Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-wxhwt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-14T02:27:56.556Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

9 - Social Signals and Persuasion

from Part I - Conceptual Models of Social Signals

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 July 2017

William D. Crano
Affiliation:
Claremont Graduate School
Jason T. Siegel
Affiliation:
Claremont Graduate School
Judee K. Burgoon
Affiliation:
University of Arizona
Nadia Magnenat-Thalmann
Affiliation:
Université de Genève
Maja Pantic
Affiliation:
Imperial College London
Alessandro Vinciarelli
Affiliation:
University of Glasgow
Get access

Summary

The pace of research devoted to the study of social and emotional intelligence has escalated exponentially (Bar-On & Parker, 2000; Goleman, 2006; Mayer & Salovey, 1997; Salovey & Mayer, 1990), and the upsurge in interest has intensified the need to understand social signals, whose production and deciphering may be strongly affected by these various intelligences (Gardner, 1983). Social signals have been conceptualized in a variety of ways, but as social psychologists, we define a social signal as any variable associated with a communicative act, excluding its written or spoken content, that carries meaning. The signal may be intentionally or unintentionally encoded by a source, and mindfully or mindlessly decoded by a receiver. This definition acknowledges and allows for the smile or smirk that accompanies the expression, “You look good today,” or the falling intonation of the word “today,” to carry more meaning than the actual content of the declaration. By this definition, research on communication and persuasion from its inception has focused on understanding social signal effects. Research designed to investigate the credibility of a source of a persuasive message, for example, often relied upon extra-communication features (age, sex, dress, apparent success, etc.) to signal the extent to which a receiver should ponder or trust the information provided by the source. The speed of speech, attractiveness of the source, and the animation of the speaker all have strong effects on persuasion. This is true even when the content of the verbal or written communication remains constant across experimental conditions.

In experimental research, the social signal is considered from the vantage point of the encoder, or in other terms, as an independent variable. As such, failures to obtain differences attributable to signal variations often were counted as manipulation breakdowns. Less frequently, researchers have focused on the receivers of social signals, and sometimes the interactive behaviors of both encoders and decoders are examined in tandem. In one such study, college-age men were led to believe that a young woman with whom they were to converse via telephone was either beautiful or plain (Snyder, Tanke, & Berscheid, 1977).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2017

