). Pragmatic deficits in the language of autistic adolescents. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 2: 176–180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind. London: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Baldwin, D., Crowson, M. (1997). Do children with autism use the speaker's direction of gaze (SDG) strategy to crack the code of language? Child Development, 68: 48–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartak, L., Rutter, M., Cox, A. (1975). Comparative study of infantile autism and specific developmental receptive language disorder 1: Children. British Journal of Psychiatry, 126: 127–145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartak, L., Rutter, M., Cox, A. (1977). Comparative study of infantile-autism and specific developmental receptive language disorders 3: Discriminant function analysis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 7: 383–396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartolucci, G., Pierce, S., Streiner, D., Eppel, P. (1976). Phonological investigation of verbal autistic and mentally retarded subjects. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 6: 303–316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartolucci, G., Pierce, S., Streiner, D. (1980). Cross-sectional studies of grammatical morphemes in autistic and mentally retarded children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 10: 39–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bennett, T., Szatmari, P., Bryson, S., et al. (2008). Differentiating autism and Asperger syndrome on the basis of language delay or impairment. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38: 616–625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, D.V.M. (1989). Autism, Asperger's syndrome and semantic-pragmatic disorder: Where are the boundaries? British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 24: 107–121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, D.V.M., Norbury, C.F. (2002). Exploring the borderlands of autistic disorder and specific language impairment: A study using standardised diagnostic instruments. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43: 917–929.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloom, P. (2000). How Children Learn the Meanings of Words. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bopp, K., Mirenda, P., Zumbo, B. (2009). Behaviour predictors of language development over 2 years in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52: 1106–1120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bott, L., Brock, J., Brockdorff, N., Boucher, J., Lamberts, K. (2006). Perceptual similarity in autism. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59: 1237–1254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Botting, N., Conti-Ramsden, G. (1999) Pragmatic language impairment without autism: The children in question. Autism, 3: 371–396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Botting, N., Conti-Ramsden, G. (2003). Autism, primary pragmatic difficulties, and specific language impairment: can we distinguish them using psycholinguistic markers? Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 45: 515–524.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boucher, J. (1976a). Articulation in early childhood autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 6: 297–302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boucher, J. (1976b). Is autism primarily a language disorder? British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 11: 135–143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boucher, J. (1998). SPD as a distinct diagnostic entity: Logical considerations and directions for future research. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 33: 71–108.Google ScholarPubMed
Boucher, J. (2000). Time parsing, normal language acquisition, and language-related developmental disorders. In New Directions in Language Development and Disorders. London: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers, pp. 13–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boucher, J., Mayes, A.Memory. In The Neuropsychology of Autism. Oxford: Oxford University Press, in press.
Boucher, J., Cowell, P., Howard, M., et al. (2005). A combined clinical neuropsychological and neuroanatomical study of adults with high-functioning autism. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 10: 165–214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boucher, J., Mayes, A., Bigham, S. (2008a). Memory, language and intellectual ability in low functioning autism. In Memory in Autism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 268–289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boucher, J., Bigham, S., Mayes, A., Muskett, T. (2008b). Recognition and language in low functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38: 1259–1269.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowler, D.M.Matthews, N., Gardiner, J. (1997). Asperger's syndrome and memory: Similarity to autism but not amnesia. Neuropsychologia, 35: 65–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brook, S.L., Bowler, D.M. (1992). Autism by another name? Semantic and pragmatic impairments in children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 22: 61–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cantwell, D., Baker, L., Rutter, M. (1978). Comparative study of infantile autism and specific developmental receptive language disorder 4: Analysis of syntax and language function. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 19: 351–362.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cardy, J., Flagg, E., Roberts, W., Roberts, T. (2008). Auditory evoked fields predict language ability and impairment in children with autism. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 68: 170–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carter, A.S., Volkmar, F., Sparrow, S., et al. (1998). The Vineland Adaptive Behaviour Scales: Supplementary norms for individuals with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28: 287–302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charman, T., Drew, A., Baird, C., Baird, G. (2003). Measuring early language development in preschool children with autism spectrum disorder using the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory (Infant Form). Journal of Child Language, 30: 213–236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Churchill, D. (1972). The relation of infantile autism and early childhood schizophrenia to developmental languge disorders of childhood. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 2: 182–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Condouris, K., Meyer, E., Tager-Flusberg, H. (2003). The relationship between standardized measures of language and measures of spontaneous speech in children with autism. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 12: 349–358.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dobbinson, S. (2000). Repetitiveness and productivity in the language of adults with autism. Unpublished PhD thesis, University of Sheffield.Google Scholar
