Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-8bljj Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-03T00:21:55.319Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

17 - Attentional and Regulatory Mechanisms of Momentary Work Motivation and Performance

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 August 2010

Howard M. Weiss
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Sciences, Purdue University
Neal M. Ashkanasy
Affiliation:
Business School, University of Queensland
Daniel J. Beal
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Sciences, Purdue University
Joseph P. Forgas
Affiliation:
University of New South Wales, Sydney
Kipling D. Williams
Affiliation:
University of New South Wales, Sydney
Simon M. Laham
Affiliation:
University of New South Wales, Sydney
Get access

Summary

INTRODUCTION

The study of affect in work settings, and particularly the study of the performance implications of affective states, has a long but especially disappointing history. We say disappointing for a number of reasons. First, even the smallest amount of reflection will convince anyone that organizations are settings of emotional intensity. If emotions are generated by appraisals of the reaching or impeding of important personal goals or values, then where is this more likely to play out than in work settings? Each day at work, our needs, desires, and identities are challenged and affirmed. So, one would think that the study of emotions at work would be a core topic of organizational research. It is not. Second, performance has been the criterion of interest for organizational researchers. So, one would think that the relationship between emotional states and work performance would receive great attention. It has not.

Third, research on the performance implications of true affective states took place at the beginning of work psychology. Hersey (1932) examined daily moods states (he didn't call them that, but he used what today would be considered a mood checklist) among blue-collar workers. He tracked those mood states with daily performance measures. He showed that on negative mood days performance deteriorated quite a bit, but on positive mood days there was little enhancement of performance.

Type
Chapter
Information
Social Motivation
Conscious and Unconscious Processes
, pp. 314 - 331
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2004