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Addington, D. W. (1971). The effect of vocal variations on ratings of source credibility. Speech Monographs, 38, 492–503.Google Scholar
Amos, C., Holmes, G., & Strutton, D. (2008). Exploring the relationship between celebrity endorser effects and advertising effectiveness: A quantitative synthesis of effect size International Journal of Advertising: The Quarterly Review of Marketing Communications, 27, 209–234.Google Scholar
Apple, W., Streeter, L. A., & Krauss, R. M. (1979). Effects of pitch and speech rate on personal attributions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 715–727.Google Scholar
Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 230–244.Google Scholar
Bar-On, R. & Parker, J. D. A. (2000). The Handbook of Emotional Intelligence: Theory, Development, Assessment, and Application at Home, School, and in the Workplace (1st edn). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Benkí, J. R., Broome, J., Conrad, F., Groves, R., & Kreuter, F. (2011). Effects of speech rate, pitch, and pausing on survey participation decisions. Paper presented at the May 2011 AAPOR meeting, Phoenix.
Boersma, P. & Weenink, D. (2000). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer. www.praat.org.
Burgoon, J. K., Birk, T., & Pfau, M. (1990). Nonverbal behaviors, persuasion, and credibility. Human Communication Research, 17, 140–169.Google Scholar
Carney, D. R., Cuddy, A. J. C., & Yap, A. J. (2010). Power posing: Brief nonverbal displays affect neuroendocrine levels and risk tolerance. Psychological Science, 21, 1363–1368.Google Scholar
Chaiken, S. (1979). Communicator physical attractiveness and persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1387–1397.Google Scholar
Chattopadhyay, A., Dahl, D. W., Ritchie, R. B., & Shahin, K. N. (2003). Hearing voices: The impact of announcer speech characteristics on consumer response to broadcast advertising. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 13, 198–204.Google Scholar
Crano, W. D. (1970). Effects of sex, response order, and expertise in conformity: A dispositional approach. Sociometry, 33, 239–252.Google Scholar
Crano, W. D. (1995). Attitude strength and vested interest. In R. E., Petty & J. A., Krosnick (Eds), Attitude Strength: Antecedents and Consequences. (pp. 131–157). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Crano, W. D., Brewer, M. B., & Lac, A. (2014). Principles and Methods of Social Research (3rd edn). New York: Psychology Press.
Festinger, L. (1957). A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Fischer, J., Fischer, P., Englich, B., Aydin, N., & Frey, D. (2011). Empower my decisions: The effects of power gestures on confirmatory information processing. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 1146–1154.Google Scholar
Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of Mind. New York: Basic Books.
Gerard, H. B. & Mathewson, G. C. (1966). The effect of severity of initiation on liking for a group: A replication. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2, 278–287.Google Scholar
Goleman, D. (2006). Emotional Intelligence. New York: Bantam Books.
Hebl, M. R. & Mannix, L. M. (2003). The weight of obesity in evaluating others: A mere proximity effect. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 28–38.Google Scholar
Herbst, K. C., Finkel, E. J., Allan, D., & Fitzsimons, G. M. (2011). On the dangers of pulling a fast one: Advertisement disclaimer speed, brand trust, and purchase intentions. Journal of Consumer Research, 38, 909–919.Google Scholar
Hovland, C. I., Janis, I. L., & Kelley, H. H. (1953). Communications and Persuasion: Psychological Studies in Opinion Change. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Hovland, C. I. & Weiss, W. (1951). The influence of source credibility on communication effectiveness. Public Opinion Quarterly, 15, 635–650.Google Scholar
Jackob, N., Roessing, T., & Petersen, T. (2011). The effects of verbal and nonverbal elements in persuasive communication: Findings from two multi-method experiments. Communications: The European Journal of Communication Research, 36, 245–271.Google Scholar
Johnson, I., Siegel, J. T., & Crano, W. D. (2014). Extending the reach of vested interest in predicting attitude-consistent behavior. Social Influence, 9, 20–36.Google Scholar
Kelman, H. C. (1958). Compliance, identification, and internalization: Three processes of opinion change. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 2, 51–60.Google Scholar
Kelman, H. C. & Hovland, C. I. (1953). “Reinstatement” of the communicator in delayed measurement of opinion change. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 48(3), 327–335.Google Scholar
Kumkale, G. T., Albarracín, D., & Seignourel, P. J. (2010). The effects of source credibility in the presence or absence of prior attitudes: Implications for the design of persuasive communication campaigns. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 40, 1325–1356.Google Scholar
LaBarbera, P. & MacLachlan, J. (1979). Time compressed speech in radio advertising. Journal of Marketing, 43, 30–36.Google Scholar
Lefkowitz, M., Blake, R. R., & Mouton, J. S. (1955). Status factors in pedestrian violation of traffic signals. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51, 704–706.Google Scholar
Lehman, B. J. & Crano, W. D. (2002). The pervasive effects of vested interest on attitude-criterion consistency in political judgment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 101–112.Google Scholar
MacLachlan, J. (1982). Listener perception of time compressed spokespersons. Journal of Advertising Research, 2, 47–51.Google Scholar
Maricchiolo, F., Gnisci, A., Bonaiuto, M., & Ficca, G. (2009). Effects of different types of hand gestures in persuasive speech on receivers' evaluations. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24(2), 239–266.Google Scholar