Dunn, M., Bates, J. (2005). Developmental change in neutral processing of words by children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35: 361–376.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, M., Gomes, H., Sebastian, M. (1996). Prototypicality of responses of autistic, language disordered, and normal children in a word fluency task. Child Neuropsychology, 2: 99–108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eigsti, I.M., Bennetto, L., Dadlani, M.B. (2007). Beyond pragmatics: morphosyntactic development in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37: 1007–1023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fay, W., Schuler, A. (1980). Emerging Language in Autistic Children. Baltimore: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
Frith, U. (1991). Autism and Asperger Syndrome. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frith, U. (2004). Confusions and controversies about Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45: 672–687.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gaffrey, M., Kleinhans, N., Haist, F., et al. (2007). Atypical participation of visual cortex during word processing in autism: An fMRI study of semantic decision. Neuropsychologia, 45: 1672–1684.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gathercole, S.E. (2006). Nonword repetition and word learning: The nature of the relationship. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27: 513–543.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S., Baddeley, A. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29: 336–360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ghaziuddin, M., Thomas, P., Napier, E., et al. (2000). Brief Report: Brief syntactic analysis in Asperger syndrome: A preliminary study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30: 67–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Groen, W., Zwiers, M., Gaag, R-J., Buitelaar, J. (2008). The phenotype and neural correlates of language in autism: An integrative review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews, 32: 1416–1425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hala, S., Pexman, P., Glenwright, M. (2007). Priming the meaning of homographs in typically developing children and children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37: 329–340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Happé, F. (1995). Understanding minds and metaphors: Insights from the study of figurative language in autism. Metaphor and Symbol, 10: 275–295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, G.J., Chabris, C.F., Clark, J., et al. (2006). Brain activation during semantic processing in autism spectrum disorders via functional magnetic resonance imaging. Brain and Cognition, 61: 54–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herbert, M.R., Kenet, T. (2007). Brain abnormalities in language disorders and autism. Pediatric Clinics of North America, 54: 563–583.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herbert, M.R., Harris, G.J., Adrien, K.T., et al. (2002). Abnormal asymmetry in language association cortex in autism. Annals of Neurology, 52: 588–596.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herbert, M.R., Ziegler, D.A., Deutsch, C.K., et al. (2005). Brain asymmetries in autism and developmental language disorder: A nested whole-brain analysis. Brain, 128: 213–226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hermelin, B., O'Connor, N. (1967). Remembering of words by psychotic and subnormal children. British Journal of Psychology, 58: 213–218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hermelin, B., O'Connor, N. (1970). Psychological Experiments with Autistic Children. Oxford: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Hobson, R.P. (1993). Autism and the Development of Mind. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Holdstock, J., Mayes, A., Isaac, C., Gong, Q, Roberts, N. (2002). Differential involvement of the hippocampus and temporal lobe cortices in rapid and slow learning of new semantic information. Neuropsychologia, 40: 748–768.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howlin, P. (1984). The acquisition of grammatical morphemes in autistic children: A critique and replication of the finding of Bartolucci, Pierce, and Streiner, 1980. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 14: 127–136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howlin, P. (2003). Outcome in high-functioning adults with autism with and without early language delays: Implications for the differentiation between autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33: 3–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howlin, P., Mawhood, L., Rutter, M. (2000). Autism and developmental receptive language disorder – a follow-up comparison in early adult life. II: Social, behavioural, and psychiatric outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41: 561–578.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kamio, Y., Robins, D., Kelley, E., Swainson, B., Fein, D. (2007). Atypical lexical/semantic processing in high-functioning autism spectrum disorders without early language delay. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37: 1116–1122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2: 217–250.Google Scholar
Kanner, L. (1946). Irrelevant and metaphorical language in early infantile autism. Journal of Pediatrics, 25: 211–217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelley, E., Paul, J., Fein, D., Naigles, L. (2006). Residual language deficits in optimal outcome children with a history of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36: 807–828.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kjelgaard, M.M., Tager-Flusberg, H. (2001). An investigation of language impairment in autism: implications for genetic subgroups. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16: 287–308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kleinhans, N., Axel-Muller, R., Cohen, D., Courchesne, E. (2008). Atypical functional lateralisation of language in autistic spectrum disorders. Brain Research, 1221: 115–125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klin, A. (1991). Young autistic children's listening preferences in regard to speech: A possible characterization of the symptom of social withdrawal. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 21: 29–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koning, C., Magill-Evans, J. (2001). Social and language skills in adolescent boys with Asperger syndrome. Autism, 5: 23–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kraijer, D. (2000). Review of adaptive behaviour studies in mentally retarded persons with autism/pervasive developmental disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30: 39–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leonard, L., Deevy, P. (2006). Cognitive and linguistic issues in the study of children with specific language impairment. In Handbook of Psycholinguistics, 2nd edn. New York: Elsevier/Academic Press.Google Scholar