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ashkanasy, N. M., Härtel, C. E. J., & Daus, C. S. (2002). Advances in organizational behavior: Diversity and emotions. Journal of Management, 28, 307–338CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Averill, J. R. (1983). Studies on anger and aggression: Implications for theories of emotion. American Psychologist, 38, 1145–1160CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron, R. A. (1990). Environmentally induced positive affect: Its impact on self-efficacy, task performance, negotiation, and conflict. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 20, 368–384CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berkowitz, L. (1989). Frustration-aggression hypothesis: Examination and reformulation. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 59–73CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bless, H., Mackie, D. M., & Schwarz, N. (1992). Mood effects on attitude judgments: Independent effects of mood before and after message elaboration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 585–595CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129–148CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bower, G. H. (1991). Mood congruity of social judgments. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Affect and social judgments (pp. 31–53). Oxford: Pergamon
Brief, A. P., & Weiss, H. M. (2002). Organizational behavior: Affect at work. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 279–307CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). The self-regulation of behavior. In R. Wyer & T. Srull (Eds.), Advances in social cognition (Vol. 12, pp. 1–105). Mahwah, NJ: ErlbaumCrossRef
Deadrick, D. L., Bennett, N., & Russell, C. J. (1997). Using hierarchical linear modeling to examine dynamic performance criteria over time. Journal of Management, 23, 745–757CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeSteno, D., Petty, R. E., Wegener, D. T., & Rucker, D. D. (2000). Beyond valence in the perception of likelihood: The role of emotion specificity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 397–416CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P., & Davidson, R. J. (1994). The nature of emotion. New York: Oxford University Press
Estrada, C. A., Isen, A. M., & Young, M. J. (1997). Positive affect facilitates integration of information and decreases anchoring in reasoning among physicians. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 72, 117–135CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiedler, K. (2001). Affective influences on social information processing. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Handbook of affect and social cognition (pp. 163–185). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Fisher, C. D. (2000). Mood and emotions while working: Missing pieces of job satisfaction?Journal of Organizational Behavior, 21, 185–2023.0.CO;2-M>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fisher, C. D., & Noble, C. S. (in press). A within-person examination of correlates of performance and emotions while working. Human Performance, 17Google Scholar
Fleeson, W. (2001). Toward a structure- and process-integrated view of personality: Traits as density distributions of states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 1011–1027CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forgas, J. P. (1998). On feeling good and getting your way: Mood effects on negotiator cognition and bargaining strategies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 565–577CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frijda, N. H. (1993). Moods, emotion episodes, and emotions. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 381–404). New York: Guilford Press
George, J. M. (1990). Personality, affect, and behavior in groups. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75, 107–116CrossRefGoogle Scholar
George, J. M. (1991). State or trait: Effects of positive mood on prosocial behaviors at work. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 299–307CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hersey, R. B. (1932). Workers' emotions in shop and home: A study of individual workers from the psychological and physiological standpoint. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press
Iaffaldano, M. T., & Muchinsky, P. M. (1985). Job satisfaction and job performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 251–273CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isen, A. M. (1999). Positive affect. In T. Dalgleish & M. Power (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 521–540). Chichester, England: WileyCrossRef
Isen, A. M. (2000). Positive affect and decision making. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 417–435). New York: Guilford Press
Isen, A., & Baron, R. (1991). Positive affect as a factor in organizational behavior. In L. L. Cummings & B. M. Staw (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 13, pp. 1–54). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press
Judge, T. A., Thoresen, C. J., Bono, J. E., & Patton, G. K. (2001). The job satisfaction–job performance relationship: A qualitative and quantitative review. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 376–407CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall
Kane, J. S., & Lawler, E. E. (1979). Performance appraisal effectiveness: Its assessment and determinants. In B. Staw (Ed.), Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 1, pp. 425–478). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press
Keltner, D., Ellsworth, P. C., & Edwards, K. (1993). Beyond simple pessimism: Effects of sadness and anger on social perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 740–752CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larsen, R. J. (1987). The stability of mood variability: A spectral analytic approach to daily mood assessments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1195–1204CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Locke, E. A. (1976). The nature and causes of job satisfaction. In M. Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 1297–1350). Chicago: Rand McNally
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and task performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall
Martin, L. L., & Stoner, P. (1996). Mood as input: What we think about how we feel determines how we think. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), Striving and feeling: Interactions among goals, affect, and self-regulation (pp. 279–301). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Martin, L. L., & Tesser, A. (1996). Some ruminative thoughts. In R. S. Wyer, Jr. (Ed.), Advances in social cognition (Vol. 9, pp. 1–47). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Miner, A. G. (2001). Experience sampling events, moods, behaviors, and performance at work. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign
Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle?Psychological Bulletin, 126, 247–259CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muraven, M., Tice, D. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Self-control as a limited resource: Regulatory depletion patterns. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 774–789CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1–32CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Park, J., & Banaji, M. R. (2000). Mood and heuristics: The influence of happy and sad states on sensitivity and bias in stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 1005–1023CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pintrich, P. R., & Groot, E. V. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated learning components of classroom academic performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 33–40CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Posner, M. I., & Snyder, C. R. R. (1975). Attention and cognitive control. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Information processing and cognition (pp. 55–85). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Robinson, M. D., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Belief and feeling: Evidence for an accessibility model of emotional self-report. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 934–960CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, W., & Fisk, A. D. (1982). Dual task automatic and control processing: Can it be done without cost?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 8, 261–278Google Scholar
Schwarz, N. (2000). Emotion, cognition, and decision making. Cognition and Emotion, 14, 433–440CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vohs, K. D. (2003, February). Nonconscious self-regulation and the limited resource model. Poster presented at the fourth annual meeting of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Los Angeles
Weiss, H. M. (2002). Conceptual and empirical foundations for the study of affect at work. In R. G. Lord, R. J. Klimoski, & R. Kanfer (Eds.), Emotions in the workplace: Understanding the structure and role of emotions in organizational behavior (pp. 20–63). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass
Weiss, H. M., Beal, D. J., & Groves, M. S. (2003, April). Injustice affects performance through anger and cognitive interference. Paper presented at the 18th annual conference of the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, Orlando, FL
Weiss, H. M., & Brief, A. P. (2001). Affect at work: An historical perspective. In R. L. Payne & C. L. Cooper (Eds.), Emotions at work: Theory, research, and application in management (pp. 133–173). Chichester, England: Wiley
Weiss, H. M., & Cropanzano, R. (1996). Affective events theory: A theoretical discussion of the structure, causes and consequences of affective experiences at work. In B. M. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 18, pp. 1–74). Greenwich, CT: Jai Press
Weiss, H. M., Nicholas, J. P., & Daus, C. (1999). An examination of the joint effects of affective experiences and job beliefs on job satisfaction and variations in affective experiences over time. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 78, 1–24CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×