Mayer, J. D. & Salovey, P. (1997). What is emotional intelligence? In P., Salovey & D., Sluyter (Eds), Emotional Development and Emotional Intelligence: Implications for Educators (pp. 3– 31). New York: Basic Books.
McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and Mind: What Gestures Reveal about Thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
McNeill, D. (2000). Language and Gesture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, Self, and Society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Miller, N., Maruyama, G., Beaber, R. J., & Valone, K. (1976). Speed of Speech and Persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 615–624.Google Scholar
Mills, J. & Jellison, J. M. (1967). Effect on opinion change of how desirable the communication is to the audience the communicator addressed. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 6, 98–101.Google Scholar
Norton, M. I., Mochon, D., & Ariely, D. (2012). The IKEA effect: When labor leads to love. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22(3), 453–460.Google Scholar
Oksenberg, L., Coleman, L., & Cannell, C. F. (1986). Interviewers' voices and refusal rates in telephone surveys. Public Opinion Quarterly, 50, 97–111.Google Scholar
Patterson, M. L. (1982). A sequential functional model of nonverbal exchange. Psychological Review, 89, 231–249.Google Scholar
Patterson, M. L. (2001). Toward a comprehensive model of non-verbal communication. In W. P., Robinson & H., Giles (Eds.), The New Handbook of Language and Social Psychology (pp. 159– 176). Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons.
Pearce, W. & Conklin, F. (1971). Nonverbal vocalic communication and perceptions of a speaker. Speech Monographs, 38, 235–241.
Petty, R. E. & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Communication and Persuasion: Central and Peripheral Routes to Attitude Change. New York: Springer.
Petty, R. E., Wheeler, S. C., & Tormala, Z. L. (2013). Persuasion and attitude change. In H., Tennen, J., Suls, & I. B., Weiner (Eds), Handbook of Psychology, Volume 5: Personality and Social Psychology (2nd edn, pp. 369–389). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Pittam, J. (2001). The relationship between perceived persuasiveness of nasality and source characteristics for Australian and American listeners. The Journal of Social Psychology, 130, 81– 87.Google Scholar
Praxmarer, S. (2011). How a presenter's perceived attractiveness affects persuasion for attractiveness-unrelated products. International Journal of Advertising, 30, 839–865.Google Scholar
Rhine, R. J. & Severance, L. J. (1970). Ego-involvement, discrepancy, source credibility, and attitude change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16, 175–190.Google Scholar
Salovey, P. & Mayer, J. D. (1990). Emotional intelligence. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality, 9, 185–211.Google Scholar
Sharf, D. J. & Lehman, M. E. (1984). Relationship between the speech characteristics and effectiveness of telephone interviewers. Journal of Phonetics, 12, 219–228.Google Scholar
Smith, S. M. & Shaffer, D. R. (1991). Celerity and cajolery: Rapid speech may promote or inhibit persuasion through its impact on message elaboration. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17, 663–669.Google Scholar
Smith, S. M. & Shaffer, D. R. (1995). Speed of speech and persuasion: Evidence for multiple effects. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1051–1060.Google Scholar
Snyder, M., Tanke, E. D., & Berscheid, E. (1977). Social perception and interpersonal behavior: On the self-fulfilling nature of social stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 656–666.Google Scholar
Streeck, J. (2009). Gesturecraft: The manu-facture of meaning. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Van der Vaart, W., Ongena, Y., Hoogendoorn, A., & Dijkstra, W. (2006). Do interviewers' voice characteristics influence cooperation rates in telephone surveys? International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 18, 488–499.Google Scholar
Vann, J. W., Rogers, R. D., & Penrod, J. P. (1970). The cognitive effects of time-compressed advertising. Journal of Advertising, 16, 10–19.Google Scholar
Wagner, P.,Malisz, Z., & Kopp, S. (2014). Gesture and speech in interaction: An overview. Speech Communication, 57, 209–232.Google Scholar
Wheeless, L. R. (1971). Some effects of time-compressed speech on persuasion. Journal of Broadcasting, 15, 415–420.Google Scholar
Wundt, W. (1911). Völkerpsychologie: Eine Untersuchung der Entwicklungsgesetze von Sprache, Mythus, und Sitte [Ethnocultural psychology: An investigation of the developmental laws of language, myth, and customs]. Aalen, Germany: Scientia Verlag.
Crano, W. D. & Prislin, R. (2006). Attitudes and persuasion. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 345–374.Google Scholar
Kelman, H. C. (1961). Processes of opinion change. Public Opinion Quarterly, 25, 57–78.Google Scholar
Petty, R. E. & Wegener, D. T. (1998). Attitude change: Multiple roles for persuasion variables. In D. T., Gilbert, S. T., Fiske, & G., Lindzey (Eds), The Handbook of Social Psychology (vols 1 and 2, 4th edn, pp. 323–390). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Petty, R. E. & Wegener, D. T. (1999). The elaboration likelihood model: Current status and controversies. In S., Chaiken & Y., Trope (Eds), Dual-process Theories in Social Psychology (pp. 37– 72). New York: Guilford Press.
Woodall, W. G. & Burgoon, J. K. (1984). Talking fast and changing attitudes: A critique and clarification. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 8, 126–142.

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×