Lepisto, T., Kujala, T., Vanhala, R., Alku, P., Huotilainen, M., Näätänen, R. (2005). The discrimination of and orienting to speech and non-speech sounds in children with autism. Brain Research, 1066: 147–157.Google ScholarPubMed
Lord, C., Paul, R. (1997). Language and communication in autism. In Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders, 1st edn. New York: Wiley and Sons, pp. 195–225.Google Scholar
Luyster, R., Lopez, K., Lord, C. (2007). Characterising communicative development in children referred for autism spectrum disorders using the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory (CDI). Journal of Child Language, 34: 623–654.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macintosh, K., Dissanayake, C. (2004). The similarities and differences between autistic disorder and Asperger's disorder: A review of the empirical evidence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45: 421–434.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, I., McDonald, S. (2004). An exploration of causes of non-literal language problems in individuals with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34: 311–328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mawhood, L., Howlin, P., Rutter, M. (2000). Autism and developmental receptive language disorder – a comparative follow-up in early adult life I: Cognitive and language outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41: 547–559.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayes, S.D., Calhoun, S. (2001). Non-significance of early speech delay in children with autism and normal language and implications for DSM-IV Asperger disorder. Autism, 5: 81–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCann, J., Peppé, S. (2003). Prosody in autism spectrum disorders: A critical review. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 38: 325–350.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Menyuk, P. (1978). The language of autistic children: What's wrong and why. In Autism: A Reappraisal of Concepts and Treatment. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 105–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menyuk, P., Quill, K. (1985). Semantic problems in autistic children. In Communication Problems in Autism. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 127–145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Minshew, N., Goldstein, G., Muenz, L., Payton, J. (1992). Neuropsychological functioning in non-mentally retarded autistic individuals. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 14: 749–761.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Minshew, N., Goldstein, G., Siegel, D. (1997). Neuropsychologic functioning in autism: Profile of a complex information processing disorder. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3: 303–317.Google ScholarPubMed
Mostert, M. (2004). Facilitated communication since 1995: A review of published studies. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31: 287–313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parish-Morris, J., Hennon, E.A., Hirsch-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R.M., Tager-Flusberg, H. (2007). Children with autism illuminate the role of social intention in word learning. Child Development, 78: 1265–1287.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perkins, M., Dobbinson, S., Boucher, J., Bol, S., Bloom, P. (2006). Lexical knowledge and lexical use in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36: 795–805.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Preissler, M. (2008). Associative learning of words and pictures in low-functioning children with autism. Autism, 12: 231–248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prior, M. (2003). Learning and Behaviour Problems in Asperger Syndrome. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Prizant, B. (1983). Gestalt language and gestalt processing in autism. Topics in Language Disorders, 3: 16–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prizant, B., Duchan, J. (1981). The functions of immediate echolalia in autism children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorder, 46: 241–249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rajendran, G., Mitchell, P., Rickards, H. (2005). How do individuals with Asperger syndrome respond to nonliteral language and inappropriate requests in computer-mediated communication? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35: 429–443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rapin, I. (1991). Autistic children: Diagnosis and clinical features. Pediatrics, 87: 751–760.Google ScholarPubMed
Rapin, I. (1996). Preschool Children With Inadequate Communication. London: Mac Keith Press.Google Scholar
Rapin, I., Allen, D. (1983). Developmental language disorders: Nosological considerations. In Neuropsychology of Language, Reading and Spelling. New York: Academic Press, pp. 155–184.Google Scholar
Rapin, I., Dunn, M. (2003). Update on the language disorders of individuals on the autistic spectrum. Brain and Development, 25: 166–172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rapin, I., Dunn, M., Allen, D., Stevens, M., Fein, D. (2009). Subtypes of language disorders in school-age children with autism. Developmental Neuropsychology, 34: 66–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ricks, D.M., Wing, L. (1975). Languge, communication, and the use of symbols in normal and autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 5: 191–221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ring, H., Sharma, S., Wheelwright, S., Barrett, G. (2007). An electrophysiological investigation of semantic incongruity processing by people with Asperger's syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37: 281–290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, J.A., Rice, M.L., Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004). Tense marking in children with autism. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29: 429–448.Google Scholar
Rutter, M. (1968). Concepts of autism: A review of research. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 9: 1–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M. (1983). Cognitive deficits in the pathogenesis of autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 24: 513–531.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M., Bartak, L., Newman, S. (1971). Autism – a central disorder of cognition and language? In Infantile Autism: Concepts, Characteristics, and Treatment. London: Churchill Livingstone.Google Scholar
