Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-rvbq7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-11T03:22:03.565Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - Evidence for pervasive synesthetic sound symbolism in ethnozoological nomenclature

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 August 2010

Leanne Hinton
Affiliation:
University of California, Berkeley
Johanna Nichols
Affiliation:
University of California, Berkeley
John J. Ohala
Affiliation:
University of California, Berkeley
Get access

Summary

Nomina debent naturis rerum congruere.

St. Thomas Aquinas

“In establishing a harmony between a thing and its name, we conform to a psychic habit as old as humanity.”

J. Vendryès (1925): 183

Introduction

Sapir's important study of sound symbolism (Sapir 1929) marks the beginning of an interest in the natural association of sound and meaning for twentieth–century linguistics. Building on Sapir's work, a large literature has developed on the subject of what the great linguist called “the expressively symbolic character of sounds quite aside from what the words in which they occur mean in a referential sense” (1929: 225).

Most of the experiments that have been carried out on sound symbolism in the last half century have been designed so as to require subjects to match pairs of words from one language with denotatively or connotatively equivalent words in an unknown language. The pairs of words utilized in these studies have almost invariably been antonyms. In spite of a number of methodological criticisms (see especially Taylor 1963; Taylor and Taylor 1965, 1967), the great majority of these experiments demonstrate unambiguously that certain sensations (such as relative size, movement, shape, texture, color) are regularly associated with the acoustic quality of specific phonetic segments.

The sound-symbolism experiment outlined in the present chapter, drawing on lexical data from Huambisa (one of the major speech communities of the Jivaroan language family in north central Peru) is in the tradition of word-matching studies, but differs from earlier experiments in that it deals not with antonymic pairs but with a large portion of zoological vocabulary.

Type
Chapter
Information
Sound Symbolism , pp. 76 - 93
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 1995

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×