Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-qs9v7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-11T06:30:52.594Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

8 - i: big, a: smal

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 August 2010

Leanne Hinton
Affiliation:
University of California, Berkeley
Johanna Nichols
Affiliation:
University of California, Berkeley
John J. Ohala
Affiliation:
University of California, Berkeley
Get access

Summary

Introduction

From the start, one must admit that the study of sound symbolism has a tarnished reputation in current linguistics. Among several reasons, the poor quality of the evidence is sufficient to explain why many theoreticians show a definite lack of interest in this topic.

Instances of sound symbolism found in languages like English tend to be limited in number, and scattered pell-mell through the language; the iconic patterns themselves are ill-structured, timidly developed, and often difficult to identify and describe. Nevertheless, the more uncertain the data the more daring the theories tend to become, and sober-minded linguists have good reasons to remain skeptical.

Among the more reasonable statements made about sound symbolism, it is often said (Sapir 1929; Newman 1933; Jespersen 1933; Tarte 1974) that if vowel quality is used for size symbolism, [i] will symbolize smallness, and the lower vowels, especially [a], will symbolize largeness, with degrees in between. Casual remarks are often made about this being another set of language “universals,” even though one major effort in this area (Ultan 1978) led only to guarded statements. Furthermore, there are suggestions (Ohala 1984) that this trait belongs not to the cultural but to the natural, innate domain.

This last point appears to be headed in the wrong direction, as I try to show below. Regarding the claim of universality, I will not have much to say, beyond deploring the incorrect use of the term “universal” to mean simply “found in a number of languages.” One learns to take such grandiloquent terminology for what it is. The real problem is in the first claim, the apparently reasonable one.

Type
Chapter
Information
Sound Symbolism , pp. 107 - 114
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 1995

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×