Saalasti, S., Lepisto, T., Toppila, E., et al. (2008). Language abilities in children with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38: 1574–1580.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scarborough, H. (1990). Index of productive syntax. Applied Psycholinguistics, 11: 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, G., Rey, M., Oram Cardy, J., Roberts, T. (2009). Absence of M100 source asymmetry in autism associated with language functioning. Neuroreport, 20: 1037–1041.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Semel, E., Wiig, E., Secord, W. (1987). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-R. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Seung, H.K. (2007). Linguistic characteristics of individuals with high functioning autism and Asperger syndrome. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 21: 247–259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shriberg, L., Paul, R., McSweeny, J., Klin, A., Cohen, D. (2001). Speech and prosody characteristics of adolescents and adults with high-functioning autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44: 1097–1115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siller, M., Sigman, M. (2008). Modeling longitudinal change in the language abilities of children with autism: Parent behaviors and child characteristics as predictors of change. Developmental Psychology, 44: 1691–1704.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swisher, L., Demetras, M. (1985). Expressive language characteristics of autistic children. In Communication Problems in Autism. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 147–162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H. (1981). On the nature of linguistic functioning in early infantile autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 11: 45–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tager-Flusberg, H. (1985). The conceptual basis for referential word use in children with autism. Child Development, 56: 1167–1178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tager-Flusberg, H. (1989). A psycholinguistic perspective on language development in the autistic child. In Autism: New Directions in Diagnosis, Nature and Treatment. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 92–115.Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H. (1991). Semantic processing in the free recall of autistic children: Further evidence of a cognitive deficit. In Autism: Nature, Diagnosis, and Treatment. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 92–109.Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004). Do autism and specific language impairment represent overlapping language disorders. In Developmental Language Disorders. Hove, UK: Psychology Press, pp. 31–52.Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H. (2006). Defining phenotypes in autism. Clinical Neuroscience Research, 6: 219–224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H., Calkins, S., Nolin, T., et al. (1990). A longitudinal study of language acquisition in autistic and Down syndrome children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20: 1–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tager-Flusberg, H., Lord, C., Paul, R. (2005). Language and communication in autism. In Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders, 3rd edn. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons, pp. 335–364.Google Scholar
Tanguay, P. (1984). Towards a new classification of serious psychopathology in children. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 23: 378–384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tantam, D. (1991). Asperger syndrome in adulthood. In Autism and Asperger Syndrome. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 147–183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tecchio, F. (2003). Auditory sensory processing in autism: A magnetoencephalographic study. Biological Psychiatry, 54: 647–654.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toichi, M. (2008). Episodic memory, semantic memory and self-awareness in high-functioning autism. In Memory in Autism: Theory and Evidence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 143–165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toichi, M., Kamio, Y. (2001). Verbal association for simple common words in high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31: 483–490.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toichi, M., Kamio, Y. (2002). Long-term memory and levels-of-processing in autism. Neuropsychologia, 40: 964–969.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tuchman, R., Rapin, I., Shinnar, S. (1991). Autistic and dysphasic children. Pediatrics, 88: 1211–1218.Google ScholarPubMed
Ullman, M. (2001). The declarative/procedural model of lexicon and grammar. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30: 37–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ullman, M., Pierpoint, E. (2005). Specific language impairment is not specific to language: The procedural deficit hypothesis. Cortex, 41: 399–433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ungerer, J., Sigman, M. (1987). Categorisation skills and receptive language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17: 3–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Volkmar, F., Klin, A. (2000). Diagnostic issues in Asperger syndrome. In Asperger Syndrome. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 25–71.Google Scholar
Walenski, M., Tager-Flusberg, H., Ullman, M. (2005). In Understanding Autism: From Basic Neuroscience to Treatment. New York: Taylor and Francis, pp. 175–203.Google Scholar
Walenski, M., Mostofsky, S., Gidley-Larson, J., Ullman, M. (2008). Brief Report: Enhanced picture naming in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38: 1395–1399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitehouse, A.J., Barry, J.G., Bishop, D.V. (2007a). The broader language phenotype of autism: a comparison with specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48: 822–830.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitehouse, A.J., Maybery, M.T, Durkin, K. (2007b). Evidence against poor semantic encoding in individuals with autism. Autism, 11: 241–254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitehouse, A.J., Barry, J.G., Bishop, D.V. (2008). Further defining the language impairment of autism: is there a specific language impairment subtype? Journal of Communication Disorders, 41: 319–336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, D.L., Goldstein, G., Minshew, N.J. (2006). Neuropsychologic functioning in children with autism: Further evidence for disordered complex information-processing. Child Neuropsychology, 12: 279–298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, D., Botting, N., Boucher, J. (2008). Language in autism and specific language impairment: Where are the links? Psychological Bulletin, 134: 944–963.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wing, L. (1996). The Autistic Continuum. London: Constable.Google Scholar
,World Health Organisation. (1992). International Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines, 10th edn. (ICD-10). Geneva: World Health Organisation.Google Scholar
Worth, S., Reynolds, S. (2008).The assessment and identification of language impairment in Asperger's syndrome: A case study. Child Language, Teaching and Therapy, 24: 55–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×